Exploring Children’s Self-Reports of Victimisation Experiences and Solitary, Prosocial, and Aggressive Behaviours
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Teacher-Reports and Self-Reports
1.2. Behaviour Conceptualisations
1.2.1. Solitary Behaviour
1.2.2. Prosocial Behaviour
1.2.3. Aggressive Behaviour and Victimisation Experiences
1.3. Research Gaps and Questions
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Ethics
2.5. Statistical Analysis Appraoch
3. Results
3.1. Self-Reports
3.1.1. ANOVA Results from Self-Reports
3.1.2. Significant Main Effects of Behaviour Subtypes on Self-Reports (Based on Table 2)
3.1.3. Significant Main Effects of Gender and Age Group Effects in Self-Reports (Based on Table 2)
3.1.4. Significant Interactions in Self-Reports (Based on Table 2)
3.2. Teacher-Reports
3.2.1. ANOVA Results from Teacher-Reports
3.2.2. Significant Main Effects and Significant Interactions of Age Group and Behaviour Subtype in Teacher-Reports (Based on Table 4)
3.2.3. Significant Main Effects of Gender and Significant Interactions with Age Group and Behaviour Subtype in Teacher-Reports (Based on Table 4)
3.3. Correlations Between Self- and Teacher-Reports of Behaviour
4. Discussion
4.1. Overview of Findings
4.2. Reports of Solitary Behaviour
4.2.1. Self and Teacher-Reports of Solitary Behaviour
4.2.2. The Role of Gender in Reports of Solitary Behaviour
4.3. Reports of Prosocial Behaviour
4.3.1. Self and Teacher-Reports of Prosocial Behaviour
4.3.2. Age Group and Gender Differences in Reports of Prosocial Behaviour
4.4. Reports of Aggressive Behaviour
4.4.1. Self and Teacher-Reports of Aggressive Behaviour
4.4.2. Age Group and Gender Differences in Reports of Aggressive Behaviour
4.5. Reports of Victimisation Experiences
4.5.1. Age Group and Gender Differences in Self- and Teacher-Reports of Victimisation Experiences
4.5.2. Comparing Self and Teacher-Reports of Individual Victimisation Experiences
4.6. Summary of Findings Across Behaviours
4.7. Strengths, Limitations, and Future Directions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Wentzel, K.R.; Jablansky, S.; Scalise, N.R. Peer Social Acceptance and Academic Achievement: A Meta-Analytic Study. J. Educ. Psychol. 2021, 113, 157–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kochenderfer-Ladd, B.; Ladd, G.W.; Thibault, S.A. Children’s Interpersonal Skills and School-Based Relationships. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2022; pp. 366–385. [Google Scholar]
- Valiente, C.; Swanson, J.; DeLay, D.; Fraser, A.M.; Parker, J.H. Emotion-Related Socialization in the Classroom: Considering the Roles of Teachers, Peers, and the Classroom Context. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 56, 578–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Malti, T.; Dys, S.P. From Being Nice to Being Kind: Development of Prosocial Behaviors. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2018, 20, 45–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baker, E.R.; Jensen, C.J. Linking Teacher-Versus Child Self-Report Discrepancies in Aggression to Demographic and Cognitive Profiles. Early Child. Educ. J. 2024, 52, 443–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coplan, R.J.; Ooi, L.L.; Xiao, B.; Rose-Krasnor, L. Assessment and Implications of Social Withdrawal in Early Childhood: A First Look at Social Avoidance. Soc. Dev. 2018, 27, 125–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malti, T.; Averdijk, M.; Zuffianò, A.; Ribeaud, D.; Betts, L.R.; Rotenberg, K.J.; Eisner, M.P. Children’s Trust and the Development of Prosocial Behavior. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2016, 40, 262–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- García-Fernández, C.M.; Romera, E.M.; Monks, C.P.; Ortega-Ruiz, R. Peer Aggression and Victimisation: Social Behaviour Strategies in Early Childhood in Spain. Early Child. Educ. J. 2023, 51, 837–849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monks, C.P.; Smith, P.K.; Kucaba, K. Peer Victimisation in Early Childhood; Observations of Participant Roles and Sex Differences. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fujiki, M.; Brinton, B.; Hart, C.H.; Olsen, J.; Coombs, M. Using Measurement Invariance to Study Social Withdrawal in Children with Developmental Language Disorders. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2019, 50, 253–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doey, L.; Coplan, R.J.; Kingsbury, M. Bashful Boys and Coy Girls: A review of Gender Differences in Childhood Shyness. Sex. Roles 2014, 70, 255–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colonnesi, C.; Nikolić, M.; de Vente, W.; Bögels, S.M. Social Anxiety Symptoms in Young Children: Investigating the Interplay of Theory of Mind and Expressions of Shyness. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2017, 45, 997–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Evans, S.C.; Frazer, A.L.; Blossom, J.B.; Fite, P.J. Forms and Functions of Aggression in Early Childhood. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2019, 48, 790–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yue, X.; Zhang, Q. The Association between Peer Rejection and Aggression Types: A Meta-Analysis. Child Abuse Negl. 2023, 135, 105974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pingault, J.-B.; Côté, S.M.; Lacourse, E.; Galéra, C.; Vitaro, F.; Tremblay, R.E. Childhood Hyperactivity, Physical Aggression and Criminality: A 19-Year Prospective Population-Based Study. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e62594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hawker, D.S.J.; Boulton, M.J. Twenty Years’ Research on Peer Victimization and Psychosocial Maladjustment: A Meta-analytic Review of Cross-sectional Studies. J. Child Psychol. 2000, 41, 441–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samara, M.; Da Silva Nascimento, B.; El-Asam, A.; Hammuda, S.; Khattab, N. How Can Bullying Victimisation Lead to Lower Academic Achievement? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Mediating Role of Cognitive-Motivational Factors. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 2209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caprara, G.V.; Barbaranelli, C.; Pastorelli, C.; Bandura, A.; Zimbardo, P.G. Prosocial Foundations of Children’s Academic Achievement. Psychol. Sci. 2000, 11, 302–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Longobardi, C.; Settanni, M.; Lin, S.; Fabris, M.A. Student–Teacher Relationship Quality and Prosocial Behaviour: The Mediating Role of Academic Achievement and a Positive Attitude Towards School. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2021, 91, 547–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christner, N.; Pletti, C.; Paulus, M. Emotion Understanding and the Moral Self-Concept as Motivators of Prosocial Behavior in Middle Childhood. Cogn. Dev. 2020, 55, 100893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griese, E.R.; Buhs, E.S. Prosocial Behavior as a Protective Factor for Children’s Peer Victimization. J. Youth Adolesc. 2014, 43, 1052–1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dryburgh, N.S.J.; Ponath, E.; Bukowski, W.M.; Dirks, M.A. Associations between Interpersonal Behavior and Friendship Quality in Childhood and Adolescence: A Meta-Analysis. Child Dev. 2022, 93, E332–E347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rix, K.; Monks, C.P.; O’Toole, S. Theory of Mind and Young Children’s Behaviour: Aggressive, Victimised, Prosocial, and Solitary. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 5892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Los Reyes, A.; Augenstein, T.M.; Wang, M.; Thomas, S.A.; Drabick, D.A.G.; Burgers, D.E.; Rabinowitz, J. The Validity of the Multi-Informant Approach to Assessing Child and Adolescent Mental Health. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 141, 858–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Howe, S. What Play Means to Us: Exploring Children’s Perspectives on Play in an English Year 1 Classroom. Eur. Early Child. Educ. Res. J. 2016, 24, 748–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nicholson, P. Play-Based Pedagogy under Threat? A Small-Scale Study of Teachers’ and Pupils’ Perceptions of Pedagogical Discontinuity in the Transition to Primary School. Education 3-13 2019, 47, 450–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morrow, M.T.; Hubbard, J.A.; Bookhout, M.K.; Grassetti, S.N.; Docimo, M.A.; Swift, L.E. Development and Validation of the Forms of Peer Victimization Scale. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2021, 67, 23–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perry, K.J.; Ostrov, J.M.; Murray-Close, D.; Blakely-McClure, S.J.; Kiefer, J.; DeJesus-Rodriguez, A.; Wesolowski, A. Measurement of Aggressive Behavior in Early Childhood: A Critical Analysis Using Five Informants. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2021, 209, 105180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rieffe, C.; Broekhof, E.; Kouwenberg, M.; Faber, J.; Tsutsui, M.M.; Güroğlu, B. Disentangling Proactive and Reactive Aggression in Children Using Self-Report. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 13, 439–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antonopoulou, K.; Chaidemenou, A.; Kouvava, S. Peer Acceptance and Friendships among Primary School Pupils: Associations with Loneliness, Self-Esteem and School Engagement. Educ. Psychol. Pract. 2019, 35, 339–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Denham, S.A.; Bassett, H.H.; Zinsser, K.; Wyatt, T.M. How Preschoolers’ Social-Emotional Learning Predicts Their Early School Success: Developing Theory-Promoting, Competency-Based Assessments. Infant. Child Dev. 2014, 23, 426–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spangler, T.; Gazelle, H. Anxious Solitude, Unsociability, and Peer Exclusion in Middle Childhood: A Multitrait-Multimethod Matrix. Soc. Dev. 2009, 18, 833–856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Malti, T.; Gasser, L.; Buchmann, M. Aggressive and Prosocial Children’s Emotion Attributions and Moral Reasoning. Aggress. Behav. 2009, 35, 90–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Measelle, J.; Ablow, J.C. The Berkeley Puppet Interview for Child Assessment. In Puppet Play Therapy; Routledge: London, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, S.-H.; Smith, P.K.; Monks, C.P. Moral Reasoning about Aggressive Behavior in Relation to Type of Aggression, Age and Gender in South Korean Pupils. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 2288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Önder, A.; Ogelman, H.; Göktaş, İ. Examining the Correlations between the Self-Perception of Preschool Children and Their Levels of Being Liked by Their Peers. Early Child Dev. Care 2021, 191, 2260–2268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rix, K. An Exploration of Four to Seven Year Olds’ Perceptions of Their Own Behaviour with Comparisons to Peers, Class Teachers and Teaching Assistants. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Greenwich, London, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Huitsing, G.; van Duijn, M.A.J.; Snijders, T.A.B.; Alsaker, F.D.; Perren, S.; Veenstra, R. Self, Peer, and Teacher Reports of Victim-Aggressor Networks in Kindergartens. Aggress. Behav. 2019, 45, 275–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huitsing, G.; Monks, C.P. Who Victimizes Whom and Who Defends Whom? A Multivariate Social Network Analysis of Victimization, Aggression, and Defending in Early Childhood. Aggress. Behav. 2018, 44, 394–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kristen-Antonow, S.; Sodian, B.; Perst, H.; Licata, M. A Longitudinal Study of the Emerging Self from 9 Months to the Age of 4 Years. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marcovitch, S.; O’Brien, M.; Calkins, S.D.; Leerkes, E.M.; Weaver, J.M.; Levine, D.W. A Longitudinal Assessment of the Relation between Executive Function and Theory of Mind at 3, 4, and 5 Years. Cogn. Dev. 2015, 33, 40–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frick, A.; Möhring, W.; Newcombe, N.S. Picturing Perspectives: Development of Perspective-Taking Abilities in 4- to 8-Year-Olds. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rakoczy, H. Foundations of Theory of Mind and Its Development in Early Childhood. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 2022, 1, 223–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Osterhaus, C.; Bosacki, S.L. Looking for the Lighthouse: A Systematic Review of Advanced Theory-of-Mind Tests Beyond Preschool. Dev. Rev. 2022, 64, 101021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Uzefovsky, F.; Knafo-Noam, A. Empathy Development throughout the Life Span. In Social Cognition; Routledge: London, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Miller, S.A. Children’s Understanding of Second-Order False Belief: Comparisons of Content and Method of Assessment. Infant. Child Dev. 2013, 22, 649–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Best, J.R.; Miller, P.H.; Jones, L.L. Executive Functions after Age 5: Changes and Correlates. Dev. Rev. DR 2009, 29, 180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coplan, R.J.; Bowker, J.C. All Alone. In The Handbook of Solitude; Coplan, R.J., Bowker, J.C., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Chichester, UK, 2014; pp. 1–13. [Google Scholar]
- Goossens, L. Affinity for Aloneness in Adolescence and Preference for Solitude in Childhood. In The Handbook of Solitude; Coplan, R.J., Bowker, J.C., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Chichester, UK, 2014; pp. 150–166. [Google Scholar]
- Bowker, J.C.; Nelson, L.J.; Markovic, A.; Luster, A.S. Social Withdrawal during Adolescence and Emerging Adulthood. In The Handbook of Solitude; Coplan, R.J., Bowker, J.C., Eds.; Wiley: Chichester, UK, 2014; pp. 167–183. [Google Scholar]
- Gazelle, H.; Druhen Shell, M. Anxious Solitude at School. In The Handbook of Solitude; Coplan, R.J., Bowker, J.C., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Chichester, UK, 2014; pp. 261–282. [Google Scholar]
- Ladd, G.W.; Ettekal, I.; Kochenderfer-Ladd, B. Longitudinal Changes in Victimized Youth’s Social Anxiety and Solitary Behavior. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2019, 47, 1211–1223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, J.; Coplan, R.J.; Chen, X.; Li, D.; Ding, X.; Zhou, Y. Unsociability and Shyness in Chinese Children: Concurrent and Predictive Relations with Indices of Adjustment. Soc. Dev. 2014, 23, 119–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zava, F.; Watanabe, L.K.; Sette, S.; Baumgartner, E.; Laghi, F.; Coplan, R.J. Young Children’s Perceptions and Beliefs about Hypothetical Shy, Unsociable, and Socially Avoidant Peers at School. Soc. Dev. 2020, 29, 89–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veiga, G.; Ketelaar, L.; De Leng, W.; Cachucho, R.; Kok, J.N.; Knobbe, A.; Neto, C.; Rieffe, C. Alone at the Playground. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2017, 14, 44–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jackson, M.; Tisak, M.S. Is Prosocial Behaviour a Good Thing? Developmental Changes in Children’s Evaluations of Helping, Sharing, Cooperating, and Comforting. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2001, 19, 349–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murray, A.L.; Nivette, A.; Obsuth, I.; Hafetz Mirman, J.; Mirman, D.; Ribeaud, D.; Eisner, M. Gender Differences in Cross-Informant Discrepancies in Aggressive and Prosocial Behavior: A Latent Difference Score Analysis. Psychol. Assess. 2022, 34, 409–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baker, E.R.; Tisak, M.S.; Tisak, J. What Can Boys and Girls Do? Preschoolers’ Perspectives Regarding Gender Roles Across Domains of Behavior. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 2016, 19, 23–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bouchard, C.; Sylvestre, A.; Forget-Dubois, N. Why Are Boys Perceived as Less Prosocial than Girls by Their Early Childhood Educators? The Role of Pragmatic Skills in Preschool and Kindergarten Children. Educ. Psychol. Lond. 2020, 40, 1190–1210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paulus, M. The Multidimensional Nature of Early Prosocial Behavior: A Motivational Perspective. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2018, 20, 111–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greener, S.H. Peer Assessment of Children’s Prosocial Behaviour. J. Moral. Educ. 2000, 29, 47–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mildrum, C.S.; Tampke, E.C.; Fite, P.J. Discrepancies between Teacher-and Child-Reports of Proactive and Reactive Aggression: Does Prosocial Behavior Matter? J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2021, 43, 70–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fabes, R.A.; Eisenberg, N. Meta-Analyses of Age and Sex Differences in Children’s and Adolescents’ Prosocial Behavior. In Handbook of Child Psychology; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Chichester, UK, 1998; Volume 3, pp. 1–29. [Google Scholar]
- Monks, C.P.; Smith, P.K.; Swettenham, J. Aggressors, Victims, and Defenders in Preschool: Peer, Self-, and Teacher Reports. Merrill Palmer Q. 2003, 49, 453–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monks, C.P.; Smith, P. Peer, Self and Teacher Nominations of Participant Roles Taken in Victimisation by Five-and Eight-year-olds. J. Aggress. Confl. Peace Res. 2010, 2, 4–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dailey, A.; Frey, A.; Walker, H. Relational Aggression in School Settings: Definition, Development, Strategies, and Implications. Child. Sch. 2015, 37, 79–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cooley, J.L.; Fite, P.J. Peer Victimization and Forms of Aggression During Middle Childhood: The Role of Emotion Regulation. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2016, 44, 535–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poling, D.V.; Smith, S.W.; Taylor, G.G.; Worth, M.R. Direct Verbal Aggression in School Settings: A Review of the Literature. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2019, 46, 127–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rose, C.A.; Richman, D.M.; Fettig, K.; Hayner, A.; Slavin, C.; Preast, J.L. Peer Reactions to Early Childhood Aggression in a Preschool Setting: Defenders, Encouragers, or Neutral Bystander. Dev. Neurorehabilit. 2016, 19, 246–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ostrov, J.M.; Kamper, K.E.; Hart, E.J.; Godleski, S.A.; Blakely-McClure, S.J. A Gender-Balanced Approach to the Study of Peer Victimization and Aggression Subtypes in Early Childhood. Dev. Psychopathol. 2014, 26, 575–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, P.K. Bullying: Recent Developments. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 2004, 9, 98–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.; Moore, K.A. The Use of Likert Scales with Children. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2014, 39, 369–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shaw, T.; Cross, D.; Thomas, L.T.; Zubrick, S.R. Bias in Student Survey Findings from Active Parental Consent Procedures. Br. Educ. Res. J. 2015, 41, 229–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Field, A. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics, 4th ed.; Sage Publications: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Blanca, M.J.; Alarcón, R.; Arnau, J. Non-Normal Data: Is ANOVA Still a Valid Option? Psicothema 2017, 29, 552–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knief, U.; Forstmeier, W. Violating the Normality Assumption May Be the Lesser of Two Evils. Behav. Res. Methods 2021, 53, 2576–2590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bryman, A.; Cramer, D. Quantitative Data Analysis with IBM SPSS 17, 18 & 19: A Guide for Social Scientists; Routledge: London, UK, 2011; ISBN 978-0-203-18099-0. [Google Scholar]
- Howell, D.C. Statistics Methods for Psychology, 5th ed.; Wadswoth Cengage Learning: Duxbury, MA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong, R. When to Use the Bonferroni Correction. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 2014, 34, 502–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monks, C.P.; Robinson, S.; Worlidge, P. The Emergence of Cyberbullying: A Survey of Primary School Pupils’ Perceptions and Experiences. Sch. Psychol. Int. 2012, 33, 477–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nicholson, K.J.; Sherman, M.; Divi, S.N.; Bowles, D.R.; Vaccaro, A.R. The Role of Family-Wise Error Rate in Determining Statistical Significance. Clin. Spine Surg. 2022, 35, 222–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, L.J.; Rubin, K.H.; Fox, N.A. Social Withdrawal, Observed Peer Acceptance, and the Development of Self-Perceptions in Children Ages 4 to 7 Years. Early Child. Res. Q. 2005, 20, 185–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mohamed, A.H.H. Gender as a Moderator of the Association between Teacher–Child Relationship and Social Skills in Preschool. Early Child Dev. Care 2018, 188, 1711–1725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grueneisen, S.; Warneken, F. The Development of Prosocial Behavior-from Sympathy to Strategy. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2022, 43, 323–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swit, C.S.; McMaugh, A.; Warburton, W.A. Preschool Children’s Beliefs about the Acceptability of Relational and Physical Aggression. Int. J. Early Child. 2016, 48, 111–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coplan, R.J.; Bullock, A.; Archbell, K.A.; Bosacki, S. Preschool Teachers’ Attitudes, Beliefs, and Emotional Reactions to Young Children’s Peer Group Behaviors. Early Child. Res. Q. 2015, 30, 117–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rubin, K.H.; Barstead, M.G. Gender Differences in Child and Adolescent Social Withdrawal: A Commentary. Sex. Roles 2014, 70, 274–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monks, C.P.; O’Toole, S. Bullying in Preschool and Infant School. In The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Bullying: A Comprehensive and International Review of Research and Intervention; Peter, K., Smith, J.O.N., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: Chichester, UK, 2021; pp. 2–19. [Google Scholar]
- Arseneault, L.; Kim-Cohen, J.; Taylor, A.; Caspi, A.; Moffitt, T.E. Psychometric Evaluation of 5-and 7-Year-Old Children’s Self-Reports of Conduct Problems. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2005, 33, 537–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kawabata, Y.; Crick, N.R. Relational and Physical Aggression, Peer Victimization, and Adjustment Problems in Asian American and European American Children. Asian Am. J. Psychol. 2013, 4, 211–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hawley, P.H. Social Dominance in Childhood and Its Evolutionary Underpinnings: Why It Matters and What We Can Do. Pediatrics 2015, 135 (Suppl. S2), S31–S38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffin, A.A.; Caldarella, P.; Sabey, C.V. The Effects of a Buddy Bench on Elementary Students Solitary Behavior during Recess. Int. Electron. J. Elem. Educ. 2017, 10, 27–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Younger Group (4–5 Years) | Older Group (6–7 Years) | Totals | |
---|---|---|---|
Boys | 50 | 46 | 96 |
Girls | 52 | 45 | 97 |
Totals | 102 | 91 | 193 |
ANOVA Results | df | F | ηp2 | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Solitary behaviour | ||||
Behaviour subtype | 1 | 3.78 | 0.02 | 0.011 * |
Gender | 3 | 5.01 | 0.03 | 0.026 * |
Age group | 3 | 0.02 | 0.00 | 0.887 |
Behaviour subtype × age group | 1 | 0.21 | 0.00 | 0.887 |
Behaviour subtype × gender | 3 | 0.17 | 0.00 | 0.363 |
Age group × gender | 1 | 0.31 | 0.00 | 0.554 |
Behaviour subtype × age group × gender | 3 | 0.22 | 0.00 | 0.881 |
Prosocial behaviour | ||||
Behaviour subtype | 2.78 | 7.44 | 0.04 | <0.001 *** |
Gender | 1 | 0.13 | 0.00 | 0.724 |
Age group | 1 | 11.60 | 0.06 | <0.001 *** |
Behaviour subtype × age group | 2.78 | 7.73 | 0.04 | <0.001 *** |
Behaviour subtype × gender | 2.78 | 0.35 | 0.00 | 0.777 |
Age group × gender | 1 | 0.13 | 0.00 | 0.724 |
Behaviour subtype × age group × gender | 2.78 | 2.49 | 0.01 | 0.064 |
Aggressive behaviour | ||||
Behaviour subtype | 2.62 | 5.30 | 0.03 | 0.001 *** |
Gender | 1 | 11.31 | 0.06 | <0.001 ** |
Age group | 1 | 15.75 | 0.08 | <0.001 ** |
Behaviour subtype × age group | 2.62 | 1.620 | 0.01 | 0.176 |
Behaviour subtype × gender | 2.2 | 0.684 | 0.00 | 0.543 |
Age group × gender | 1 | 6.310 | 0.03 | 0.013 * |
Behaviour subtype × age group × gender | 2.62 | 0.745 | 0.00 | 0.509 |
Victimisation | ||||
Behaviour subtype | 3 | 0.79 | 0.00 | 0.499 |
Gender | 1 | 2.49 | 0.01 | 0.116 |
Age group | 1 | 4.48 | 0.01 | 0.036 * |
Behaviour type × age group | 3 | 1.68 | 0.01 | 0.170 |
Behaviour type × gender | 3 | 0.27 | 0.00 | 0.838 |
Age group × gender | 1 | 0.25 | 0.00 | 0.620 |
Behaviour subtype × age group × gender | 3 | 3.27 | 0.02 | 0.022 * |
EMM | SE | |
---|---|---|
Solitary behaviour | ||
Unsociable | 1.84 *a | 0.06 |
Contextual solitude | 1.79 | 0.05 |
Shyness | 1.67 | 0.06 |
Active isolation | 1.62 *a | 0.06 |
Prosocial behaviour | ||
Helping | 2.79 ***ab | 0.04 |
Sharing | 2.73 | 0.04 |
Including | 2.59 ***a | 0.05 |
Caring | 2.57 ***b | 0.05 |
Aggressive behaviour | ||
Direct relational | 1.35 *a | 0.05 |
Indirect relational | 1.30 **b | 0.04 |
Verbal | 1.25 *c | 0.04 |
Physical | 1.15 *ac**b | 0.03 |
Victimisation | ||
Direct relational | 1.77 | 0.05 |
Indirect relational | 1.69 | 0.06 |
Verbal | 1.79 | 0.06 |
Physical | 1.78 | 0.06 |
df | F | ηp2 | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Solitary behaviour | ||||
Behaviour subtype | 2.74 | 17.16 | 0.00 | 0.021 * |
Gender | 1 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.937 |
Age group | 1 | 2.56 | 0.01 | 0.111 |
Behaviour subtype × age group | 2.74 | 17.16 | 0.08 | <0.001 *** |
Behaviour subtype × gender | 2.74 | 1.44 | 0.01 | 0.232 |
Age group × gender | 1 | 0.43 | 0.00 | 0.511 |
Behaviour subtype × age group × gender | 2.74 | 1.38 | 0.01 | 0.250 |
Prosocial behaviour | ||||
Behaviour subtype | 2.47 | 37.55 | 0.17 | <0.001 *** |
Gender | 1 | 11.98 | 0.06 | <0.001 *** |
Age group | 1 | 55.31 | 0.23 | <0.001 *** |
Behaviour subtype × age group | 2.47 | 6.09 | 0.31 | <0.001 *** |
Behaviour subtype × gender | 2.47 | 1.00 | 0.01 | 0.394 |
Age group × gender | 1 | 0.40 | 0.00 | 0.528 |
Behaviour subtype × age group × gender | 2.47 | 3.85 | 0.02 | 0.015 * |
Aggressive behaviour | ||||
Behaviour subtype | 2.67 | 24.44 | 0.11 | <0.001 *** |
Gender | 1 | 0.10 | 0.00 | 0.750 |
Age group | 1 | 22.21 | 0.11 | <0.001 *** |
Behaviour subtype × age group | 2.67 | 10.64 | 0.05 | <0.001 *** |
Behaviour subtype × gender | 2.67 | 10.90 | 0.05 | <0.001 *** |
Age group × gender | 1 | 1.96 | 0.01 | 0.160 |
Behaviour subtype × age group × gender | 2.67 | 3.63 | 0.02 | 0.016 * |
Victimisation | ||||
Behaviour subtype | 2.60 | 20.97 | 0.10 | <0.001 *** |
Gender | 1 | 2.44 | 0.01 | 0.120 |
Age group | 1 | 6.49 | 0.89 | 0.012 * |
Behaviour subtype × age group | 2.60 | 3.92 | 0.02 | 0.012 * |
Behaviour subtype × gender | 2.60 | 3.53 | 0.02 | 0.020 * |
Age group × gender | 1 | 0.08 | 0.00 | 0.777 |
Behaviour subtype × age group × gender | 2.60 | 2.68 | 0.01 | 0.055 |
EMM | SE | EMM | SE | EMM | SE | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Whole sample | Younger | Older | ||||
Solitary behaviour | ||||||
Contextual solitude | 1.76 | 0.07 | 1.85 ***f | 0.10 | 1.66 ***f | 0.11 |
Shyness | 1.74 **a | 0.06 | 1.51 ***fg | 0.09 | 1.97 ***fg | 0.11 |
Unsociable | 1.68 | 0.07 | 1.80 ***gh | 0.10 | 1.57 ***gh | 0.11 |
Active isolation | 1.54 **a | 0.67 | 1.23 ***fh | 0.09 | 1.86 ***fh | 0.10 |
Prosocial behaviour | ||||||
Sharing | 3.92 ***ad | 0.07 | 4.10 *fg | 0.10 | 3.72 *fg | 0.10 |
Caring | 3.85 ***be | 0.06 | 4.38 *gh | 0.08 | 3.31 *gh | 0.09 |
Helping | 3.64 ***cde | 0.07 | 4.10 *f | 0.10 | 3.18 *f | 0.10 |
Including | 3.07 ***abc | 0.09 | 3.39 *h | 0.12 | 2.76 *h | 0.13 |
Aggressive behaviour | ||||||
Indirect relational | 2.16 ***ab | 0.08 | 1.73 ***f | 0.11 | 2.38 ***f | 0.08 |
Direct relational | 2.05 ***cd | 0.08 | 1.72 ***g | 0.11 | 2.60 ***g | 0.08 |
Verbal | 1.77 ***ace | 0.07 | 1.43 ***h | 0.10 | 2.08 ***h | 0.07 |
Physical | 1.57 ***bde | 0.07 | 1.54 ***fgh | 0.09 | 1.60 ***fgh | 0.07 |
Victimisation | ||||||
Indirect relational | 1.96 ***abc | 0.08 | 1.69 *fg**h | 0.11 | 2.32 *fg**h | 0.11 |
Direct relational | 0.61 ***ad | 0.07 | 1.56 *f | 0.09 | 1.72 *f | 0.10 |
Verbal | 1.49 ***b | 0.06 | 1.44 **h | 0.08 | 1.54 **h | 0.08 |
Physical | 1.43 ***cd | 0.05 | 1.36 *g | 0.07 | 1.49 *g | 0.08 |
Behaviour | Overall rs (n = 193) | Younger (n = 102) | Older (n = 91) | Boys (n = 96) | Girls (n = 97) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Solitary behaviour | |||||
Shyness | −0.09 | −0.15 | −0.01 | −0.05 | −0.17 |
Unsociable | 0.15 * | 0.25 * | 0.04 | −0.01 | 0.32 *** |
Active isolation | 0.03 | 0.13 | −0.10 | 0.03 | 0.03 |
Contextual solitude | 0.01 | −0.01 | 0.12 | 0.12 | 0.04 |
Prosocial behaviour | |||||
Sharing | 0.10 | 0.11 | 0.03 | 0.02 | 0.19 |
Caring | −0.08 | −0.03 | 0.17 | −0.01 | −0.10 |
Helping | −0.56 | 0.05 | 0.11 | −0.06 | −0.09 |
Including | −0.26 | 0.05 | −0.06 | 0.03 | −0.02 |
Aggressive behaviour | |||||
Direct relational | 0.08 | 0.09 | 0.12 | 0.03 | 0.17 |
Indirect relational | −0.01 | 0.06 | 0.14 | 0.01 | −0.03 |
Verbal | 0.06 | 0.12 | 0.11 | 0.13 | −0.08 |
Physical | 0.20 *** | 0.27 ** | 0.12 | 0.24 ** | −0.00 |
Victimisation | |||||
Direct relational | 0.15 * | 0.15 | 0.16 | 0.09 | 0.22 * |
Indirect relational | 0.16 * | 0.19 | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.25 * |
Verbal | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.10 | −0.15 | 0.20 |
Physical | 0.03 | 0.12 | −0.08 | −0.01 | −0.01 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Rix, K.; Monks, C.P. Exploring Children’s Self-Reports of Victimisation Experiences and Solitary, Prosocial, and Aggressive Behaviours. Psychol. Int. 2024, 6, 868-889. https://doi.org/10.3390/psycholint6040056
Rix K, Monks CP. Exploring Children’s Self-Reports of Victimisation Experiences and Solitary, Prosocial, and Aggressive Behaviours. Psychology International. 2024; 6(4):868-889. https://doi.org/10.3390/psycholint6040056
Chicago/Turabian StyleRix, Katie, and Claire P. Monks. 2024. "Exploring Children’s Self-Reports of Victimisation Experiences and Solitary, Prosocial, and Aggressive Behaviours" Psychology International 6, no. 4: 868-889. https://doi.org/10.3390/psycholint6040056
APA StyleRix, K., & Monks, C. P. (2024). Exploring Children’s Self-Reports of Victimisation Experiences and Solitary, Prosocial, and Aggressive Behaviours. Psychology International, 6(4), 868-889. https://doi.org/10.3390/psycholint6040056