Assessing the Influence of Visual-Taste Congruency on Perceived Sweetness and Product Liking in Immersive VR
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Influence of Visual Cues on Perception of Sweetness
1.2. Application of VR in Sensory and Consumer Research
1.3. Objective Biometric Measurement of Liking
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Experimental Procedures
2.2. Stimulus
2.2.1. Taste Stimuli and Palate Cleanser
2.2.2. Visual Stimuli
2.3. Questionnaires
2.3.1. Pilot Questionnaires
2.3.2. Experiment Questionnaires
2.4. EEG Recording
2.5. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Visual-Taste Association and Liking of Three VR Environments (Pilot Survey)
3.2. Demographic Characteristics and Preferences
3.3. Visual-Taste Congruency on Perceived Taste
3.4. Visual-Taste Congruency on Liking
3.5. Gender Difference on Liking
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Spence, C. Multisensory flavour perception. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, R365–R369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Carvalho, F.M.; Spence, C. Cup colour influences consumers’ expectations and experience on tasting specialty coffee. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 75, 157–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fairhurst, M.T.; Pritchard, D.; Ospina, D.; Deroy, O. Bouba-Kiki in the plate: Combining crossmodal correspondences to change flavour experience. Flavour 2015, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hidaka, S.; Shimoda, K. Investigation of the Effects of Color on Judgments of Sweetness Using a Taste Adaptation Method. Multisens. Res. 2014, 27, 189–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wang, Q.; Carvalho, F.R.; Persoone, D.; Spence, C. Assessing the effect of shape on the evaluation of expected and actual chocolate flavour. Flavour 2017, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wang, Q.J.; Mielby, L.A.; Junge, J.Y.; Bertelsen, A.S.; Kidmose, U.; Spence, C.; Byrne, D.V. The Role of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Sensory Factors in Sweetness Perception of Food and Beverages: A Review. Foods 2019, 8, 211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hershenson, M. Reaction time as a measure of intersensory facilitation. J. Exp. Psychol. 1962, 63, 289–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krishna, A.; Elder, R.S.; Caldara, C. Feminine to smell but masculine to touch? Multisensory congruence and its effect on the aesthetic experience. J. Consum. Psychol. 2010, 20, 410–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Labroo, A.A.; Dhar, R.; Schwarz, N. Of Frog Wines and Frowning Watches: Semantic Priming, Perceptual Fluency, and Brand Evaluation. J. Consum. Res. 2008, 34, 819–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wang, Q.; Spence, C. Assessing the Effect of Musical Congruency on Wine Tasting in a Live Performance Setting. i-Perception 2015, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hagtvedt, H.; Brasel, S.A. Cross-Modal Communication: Sound Frequency Influences Consumer Responses to Color Lightness. J. Mark. Res. 2016, 53, 551–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Velasco, C.; Adams, C.; Petit, O.; Spence, C. On the localization of tastes and tasty products in 2D space. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 71, 438–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Nouchi, R.; Kawashima, R.; Sugiura, M. A Sweet Voice: The Influence of Cross-Modal Correspondences Between Taste and Vocal Pitch on Advertising Effectiveness. Multisens. Res. 2019, 32, 401–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spence, C.; Velasco, C.; Knöferle, K. A large sample study on the influence of the multisensory environment on the wine drinking experience. Flavour 2015, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Velasco, C.; Woods, A.T.; Marks, L.E.; Cheok, A.D.; Spence, C. The semantic basis of taste-shape associations. PeerJ 2016, 4, e1644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Velasco, C.; Woods, A.T.; Deroy, O.; Spence, C. Hedonic mediation of the crossmodal correspondence between taste and shape. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 41, 151–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wan, X.; Woods, A.T.; van Den Bosch, J.J.F.; McKenzie, K.J.; Velasco, C.; Spence, C. Cross-cultural differences in crossmodal correspondences between basic tastes and visual features. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Woods, A.T.; Spence, C.; Butcher, N.; Deroy, O. Fast Lemons and Sour Boulders: Testing Crossmodal Correspondences Using an Internet-Based Testing Methodology. i-Perception 2013, 4, 365–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Velasco, C.; Woods, A.T.; Hyndman, S.; Spence, C. The Taste of Typeface. i-Perception 2015, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Velasco, C.; Salgado-Montejo, A.; Marmolejo-Ramos, F.; Spence, C. Predictive packaging design: Tasting shapes, typefaces, names, and sounds. Food Qual. Prefer. 2014, 34, 88–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Nouchi, R.; Kawashima, R.; Sugiura, M. Round Faces Are Associated with Sweet Foods: The Role of Crossmodal Correspondence in Social Perception. Foods 2019, 8, 103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Clydesdale, F.M.; Gover, R.; Fugardi, C. The effect of color on thirst quenching, sweetness, acceptability and flavor intensity in fruit punch flavored beverages. J. Food Qual. 1992, 15, 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spence, C.; Wan, X.; Woods, A.; Velasco, C.; Deng, J.; Youssef, J.; Deroy, O. On tasty colours and colourful tastes? Assessing, explaining, and utilizing crossmodal correspondences between colours and basic tastes. Flavour 2015, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Oberfeld, D.; Hecht, H.; Allendorf, U.; Wickelmaier, F. Ambient lighting modifies the flavor of wine. J. Sens. Stud. 2009, 24, 797–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spence, C. On the psychological impact of food colour. Flavour 2015, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- O’Mahony, M. Gustatory Responses to Nongustatory Stimuli. Perception 1983, 12, 627–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasik-Krótki, J.; Strojny, J. Scaling of sensory impressions. J. Sens. Stud. 2008, 23, 251–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piqueras Fiszman, B.; Giboreau, A.; Spence, C. Assessing the influence of the color of the plate on the perception of a complex food in a restaurant setting. Flavour 2013, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Alcaide, J.; Roura, E.; Spence, C. Is it the plate or is it the food? Assessing the influence of the color (black or white) and shape of the plate on the perception of the food placed on it. Food Qual. Prefer. 2012, 24, 205–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harrar, V.; Spence, C. The taste of cutlery: How the taste of food is affected by the weight, size, shape, and colour of the cutlery used to eat it. Flavour 2013, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Okajima, K.; Spence, C. Effects of Visual Food Texture on Taste Perception. i-Perception 2011, 2, 966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Rompay, T.J.L.; Finger, F.; Saakes, D.; Fenko, A. “See me, feel me”: Effects of 3D-printed surface patterns on beverage evaluation. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 62, 332–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biggs, L.; Juravle, G.; Spence, C. Haptic exploration of plateware alters the perceived texture and taste of food. Food Qual. Prefer. 2016, 50, 129–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slocombe, B.G.; Carmichael, D.A.; Simner, J. Cross-modal tactile–taste interactions in food evaluations. Neuropsychologia 2016, 88, 58–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tu, Y.; Yang, Z.; Ma, C. Touching tastes: The haptic perception transfer of liquid food packaging materials. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 39, 124–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spence, C.; Carvalho, F.M. The coffee drinking experience: Product extrinsic (atmospheric) influences on taste and choice. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stelick, A.; Penano, A.G.; Riak, A.C.; Dando, R. Dynamic Context Sensory Testing—A Proof of Concept Study Bringing Virtual Reality to the Sensory Booth. J. Food Sci. 2018, 83, 2047–2051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sinesio, F.; Moneta, E.; Porcherot, C.; Abbà, S.; Dreyfuss, L.; Guillamet, K.; Bruyninckx, S.; Laporte, C.; Henneberg, S.; McEwan, J.A. Do immersive techniques help to capture consumer reality? Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 77, 123–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennanen, K.; Närväinen, J.; Vanhatalo, S.; Raisamo, R.; Sozer, N. Effect of virtual eating environment on consumers’ evaluations of healthy and unhealthy snacks. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boutrolle, I.; Delarue, J. Studying Meals in the Home and in the Laboratory. In Meals in Science and Practice; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2009; pp. 128–165. [Google Scholar]
- King, S.C.; Weber, A.J.; Meiselman, H.L.; Lv, N. The effect of meal situation, social interaction, physical environment and choice on food acceptability. Food Qual. Prefer. 2004, 15, 645–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meiselman, H.L.; Johnson, J.L.; Reeve, W.; Crouch, J.E. Demonstrations of the influence of the eating environment on food acceptance. Appetite 2000, 35, 231–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Higgins, E.T.; Scholer, A.A. Engaging the consumer: The science and art of the value creation process. J. Consum. Psychol. 2009, 19, 100–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bangcuyo, R.G.; Smith, K.J.; Zumach, J.L.; Pierce, A.M.; Guttman, G.A.; Simons, C.T. The use of immersive technologies to improve consumer testing: The role of ecological validity, context and engagement in evaluating coffee. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 41, 84–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sinesio, F.; Saba, A.; Peparaio, M.; Saggia Civitelli, E.; Paoletti, F.; Moneta, E. Capturing consumer perception of vegetable freshness in a simulated real-life taste situation. Food Res. Int. 2018, 105, 764–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Corporation, I.D. Worldwide Augmented and Virtual Reality Spending Guide. Available online: https://www.idc.com/getdoc.jsp?containerId=IDC_P34919 (accessed on 13 March 2020).
- Kemp, S.E. Sensory Evaluation: A Practical Handbook; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, C.; Randolph, A.; Burkhalter, J. The Story of Taste: Using EEGs and Self-Reports to Understand Consumer Choice. Kennesaw J. Undergrad. Res. 2012, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yoto, A.; Sato, K.; Yokogoshi, H. Correlations between Central Nervous Activity and Subjective Evaluation of Beverage. Int. J. Affect. Eng. 2013, 12, 45–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manippa, V.; van der Laan, L.N.; Brancucci, A.; Smeets, P.A.M. Health body priming and food choice: An eye tracking study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 72, 116–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gunaratne, T.M.; Fuentes, S.; Gunaratne, N.M.; Torrico, D.D.; Gonzalez Viejo, C.; Dunshea, F.R. Physiological Responses to Basic Tastes for Sensory Evaluation of Chocolate Using Biometric Techniques. Foods 2019, 8, 243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Wijk, R.A.; Kaneko, D.; Dijksterhuis, G.B.; van Zoggel, M.; Schiona, I.; Visalli, M.; Zandstra, E.H. Food perception and emotion measured over time in-lab and in-home. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 75, 170–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Nouchi, R.; Kawashima, R.; Sugiura, M. Anxiety increases visual attention to hedonic foods: A preliminary eye-tracking study on the impact of the interplay between integral and incidental affect on foods. Appetite 2019, 137, 218–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seo, H.-S.; Iannilli, E.; Hummel, C.; Okazaki, Y.; Buschhüter, D.; Gerber, J.; Krammer, G.; Lengerich, B.; Hummel, T. A salty-congruent odor enhances saltiness: Functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Nouchi, R.; Kawashima, R.; Sugiura, M. Tastiness but not healthfulness captures automatic visual attention: Preliminary evidence from an eye-tracking study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 64, 148–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gere, A.; Danner, L.; Dürrschmid, K.; Kókai, Z.; Sipos, L.; Huzsvai, L.; Kovács, S. Structure of presented stimuli influences gazing behavior and choice. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 83, 103915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Teplan, M. Fundamental of EEG Measurement. Meas. Sci. Rev. 2002, 2, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
- Davidson, R.J.; Ekman, P.; Saron, C.D.; Senulis, J.A.; Friesen, W.V. Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology: I. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1990, 58, 330–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Gable, P.A.; Peterson, C.K. The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: A review and update. Biol. Psychol. 2010, 84, 451–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davidson, R.J. Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain Cogn. 1992, 20, 125–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coan, J.A.; Allen, J.J.B. Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biol. Psychol. 2004, 67, 7–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silberman, E.K.; Weingartner, H. Hemispheric lateralization of functions related to emotion. Brain Cogn. 1986, 5, 322–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Di Flumeri, G.; Herrero, M.; Trettel, A.; Cherubino, P.; Maglione, A.; Moneta, E.; Peparaio, M.; Babiloni, F. EEG Frontal Asymmetry Related to Pleasantness of Olfactory Stimuli in Young Subjects; Springer: Belrin, Germany, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Modica, E.; Cartocci, G.; Rossi, D.; Martinez Levy, A.C.; Cherubino, P.; Maglione, A.G.; Di Flumeri, G.; Mancini, M.; Montanari, M.; Perrotta, D.; et al. Neurophysiological Responses to Different Product Experiences. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2018, 2018, 9616301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McBurney, D.H.; Collings, V.B.; Glanz, L.M. Temperature dependence of human taste responses. Physiol. Behav. 1973, 11, 89–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Talavera, K.; Ninomiya, Y.; Winkel, C.; Voets, T.; Nilius, B. Influence of temperature on taste perception. Cell Mol. Life Sci. 2007, 64, 377–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, E.A.; Vickers, Z. The effectiveness of palate cleansing strategies for evaluating the bitterness of caffeine in cream cheese. Food Qual. Prefer. 2004, 15, 311–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delwiche, J.; O’Mahony, M. Flavour discrimination: An extension of thurstonian ‘Paradoxes’ to the tetrad method. Food Qual. Prefer. 1996, 7, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vickers, Z.; Morris, E.E.; Savaria, M. Effectiveness of oral cleansers for evaluating sourness. J. Sens. Stud. 2008, 23, 526–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lucak, C.; Delwiche, J. Efficacy of Various Palate Cleansers with Representative Foods. Chemosens. Percept. 2009, 2, 32–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delorme, A.; Makeig, S. EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 2004, 134, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Allen, J.J.B.; Coan, J.A.; Nazarian, M. Issues and assumptions on the road from raw signals to metrics of frontal EEG asymmetry in emotion. Biol. Psychol. 2004, 67, 183–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ross, C.F.; Bohlscheid, J.; Weller, K. Influence of Visual Masking Technique on the Assessment of 2 Red Wines by Trained and Consumer Assessors. J. Food Sci. 2008, 73, S279–S285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morenga, L.T.; Mallard, S.; Mann, J. Dietary sugars and body weight: Systematic review and meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials and cohort studies. BMJ 2013, 346, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Drewnowski, A. The real contribution of added sugars and fats to obesity. Epidemiol. Rev. 2007, 29, 160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rippe, J.; Angelopoulos, T. Sugars, obesity, and cardiovascular disease: Results from recent randomized control trials. Eur. J. Nutr. 2016, 55 (Suppl. S2), 45–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wang, Q.J.; Mielby, L.A.; Thybo, A.K.; Bertelsen, A.S.; Kidmose, U.; Spence, C.; Byrne, D.V. Sweeter together? Assessing the combined influence of product-related and contextual factors on perceived sweetness of fruit beverages. J. Sens. Stud. 2019, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Nouchi, R.; Kawashima, R.; Sugiura, M. Light colors and comfortable warmth: Crossmodal correspondences between thermal sensations and color lightness influence consumer behavior. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 72, 45–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Park, J.; Velasco, C.; Spence, C.; Sugiura, M. Tasting names: Systematic investigations of taste-speech sounds associations. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pramudya, R.C.; Choudhury, D.; Zou, M.; Seo, H.-S. “Bitter Touch”: Cross-modal associations between hand-feel touch and gustatory cues in the context of coffee consumption experience. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 83, 103914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
df | F | p-Value | η2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Sweetness | 2 | 9.391 | <0.0005 | 0.135 |
Bitterness | 2 | 0.155 | 0.857 | 0.003 |
Sourness | 2 | 0.002 | 0.998 | <0.0005 |
Beverage liking | 2 | 0.563 | 0.571 | 0.009 |
Environment liking | 2 | 6.490 | 0.073 | 0.043 |
FAA | 2 | 0.563 | 0.571 | 0.009 |
Congruency | 2 | 18.530 | <0.0005 | 0.236 |
Vividness | 2 | 2.240 | 0.111 | 0.036 |
Comfort | 2 | 1.041 | 0.356 | 0.017 |
ENVIRONMENT | MEAN(SD) | SEM | COMPARISON | |
---|---|---|---|---|
SWEETNESS | Sweet | 2.42(1.31) | 0.21 | A |
Bitter | 1.27(1.34) | 0.21 | B | |
Neutral | 1.61(1.12) | 0.17 | AB | |
BITTERNESS | Sweet | −3.32(1.29) | 0.2 | A |
Bitter | −3.17(1.12) | 0.17 | A | |
Neutral | −3.24(1.16) | 0.18 | A | |
SOURNESS | Sweet | −2.07(2.07) | 0.32 | A |
Bitter | −2.05(1.90) | 0.29 | A | |
Neutral | −2.05(2.12) | 0.33 | A | |
BEVERAGE LIKING | Sweet | 1.29(1.72) | 0.27 | A |
Bitter | 0.9(1.77) | 0.28 | A | |
Neutral | 1.12(1.53) | 0.24 | A | |
ENVIRONMENT LIKING | Sweet | 1.39 (1.63) | 0.25 | A |
Bitter | 0.29 (1.60) | 0.25 | B | |
Neutral | 0.34 (1.44) | 0.23 | AB | |
FAA | Sweet | −0.01(0.07) | 0.01 | A |
Bitter | 0.01(0.07) | 0.01 | A | |
Neutral | 0.00(0.07) | 0.01 | A | |
CONGRUENCY | Sweet | 1.90(1.90) | 0.31 | A |
Bitter | −0.83(2.40) | 0.37 | B | |
Neutral | −0.10(1.89) | 0.29 | B | |
VIVIDNESS | Sweet | 1.39(1.75) | 0.27 | A |
Bitter | 0.80(1.78) | 0.28 | A | |
Neutral | 0.59(1.82) | 0.28 | A | |
COMFORT | Sweet | 2.39(1.24) | 0.19 | A |
Bitter | 2.07(1.69) | 0.27 | A | |
Neutral | 1.93(1.49) | 0.23 | A |
(A) Sweet environment | ||||
Congruency | Beverage Liking | Environment Liking | FAA | |
Congruency | 1.0 | 0.12 | 0.47 ** | −0.01 |
Beverage liking | - | 1.0 | 0.30 * | −0.08 |
Environment liking | - | - | 1.0 | −0.34 |
FAA | - | - | - | 1.0 |
(B) Bitter environment | ||||
Congruency | Beverage Liking | Environment Liking | FAA | |
Congruency | 1.0 | 0.25 | 0.45 ** | −0.24 |
Beverage liking | - | 1.0 | 0.40 * | −0.21 |
Environment liking | - | - | 1.0 | −0.1 |
FAA | - | - | - | 1.0 |
(C) Neutral environment | ||||
Congruency | Beverage Liking | Environment Liking | FAA | |
Congruency | 1.0 | 0.16 | 0.18 * | −0.14 |
Beverage liking | - | 1.0 | 0.31 * | −0.38 |
Environment liking | - | - | 1.0 | −0.17 |
(A) Sweet environment | |||
Environment liking | Beverage liking | Sweetness | |
Male | 1(−0.5,2) | 2(1,2.5) | 2(2,4) |
Female | 2(0,3) | 1(0,3) | 3(2,3) |
Z | −1.515 | −0.890 | −0.027 |
P | 0.130 | 0.373 | 0.978 |
(B) Bitter environment | |||
Environment liking | Beverage liking | Sweetness | |
Male | 0(−1,0.5) | 1(−0.5,3) | 1(1,2) |
Female | 0(−1,2) | 1(0,2) | 1.5(1,2) |
Z | −0.860 | −0.526 | −0.125 |
P | 0.390 | 0.599 | 0.901 |
(C) Neutral environment | |||
Environment liking | Beverage liking | Sweetness | |
Male | 1(−1,2) | 2(1,2) | 1(−1,2) |
Female | 0(0,1) | 2(1,2.75) | 0(0,1) |
Z | −0.464 | −0.411 | −0.464 |
P | 0.642 | 0.681 | 0.642 |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Chen, Y.; Huang, A.X.; Faber, I.; Makransky, G.; Perez-Cueto, F.J.A. Assessing the Influence of Visual-Taste Congruency on Perceived Sweetness and Product Liking in Immersive VR. Foods 2020, 9, 465. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040465
Chen Y, Huang AX, Faber I, Makransky G, Perez-Cueto FJA. Assessing the Influence of Visual-Taste Congruency on Perceived Sweetness and Product Liking in Immersive VR. Foods. 2020; 9(4):465. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040465
Chicago/Turabian StyleChen, Yang, Arya Xinran Huang, Ilona Faber, Guido Makransky, and Federico J. A. Perez-Cueto. 2020. "Assessing the Influence of Visual-Taste Congruency on Perceived Sweetness and Product Liking in Immersive VR" Foods 9, no. 4: 465. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040465
APA StyleChen, Y., Huang, A. X., Faber, I., Makransky, G., & Perez-Cueto, F. J. A. (2020). Assessing the Influence of Visual-Taste Congruency on Perceived Sweetness and Product Liking in Immersive VR. Foods, 9(4), 465. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040465