Next Article in Journal
Development of Primer Sets for Loop-Mediated Isothermal Amplification that Enables Rapid and Specific Detection of Streptococcus dysgalactiae, Streptococcus uberis and Streptococcus agalactiae
Next Article in Special Issue
A Simulated Environment Experiment on Annoyance Due to Combined Road Traffic and Industrial Noises
Previous Article in Journal
The Potential Role of Neglected and Underutilised Crop Species as Future Crops under Water Scarce Conditions in Sub-Saharan Africa
Previous Article in Special Issue
City Dweller Responses to Multiple Stressors Intruding into Their Homes: Noise, Light, Odour, and Vibration
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Road-Traffic Noise: Annoyance, Risk Perception, and Noise Sensitivity in the Finnish Adult Population

Department of Health Protection, National Institute for Health and Welfare, Neulaniementie 4, P.O. Box 95, FI-70701 Kuopio, Finland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12(6), 5712-5734; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph120605712
Submission received: 28 March 2015 / Revised: 16 May 2015 / Accepted: 19 May 2015 / Published: 26 May 2015
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Combined Health Effects of Environmental Exposures)

Abstract

:
Exposure to road-traffic noise commonly engenders annoyance, the extent of which is determined by factors not fully understood. Our aim was to estimate the prevalence and determinants of road-traffic noise annoyance and noise sensitivity in the Finnish adult population, while comparing the perceptions of road-traffic noise to exhausts as environmental health problems. Using a questionnaire that yielded responses from 1112 randomly selected adult Finnish respondents, we estimated road-traffic noise- and exhausts-related perceived exposures, health-risk perceptions, and self-reported annoyance on five-point scales, while noise sensitivity estimates were based on four questions. Determinants of noise annoyance and sensitivity were investigated using multivariate binary logistic regression and linear regression models, respectively. High or extreme noise annoyance was reported by 17% of respondents. Noise sensitivity scores approximated a Gaussian distribution. Road-traffic noise and exhausts were, respectively, considered high or extreme population-health risks by 22% and 27% of respondents. Knowledge of health risks from traffic noise, OR: 2.04 (1.09–3.82) and noise sensitivity, OR: 1.07 (1.00–1.14) were positively associated with annoyance. Knowledge of health risks (p < 0.045) and positive environmental attitudes (p < 000) were associated with higher noise sensitivity. Age and sex were associated with annoyance and sensitivity only in bivariate models. A considerable proportion of Finnish adults are highly annoyed by road-traffic noise, and perceive it to be a significant health risk, almost comparable to traffic exhausts. There is no distinct noise-sensitive population subgroup. Knowledge of health risks of road-traffic noise, and attitudinal variables are associated with noise annoyance and sensitivity.

1. Introduction

Road-traffic noise is a widespread environmental nuisance which affects people in their residential dwellings and workplaces [1]. It interferes with human ability to function optimally in daily life [2,3,4] and is rated as the most important source of community noise [5,6]. A WHO report estimates that about 65% of the population in the European Union resides in places where they are regularly exposed to noise levels ranging from 55 to 65 dB. Much of this noise is road traffic related. Exposure of this magnitude has been associated with stress reactions, sleep disturbances and poor health [7,8,9,10]. Of further concern are links which have been shown to exist between sustained noise exposure and an increased mortality risk [4,11,12,13,14]. A common human reaction to noise is annoyance. Noise annoyance can be seen as a negative emotional and attitudinal reaction to noise. The concept of noise annoyance is, however, not unambiguous [15], even though an ISO standard has been created for the estimation of annoyance [16].
Noise annoyance is thought to be an indicator of environmental wellbeing, and is sometimes considered to be a harbinger of subsequent health effects engendered by noise [8]. Although annoyance is a subjective response to noise exposure, it is not explained entirely by ambient acoustics [17]. Rather, it is subject to the influence of personal traits, demographic characteristics, and also physical attributes of the environment which collectively modify the complex psychophysiological sequence leading from noise exposure to annoyance [18]. Interventions designed to reduce noise nuisance should take into account subject-specific determinants of annoyance, as this could increase citizens’ support for such initiatives, and consequently, increase success rate.
Individual attributes observed to influence noise annoyance include: age, sex, marital status, [19,20], having children, level of education [21] and occupational status, for example [22]. Age and sex have been widely investigated as determinants of noise annoyance, but frequently conflicting results are reported [8]. Nonetheless, these variables are relevant as proxies for other factors that modify the individual’s perception of and response to environmental stimuli. Subjective appraisal of environmental quality has been shown to influence human perception of noise and consequent annoyance [23]. Relatively few studies have thus far explored the relationship between attitudinal factors and noise annoyance. For some time has been known that persons who pay closer attention to their immediate environment have a tendency to be more disturbed by noise intrusion, suggesting a proneness to attentional processes [24]. It is not known, however, if this tendency can be generalised to people who care for the environment in general (people having positive environmental attitudes) or those who prefer nature. These are attributes which are subsets of an individual’s psychosocial construct, which evidently mediate subjective response to environmental stimuli [9].
Relating to the physical environment, it must be stressed that although the residential milieu is for most people distinct from the occupational environment, the individual who traverses both settings may ‘import’ stress from one place to another. For example, it has been suggested that noise at work likely modifies the individual’s attitude to noise in general by increasing susceptibility to negative noise response [5]. It may be that one who endures noise exposure at work longs for an escape at the cessation of work, but further noise encounter at home escalates incipient frustration and annoyance.
Noise sensitivity is the singular personal attribute which demonstrably is the most consistent predictor of noise annoyance [5,25]. Noise sensitive persons are known to be more subject to noise annoyance than non-sensitive persons. Furthermore, noise sensitivity has been suggested to predict noise-induced health effects [26]. Despite the association between this trait and noise annoyance, variations in noise sensitivity have no established relationship with the extent of noise exposure [25,27]. Paradoxically, some studies have proposed that prolonged noise exposure may heighten noise sensitivity [25,28].
Noise sensitivity is further described as an intrinsic personal trait having an affective dimension [29]. Meijer et al. [23], and Ryu and Jeon [30] argue that it may be an indicator of general sensitivity to poor environmental quality; Stansfeld has reported a relationship between noise sensitivity, anxiety and depression [31]. Of further note is the observation that noise sensitive persons could exhibit negative affectivity toward self and a broad range of environmental factors in a manner not unlike trait anxiety [31,32]. In contrast, Schreckenberg et al. maintains that noise sensitivity is related to physical rather than mental health states. Schreckenberg et al. [24] and Miedema and Vos [27] separately frame noise sensitivity as a more discriminating sensitivity toward sound quality and residential environmental safety rather than being a broad negative attitude toward the general environment.
Studies exploring the relationship between noise levels and noise annoyance have traditionally used measured- or modelled-sound levels. This approach has limited application in wide-scale national surveys which involve participants recruited from urban and rural areas because noise maps exist mostly for cities, and the cost of instrumental monitoring in multiple non-contiguous rural settings could be daunting. Another consideration is that between the acoustic parameters of emitted sound and what the human ear perceives as noise are several personal and environmental factors which remain only partially understood [33,34].
This study applied a questionnaire assessment of perceived road-traffic noise and air pollution to explore the complex relationship between exposures and human response in adults living in Finland: a northern European country with a low population density estimated at 16 persons per square kilometer [35]. Road-traffic noise and exhausts are investigated together because they are co-stressors which arise from a shared ubiquitous source, and from a public well-being perspective, it is important to know which of these is considered more important by the public. It is also pertinent to know if the discomfort caused by these stressors accumulate on certain individuals, and what factors identify these individuals. The survey evaluated self-reported noise and air pollution exposures, annoyance, perceived health risks from exposures, noise sensitivity, and the knowledge and attitudes pertaining to these exposures, with the following aims: (1) to determine the prevalence of road-traffic related noise annoyance and noise sensitivity; (2) to identify and quantify factors associated with road-traffic noise annoyance, and noise sensitivity; (3) to compare the perceptions of road-traffic noise and air pollution as environmental health problems.

2. Methodology

This study was survey based and involved the self-administration of a questionnaire by Finnish adults.

2.1. Study Sample

A simple random sample of 3000 Finnish-speaking persons living in mainland Finland (excluding Åland), aged between 25 and 74 years, was obtained from the Population Register Centre (2011). Data collection was conducted in the autumn of 2011 by postal dissemination of the study questionnaire. After one round of written reminders, 1112 respondents returned the questionnaire yielding a response rate of 37% (43.9% male and 56.1% female). The data well represents the socio-demographic structure of the target population. Deviations from the target population are: <1% based on region, and 0.2% to 4.7% based on vocational status. Women (deviation from the target population being 5.8%) and older people (deviation from the target population in different age groups of 0.5%–4.6%) were slightly overrepresented.

2.2. Study Variables

We used a semi-structured questionnaire constructed to assess road-traffic noise and road-traffic exhaust related parameters such as perceived exposure, annoyance, perceptions and knowledge of health risks, noise sensitivity, concerns about health risks from home and occupational environments, and environmental attitudes. (Table A1). Additionally, the instrument had entries for respondents’ demographic data. We focused on demographics which, according to the literature, modify noise annoyance, such as: age, sex, marital status, presence of children in the family, residential area, vocational education and other demographic variables. The questionnaire was not supplemented by noise maps or instrumental measurements because of the impracticality of implementing such measurements across the broad geographical spread of respondents—who were sampled across Finland, noting that Finns are known to live far apart outside cities. Noise maps are mostly available for cities, but rural areas, such as we drew some of our respondents from, are poorly covered by these maps.
Perceived exposures were determined with questionnaire items 1–4 with responses rated on a five-point scale. Annoyance due to noise or exhaust exposure was also assessed on a five-point scale using questionnaire items 6–7. Symptoms developed from either noise or exhaust exposures were assessed with questionnaire items 7–8. Subjective estimation of health risks from environmental stressors was assessed with questionnaire items 9–12 (Table A1). Composite measures were calculated for: environmental attitudes (items 16–18), based on questions that were adopted from the 2010 International Social Survey Programme questionnaire [36]; nature orientedness (items 19–25), based on a scale developed by Korpela et al. [37]; and noise sensitivity (items 26–29) which was based on four items excerpted from Weinstein’s 21-item noise sensitivity scale [38]. Composite scores for environmental attitudes were 0 to 12 with lower scores representing positive environmental attitudes and vice versa; sum scores for nature orientedness ranged from 0 to 28 with higher scores representing preference for nature while lower scores denote preference for built-up areas. Noise sensitivity composite scores ranged from 0 to 16 with higher scores denoting higher sensitivity.
We reclassed persons who neither reported residential nor workplace traffic-noise (or exhausts) exposures as not experiencing annoyance or symptoms: regardless of their responses to the pertinent question. This is because these variable were framed to elicit responses to ‘felt’ pressures from exposures. Also, persons reporting no exposures at home, and who were not working during the period of data collection, were reclassed as neither annoyed nor having symptom.
For statistical modelling of covariates of noise annoyance, we collapsed annoyance ratings into dichotomous groups by combining the answers high and extreme (score points 4 and 5) as annoyed and the other categories as not annoyed; this was done to facilitate an increased statistical power of our model to identify covariates of high to extreme noise annoyance. We recoded the predictors of noise annoyance (perceptions of residential traffic noise exposure; perceptions of occupational traffic noise exposure; worry about health risk to self and family arising from the home environment; worry about health risks to self arising from the occupational environment; and knowledge of health risks associated with traffic noise into three categories) viz. score points 1 and 2 = category 1, score points 3 = category 2 and score points 4 and 5 = category 3.
For modelling of noise sensitivity, noise sensitivity was inserted into the model as a continuous variable. we kept the covariates of noise sensitivity in their original scales, specifically, a five-point scale for discrete variables that were elicited in this manner, a dichotomous scale for variables such as sex and hearing impairment, or the actual numeric values for continuous variables (e.g., age). For convenience of statistical analyses, we also recoded respondents’ occupational groupings from the original 9 categories into 5 categories: executive employee/upper clerical worker = category 1, lower clerical worker/employee = category 2, entrepreneur or self-employed/agricultural entrepreneur or farmer = category 3, pensioner = category 4 and student/homemaker/unemployed/others= category 5).

2.3. Statistical Analyses

We used the binary logistic regression analysis to estimate which factors were associated with noise annoyance. Logistic regression models the odds of the probability of an event occurring on a logarithmic scale. This transformation (log odds) allows an approximation of the linear regression model for a binary variable. We used a multivariate linear regression analysis to estimate covariates of noise sensitivity.
In building the multivariate models, we first checked for association between predictor variables and outcome variables using bivariate binary logistic regression and bivariate linear regression for noise annoyance and noise sensitivity, respectively. Pre-tested predictor variables were: sex, age, education, occupational status, having children, residential road-traffic noise exposures, occupational road-traffic noise exposures, symptomatic reactions to road-traffic noise exposure, personal worry about environmental health risks to self and family arising from the residential environment, personal worry about environmental health risks to self arising from the occupational environment, self-rated personal knowledge of health risks associated with road-traffic noise, subjective assessment of personal health, hearing impairment, environmental attitudes, nature orientedness, noise sensitivity (in the noise annoyance prediction model) and noise annoyance (in the noise sensitivity prediction model).
We simultaneously incorporated all predictors which yielded statistically significant results from bivariate models into the multivariate models. At successive iterations, single predictors which did not yield statistically significant association, and which additionally had the largest p-values were eliminated from the model leaving only predictors with p-values < 0.2 in the final output. Post hoc, we assessed for differences in the expressions of covariates of noise annoyance and noise sensitivity between sexes using a cross-tabulation technique for categorical variables and the Wilcoxon Rank Sum test for the attitudinal variables.
We tested for multi-collinearity between noise annoyance, noise sensitivity, environmental attitudes and nature orientedness. Although these variables were correlated (with statistical significance), multicollinearity was minimal in all cases. We discovered a high statistical correlation between noise annoyance and development of symptoms from traffic-noise exposures, worry about health risks from the residential environment, worry about health risks from the occupational environment. We believe that these variables measured the same underlying construct; therefore, we decided to exclude ‘symptom development’ and ‘worry about health risks arising from the occupational environment’ from the model building process.
We used the Wilcoxon Signed-Rank test to determine if persons reporting higher (high to extreme categories) exposure to road-traffic noise or exhausts at home would report similar exposure at work. Similarly, we compared symptom perception from exposure to road-traffic noise and exhaust exposures. We explored the relationship between road-traffic related noise annoyance and road-traffic exhausts using Spearman’s Rank Order correlation coefficient. We used this same method for all correlation analyses. All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics version 22 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) and with a 0.05 level of significance.

3. Results

In sum, 1112 persons returned the questionnaire which implies a response rate of 37%. The mean age of the respondents was 53.7 ± 13.4 years (range: 25–75 years), and 40% of these were elderly persons (aged 60–74 years). The majority of the respondents were females (56%), and 63% of all respondents were working at the time of the survey (Table 1).
Table 1. Demographic data of respondents.
Table 1. Demographic data of respondents.
VariableCategories% (Number)
SexMales43.9 (488)
Females56.1 (624)
Age group25–4426.0 (289)
45–5933.9 (377)
60–7440.1 (446)
Marital statusSingle13.2 (146)
Married or in a registered relationship, cohabiting74.1 (820)
Divorced or separated or widowed12.8 (141)
Occupational statusExecutive employee, upper clerical worker17.8 (197)
Lower clerical worker, employee35.2 (390)
Entrepreneur, self-employed, agricultural entrepreneur, farmer8.9 (99)
Pensioner29.5 (327)
Student/Homemaker/unemployed/others8.7 (96)
Children in the familyNo22.4 (246)
Yes77.6 (853)
Residential areaDowntown city centre13.1 (145)
City suburb48.5 (537)
Population centre in the countryside17.4 (193)
Sparsely populated area21.0 (232)
Vocational educationNo vocational training, professional course, other short vocational training23.1 (253)
Vocational school, school level vocational examination, college level vocational examination46.6 (509)
Higher vocational diploma, University degree30.4 (332)

3.1. Prevalence of Road Traffic-Noise and Exhaust Exposures

In total, 80% of the respondents reported some level of residential exposure to road-traffic noise, and 18% reported high to extreme exposure (score points 4 and 5). Similarly, 13% of respondents reported high to extreme residential exposure to traffic exhausts. The prevalence of high to extreme traffic noise exposure was higher at home than in the workplace (p < 0.001) (Table 2).
On selectively comparing home versus workplace ratings of road-traffic noise only for persons reporting high to extreme exposures, there was a statistically significant difference (p < 0.020). Disparately, equivalent ratings were seen on comparing home versus workplace ratings of road-traffic exhaust exposure (p < 0.504).
Table 2. Self-rated exposures to road traffic related noise and air pollution.
Table 2. Self-rated exposures to road traffic related noise and air pollution.
Exposure Variable1 = No Exposure2345 = Extreme ExposureTotal
% (Number)% (Number)% (Number)% (Number)% (Number)% (Number)
Perceived Residential Exposure (n = 1112)
Traffic noise19.8 (216)37.7 (411)24.7 (270)12.9 (141)4.9 (53)100.0 (1091)
Traffic exhaust17.0 (184)39.1 (423)30.9 (334)10.2 (110)2.8 (30)100.0 (1081)
Perceived Workplace Exposure (n = 698)
Traffic noise30.8 (207)31.0 (208)20.9 (140)12.7 (85)4.6 (31)100.0 (671)
Traffic exhaust29.0 (195)33.9 (228)22.7 (153)9.8 (66)4.6 (31)100.0 (673)

3.2. Subjective Reaction to Road-Traffic Noise and Exhausts Exposures

Many respondents (65%) reported some (any) annoyance from traffic noise, about 17% reported high to extreme annoyance. About 34% of downtown/city dwellers and 21% of city suburb dwellers reported high to extreme noise annoyance. Road-traffic noise annoyance correlated well with traffic-exhaust annoyance (rho = 0.6). Selectively comparing symptom ratings only for respondents reporting high to extreme symptomatic reaction to either road-traffic noise or road-traffic exhausts yielded no difference (p < 0.671).
A considerable proportion of people, 22%, perceived that noise is a population-health risk of high to extreme grade. However, a higher percentage (27%) of people perceived road-traffic exhausts as being of high or extreme risk to the health of the general population. Overall, more respondents perceived that they were at some (personal) health risk from road-traffic exhausts than from noise (p < 0.001); but, the proportion of respondents reporting high to extreme risk from both road-traffic noise and exhaust were comparable. More subjects perceived that general population-health risk from traffic noise was higher when compared to their individual-health risk (p < 0.001). This finding was similar for perceived risk from traffic exhausts (Table 3). The median noise sensitivity (composite) score was 9 (interquartile range: 6–11), and the distribution approximated a Gaussian distribution. (Figure 1). Self-ratings of noise sensitivity in response to the single question (questionnaire item number 26, Appendix) showed that 35% and 11% of study respondents considered themselves to be highly or extremely noise sensitive, respectively.
Figure 1. Distribution of noise sensitivity scores in the study population (the solid line indicates the Gaussian distribution).
Figure 1. Distribution of noise sensitivity scores in the study population (the solid line indicates the Gaussian distribution).
Ijerph 12 05712 g001
Table 3. Self-rated feelings relating to road-traffic noise and exhaust exposures.
Table 3. Self-rated feelings relating to road-traffic noise and exhaust exposures.
Exposure Variable1 = None2345 = ExtremeTotal
% (Number)% (Number)% (Number)% (Number)% (Number)% (Number)
Personal Annoyance a
Traffic noise34.9 (388)29.0 (323)18.9 (210)12.3 (137)4.9 (54)100.0 (1112)
Traffic exhaust39.9 (444)36.1 (401)14.7 (163)6.9 (77)2.4 (27)100.0 (1112)
Develops Symptoms from Exposure
Traffic noise63.5 (706)19.0 (211)10.0 (111)5.4 (60)2.2 (24)100.0 (1112)
Traffic exhaust65.1 (724)18.3 (204)9.2 (102)4.9 (55)2.4 (27)100.0 (912)
Perception of Personal Health Risk
Traffic noise44.5 (430)30.0 (290)15.4 (149)7.6 (73)2.6 (25)100.0 (967)
Traffic exhaust29.0 (283)36.3 (354)23.5 (229)7.0 (68)4.3 (42)100.0 (976)
Perception of Population Health Risk
Traffic noise7.7 (79)32.7 (337)37.3 (384)17.3 (178)5.0 (52)100.0 (1030)
Traffic exhaust3.6 (38)28.9 (303)40.9 (429)19.5 (205)7.1 (75)100.0 (1050)
Noise Sensitivity b
Noise sensitivity13.5 (149)25.7 (283)25.5 (280)24.7 (272)10.5 (116)100.0 (1100)
a Traffic noise annoyance was calculated only for respondents who felt they were exposed either at home or at work or both; b Noise sensitivity was calculated for all respondents who provided self-estimate by answering the single question, “Are you noise sensitive?”.

3.3. Determinants of Noise Annoyance

Covariates which were statistically significantly associated with noise annoyance in the bivariate model were sex (women showed more annoyance to noise, p < 0.047), perception of residential exposure to traffic noise, perception of workplace exposure to traffic noise, worry about health risks to self and family arising from the home environment, worry about health risks to self arising from the occupational environment, knowledge of health risks associated with traffic noise, hearing impairment, environmental attitudes, nature orientedness and noise sensitivity.
The multivariate model showed that increased annoyance was positively associated with: self-rated home exposures, personal worry about risks from home environment, and knowledge of health risks due to traffic noise (Table 4). Noise sensitivity and nature orientedness showed borderline significant associations with road-traffic noise annoyance. Nagelkerke R-squared for this model was 0.54, indicating a reasonable explanatory power for the outcome. The distribution of these variables between the two levels of annoyance is shown in Table A2.
Table 4. Determinants of high to extreme noise annoyance.
Table 4. Determinants of high to extreme noise annoyance.
ParameterResponse CategoryOdds Ratio95% C.I. of Odds Ratios
LowerUpper
Perceived exposure to traffic noise at homeNo exposureref.
Some exposure3.782.027.06
Extreme exposure56.9430.81105.23
Worry about health risk from residential environmentNot worriedref.
Worried2.151.273.64
Extremely worried2.891.565.37
Knowledge about health risk from traffic noiseno knowledgeref.
some knowledge1.240.692.22
much knowledge2.041.093.82
Noise sensitivity a-1.071.001.14
Nature orientedness b-1.051.001.10
a Noise sensitivity was rated on a scale of 0–16; b Rated on a scale of 0–28, with higher scores indicating love for nature while lower scores signify love for built-up areas.
We carried out a sensitivity analysis (not shown) for the binary logistic model using score points three and above to determine covariates of moderate to extreme noise annoyance. Covariates similar to those found in the first model (excluding nature orientedness) remained after step-wise elimination; in addition, environmental attitudes was also retained in this model (Nagelkerke R2 = 0.53). Outcome estimates in this model were: (1) perceived residential exposures to traffic noise—“some exposure” OR 9.89 (6.66–14.67), “extreme exposure”, OR 52.72 (29.09–95.56); (2) worry about risk from residential environment-“worried” OR 1.18 (0.77–1.79), “extremely worried” OR 1.83 (1.06–3.15); (3) knowledge about risk from traffic noise exposure—“some knowledge” OR 1.30 (0.83–2.04), “much knowledge” OR 1.85 (1.12–3.07); noise sensitivity OR 1.09 (1.03–1.15); environmental attitudes OR 0.96 (0.89–1.03).

3.4. Determinants of Noise Sensitivity

Factors which showed statistically significant associations with noise sensitivity in the bivariate model were sex (women were more sensitive to noise, p < 0.008), occupational status (the occupational category consisting of entrepreneurs, self-employed and farmers was more sensitive), perceptions of residential exposure to traffic noise, traffic-noise annoyance, symptomatic reaction following traffic-noise exposure, worry about health risk to self and family arising from the home environment, worry about health risk to self and from the occupational environment, knowledge of health risks associated with traffic noise (higher knowledge was associated with higher sensitivity), environmental attitudes, nature orientedness.
The multivariate linear regression model showed evidence of statistical associations between noise sensitivity and the following covariates: vocational status, knowledge of health risk from residential noise exposure, positive environmental attitudes, and nature orientedness (respondents with positive environmental attitudes or preference for nature were more noise sensitive). The model accounted for approximately 17% of the variability in noise sensitivity and the overall predictive capacity was statistically significant (F = 12,628, p < 0.001) (Table 5).
Table 5. Determinants of noise sensitivity.
Table 5. Determinants of noise sensitivity.
ParameterResponse Categoryβ95% C.I. of Coefficients ap-Value b
Lower BoundUpper Bound
Intercept9.858.1911.500.000
Occupational statusStudent, homemaker, unemployed, others workerRef.0.026
Pensioner−0.19−1.020.65
Entrepreneur, self-employed, agricultural entrepreneur, farmer−1.26−2.27−0.25
Lower clerical worker, employee−0.79−1.580.01
Executive employee, upper clerical−0.75−1.610.11
Traffic noise annoyance5 = extremely annoyedRef.0.000
40.77−0.351.89
30.42−0.651.48
2−0.12−1.160.91
1 = not annoyed−1.53−2.59−0.47
Knowledge of health risks from traffic noise5 = extremely knowledgeableRef.0.045
4−1.11−2.20−0.03
3−1.26−2.31−0.22
2−1.65−2.74−0.57
1 = not knowledgeable−1.53−3.170.11
Environmental attitudes c-−0.23−0.31−0.150.000
Nature orientedness d-0.120.080.160.000
a Linear regression coefficient; b P-values are based on between-subject difference for each variable; c Rated on a scale of 0 to 12; persons with lower scores had positive environmental attitudes and the converse holds for negative environmental attitudes; d Rated on a scale of 0–28, with higher scores indicating preference for nature while lower scores signify love for built-up areas.
The post hoc exploration of covariates of noise annoyance and noise sensitivity by sex showed statistically significant differences between men and women for all covariates except for nature orientedness. Women tended to be more knowledgeable about risk from traffic noise (χ2 = 8.18, p < 0.17), more worried personal and family risk arising from the residential environment (χ2 = 31.2, p < 0.001), and had more positive environmental attitudes (p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

We conducted a survey to assess perceived road-traffic noise and exhaust exposure, risk perception, and measures of subjective response to exposures. Our results show that more (17%) respondents considered themselves exposed to high or extreme levels of residential road-traffic related noise than high or extreme levels of road-traffic exhaust (13%). Perception of high to extreme health risks was similar for both road-traffic noise and road-traffic exhaust. The distribution of noise sensitivity within the study population approximated a Gaussian distribution. High to extreme noise annoyance was associated with high perceived exposure, increased noise sensitivity, increased worry about environmental safety, greater knowledge of traffic noise health risks and nature orientedness. High to extreme road-traffic noise sensitivity was associated with increased traffic noise annoyance, higher knowledge of health risks from traffic-noise, positive environmental attitudes and nature orientedness. Much of the variability in noise sensitivity remained unexplained.

4.1. Perceived Traffic Noise and Exhaust Exposures and Subjective Response

Noise perception draws upon physical characteristics of noise, the immediate disposition of an exposed subject and his attitudes which are determined by underlying personal traits [3]. It is a closer representation of the perceptual quality of noise to a hearer than loudness, and a better predictor of noise-related health outcomes [3]. Comparisons between objective sound measures and noise perception show some agreement, but this has been in so complex a manner as cannot be easily predicted or modelled [7]. Subjective assessments of noise exposure have been previously used mostly in occupational surveys. Neitzel used questionnaire items to assess the perceived intensity of noise in three different work-noise (continuous, intermittent and highly variable) environments. Responses to questions framed to evaluate the absolute loudness of noise and relative loudness were then analysed for relationship with time-logged Leq and Lavg readings; their results showed that the survey response categories correlated with measured noise levels comparably to or slightly better than an exposure assignment based on job title. The survey instrument could reasonably contrast between noise levels and between degrees of noise variability [39]. An earlier study among patients with acoustic neuroma and their controls showed that in both groups, there was good agreement between self-reported occupational noise exposure and a job-exposure matrix, with study participants showing a clear distinction of occupations subject to noise levels of 80 dB(A) and above—a limit at which regulations require use of personal protective devices [40]. Dzhambov and Dimitrova, in a community survey, compared a subjective noise exposure, measured on a visual analogue scale, with residential Lden and found a high correlation between both metrics [41]. The investigators developed the instrument through rigorous qualitative and quantitative phases, and it is noteworthy that the instrument could predict annoyance, and like measured noise it had no association with noise sensitivity. Although several of these studies have used modified questionnaire items from prior studies, there is presently no broad consensus on the content of survey instruments.
Heinonen-Gujezev et al. [42] compared self-reported noise exposure to a map-predicted (modelled) noise exposure. Dichotomized scales for both measures agreed with each other, but the agreement was more for aircraft noise than either railway or road-traffic noise. Aydin observed different patterns of correlation between modelled noise emanating from Frankfurt airport and perceived noise by residents living east or west of the airport with distinct exposure levels [7]. We found that a considerable proportion of Finnish adults reported high to extreme levels of perceived residential road-traffic noise. This underscores the fact that road traffic noise is not a problem that is limited to only densely populated countries with commensurate dense vehicular traffic. More respondents felt they received higher exposure to both road-traffic noise and exhaust emissions at home than at work. Persons reporting high to extreme exposures to road-traffic noise at home were distinct from those reporting same exposure levels at work. On the other hand, persons reporting high to extreme residential traffic exhaust exposures also reported similar occupational road-traffic exhaust exposure. Subjective rating of high to extreme exposure was higher for road-traffic noise than road-traffic exhaust exposures. A similar perceptive pattern was reported in a survey which was conducted in Edinburgh [43].
In Finnish adults, we found a population prevalence of 17% for high to extreme levels of noise annoyance. Other studies which used similar five-point scales have reported higher prevalence for these upper-end responses. For example, a prevalence of 31.4% for “very annoyed” and “extremely annoyed” was obtained in a survey that was conducted in Belgrade [44], and an Egyptian study yielded 65.3% prevalence for similar categories of noise annoyance [45]. The higher prevalence may be explained by differences in traffic composition, population size and transportation policy. Belgrade is reported to have a high number heavy duty vehicles moving both during the day and at night [44], Cairo has a large population and heavier traffic load [45]. Finland on the other hand has an infrastructure that supports bicycle use as a common means of transportation all the year round. A pertinent difference between this study and others, which may account the lower prevalence of annoyance, is the fact that respondents from the countryside were included—other studies were city based.
An experience of high or extreme annoyance from noise exposure was higher than due to traffic exhausts. However, approximately equal numbers of persons reported symptoms from either road-traffic noise or road-traffic exhausts. Additionally, people who reported high to extreme symptoms from road-traffic noise were more likely to report high to extreme symptoms from road-traffic exhausts. Although fewer persons reported exposure to exhaust emissions, the proportion of persons who experienced symptoms from this pollutant was comparable to noise, suggesting a higher potency of traffic exhaust to induce symptoms.
We also observed that respondents estimated that they perceived higher personal and population health risk due to traffic exhausts exposure than from traffic noise. This may explain their higher perception of bodily symptom due to exhaust exposure. A similar pattern of health risk perception was documented in another study, which appraised respondents’ ratings of overall community levels of pollutants, in which air pollution was rated to be the most offensive environmental stressor followed by community noise [44]. In our study, perceptions of risks to the general population, from these stressors, were always higher than perceptions of personal risk. Although population perception of health risk from road-traffic noise receives very scant mention in literature, it may provide an insight into how annoyance and symptoms due to noise can vary in a population.

4.2. Factors Associated with Noise Annoyance

We found that self-rated ‘high to extreme’ noise annoyance was influenced by perceived exposures. An enquiry into the relationship between residential (measured) noise levels and subjective noise perception, as well as their influence on noise annoyance has been made by Heinonen-Gujezev et al. [42]. The authors used a single item self-rated question to assess noise exposure, and a 10-item question, mostly on disturbance parameters, to estimate noise annoyance. A positive association was seen between increasing noise exposure and annoyance. Mostly, studies investigating associations between noise exposure and noise annoyance have used measured or modelled noise exposure levels [2,5,8,46,47,48].
In this study, noise sensitivity was a borderline significant positive predictor of high to extreme annoyance, but a strong predictor of moderate to extreme levels of annoyance (in the binary logistic sensitivity model). It has been reported that subjective noise exposure is predicted by noise sensitivity [42]. In our bivariate model, noise sensitivity showed strong association with reported noise levels. Multicollinearity between these associated predictors may have attenuated the modelled influence of noise sensitivity on annoyance. It can be cautiously argued that should noise sensitivity lower the threshold of noise annoyance, other circumstantial and personal factors may then contribute more strongly to increased noise annoyance.
Worry about environmental health risks was associated with noise annoyance. Concerns about environmental safety is reportedly more common in noise sensitive people than the general public. This relationship may cause the “worry about environmental health risk” variable to behave in a similar way as noise sensitivity in a predictive model for noise annoyance. Miedema made conclusions supporting this view from a pooled analysis of 28 data sets [49].
Knowledge of health risks associated with traffic noise exposure was strongly associated with noise annoyance in our model. Intimate knowledge of risks associated with a stressor can breed concerns which would accentuate a subject’s reaction to that stressor. Knowledge could influence personal disposition and attitudes, and consequently may condition subjective responses to a perceived risk factor. Negative attitudes to a noise source could induce negative response to noise, but the relationship could go either way [3,27,47]. Awareness of residential noise risks can result because noise sensitive persons delve further into knowledge of risks than non-sensitive persons [47]. Although this study did not specifically evaluate attitude as a formal variable, a variable of similar construct could be worry about environmental risk.
Individual preference for nature has been observed to have a positive gradient with noise annoyance [24,25,50]. Greenery and scenic landscapes are thought to exert a restorative influence on the mind, thus diluting the anxieties of daily life [51]. Putatively, nature-oriented individuals would prefer that the equilibrium of nature remains pristine. Heinonen-Guzejev had adduced the noise reaction of the Finnish population to the swift transition from a long-held sparsely-populated agrarian territory to a highly industrialized economy with commensurate urbanization trends [52].

4.3. Factors Associated with Noise Sensitivity

We found that the distribution of noise sensitivity (composite scores) in the study population followed a Gaussian-type distribution, indicating that, in the general population, there are people at all estimated levels of noise sensitivity and that no clear cut-off mark exists delineating non-sensitive and sensitive subpopulations. In a study where noise sensitivity was measures on a five-point scale, Shepherd et al. found that their respondents were spread across all levels of noise sensitivity, with no one indicating a zero-level noise sensitivity [9]. The prevalence of high to extreme noise sensitivity, as assessed using a single question, was consistent with a preceding study based on a 1988 survey conducted among 1495 Finnish adults in which noise sensitivity was measured on a five-point scale using a single item question [42,52]. Overall, high to extreme noise sensitivity was quite prevalent. However, we cannot discount the possibility of study participation being influenced by a self-selection of environmentally sensitive individuals into the survey [53,54,55].
Noise sensitivity regressed strongly on noise annoyance. It is not implausible that a possible “positive feed-back loop” exists in which noise sensitivity lowers the threshold for noise annoyance, and noise annoyance—so induced—aggravates noise sensitivity. This view was initially proposed by Job [56] and later supported by Paunovic [5] who suggested that extended noise annoyance could worsen moods and trigger depressive episodes thereby rendering exposed subjects further noise sensitive.

4.4. Role of Age and Sex

In the bivariate models, being female showed some evidence of an association with noise annoyance and strong evidence of association with noise sensitivity. Age was not associated with these outcomes in any model. This is in contrast to a prior publication that showed associations between age-group and noise annoyance on one hand, and noise sensitivity on the other hand [48]. Sex was neither associated with noise sensitivity nor annoyance in any of the multivariate models. An early report of the elusiveness of such associations was made in 1972 [48]. Similar to a reports by van Gerven, our sensitivity model for noise annoyance yielded a statistically significantly association between age (45–59 years) and moderate to extreme noise annoyance in bivariate regression [10]. This association disappeared in the model where the response variable was high to extreme annoyance.
Although sex regressed well on both outcomes in bivariate models, presumably, the effect of sex may have been dampened by simultaneous inclusion with other covariates in the model. The effect of sex may have been confounded by variables which showed a sex differential and were associated with noise annoyance and sensitivity in the models; for example, knowledgeable about health risks, worry about environmental risks and positive environmental attitude. Our finding is corroborated by Li [50] who explored the role of sex within a multivariate model on noise sensitivity and by Bluhm [54] who compared the standardized mean difference in noise annoyance between men and women. Nevertheless, it has been reported that being female predicted higher noise annoyance and noise sensitivity in multivariate models [25].

4.5. Strengths and Weaknesses

This study relied on data from a broad-based national survey in which respondents were drawn from urban and rural residential areas, and provides a representative sample for the Finnish adult population. We assessed road-traffic noise exposure using subjective ratings of exposure which, in our opinion, is a closer representation of respondents’ actual experience than instrumental measurement or modelled noise levels. Like many previous studies, we focused on residential exposure because people spend more time at home, which makes exposure contribution from the home environment more substantial than contributions from other settings for most people. Based on results from previous studies, we believe that noise exposure-scale ratings agree to a reasonable extent with instrumentally measured levels of noise [25,39,41], with the added advantage that self-rated noise exposure incorporates individual factors which could influence perception of noise but would be lost during instrumental determination [28,42,47,57]. This paper re-emphasizes the importance of attitudinal variables, subjective knowledge and perception of health risks associated with road-traffic noise and exhausts, and how these could influence annoyance and sensitivity. These factors are less commonly explored among predictors in noise-effects research, and could explain some of the variation in noise annoyance and sensitivity.
An obvious weakness in this study is that the assessment of perceived noise exposure did not facilitate partitioning into daytime, evening or nighttime exposure. It therefore impairs any effort to isolate annoyance pertaining to specific segments of the 24-h day. Also, the modifying influence of building insulation is not taken into account. Our objective was to account for the best representation of the overall experience of respondents in their residential settings and to gauge factors that could explain this experience. We do not therefore see these omissions as a setback. Another weakness is that the questionnaire did not scrutinize personal circumstances and mental states that could impact on respondents’ assessment of exposure and annoyance. Our belief is that population-wide differences in such modifiers will not be distributed in such a way as would significantly alter our result when considered.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, four in five Finnish adults felt exposed to road-traffic noise in their residential environment, proportionally more than in the occupational environment. The population health risk was also considered substantial. However, road-traffic noise was perceived to be of lower health risk than exhausts. Annoyance from noise was also widespread within the study population. These findings emphasise that road-traffic noise is a problem even in less populated, quieter societies.
High levels of noise annoyance were explained in part by the high perception of exposure, and noise sensitivity. No distinct noise sensitive subgroup could be identified. Only a low percentage of the variation in individual noise sensitivity could be explained; high sensitivity was associated with high noise annoyance. Knowledge of the health risks of noise and attitudinal variables, such as environmental attitudes and nature orientedness, seem to explain some of the variation in both noise annoyance and sensitivity. It would be informative to noise epidemiologists to further explore these variables in longitudinal studies, to elucidate their ramifications. That nature orientedness can predispose to noise annoyance and sensitivity should be of interest to town planners. There is a growing body of research which shows that green areas can ameliorate the impact of environmental stressors by providing a restorative influence on the mind.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the contribution of Jos Verbeek, who read through the manuscript and offered useful suggestions toward a more focused presentation.

Author Contributions

Timo Lanki and Sari Ung-Lanki developed the study concepts and instrument. Enembe Okokon did the data analysis and drafted the manuscript with helpful suggestions from Anu Turunen. Pekka Tiitanen gave valuable consultation during the data analysis. All authors read through, made contributions to improve the manuscript, and approved the final version.

Appendix

Table A1. Questionnaire items on key variables.
Table A1. Questionnaire items on key variables.
No.Rating of Personal Noise ExposureResponse Scale
No ExposureExtreme ExposureUnable to Answer
1Please rate on a scale of 1–5 the typical extent of your exposure to road-traffic noise in your residential environment123458
2Please rate on a scale of 1–5 the typical extent of your exposure to road-traffic noise in your occupational environment123458
Rating of Personal Traffic Exhausts ExposureNo ExposureExtreme ExposureUnable to Answer
3Please rate on a scale of 1–5 the typical extent of your exposure to road-traffic exhausts in your residential environment123458
4Please rate on a scale of 1–5 the typical extent of your exposure to road-traffic exhausts in your occupational environment123458
Noise AnnoyanceNo DisturbanceExtreme DisturbanceUnable to Answer
5Are you disturbed in some way by road-traffic noise?123458
6Are you disturbed in some way by road traffic exhaust?123458
Developing SymptomsNot at AllVery MuchUnable to Answer
7Does road-traffic noise usually cause you to experience some kind of symptoms, for example, feeling ill, headaches, respiratory symptoms, eye irritation?123458
8Does road-traffic exhaust usually cause you to experience some kind of symptoms, for example feeling ill, headaches, respiratory symptoms, eye irritation?123458
No.Perception of Health RiskResponse Scale
No RiskExtreme RiskUnable to Answer
9By your rating, how big a health risk is road-traffic noise to you?123458
10By your rating, how big a health risk is road-traffic exhaust to you?123458
11By your rating, how big a health risk is road-traffic noise to the general Finnish population?123458
12By your rating, how big a health risk is road-traffic exhaust to the general Finnish population?123458
Personal worryNot WorriedExtremely WorriedUnable to Answer
13In general, how worried are you about the health risks posed to you and your family by your residential environment?123458
Assessment of Personal KnowledgeNothingLittleSomeA LotVery Much
14In your opinion, how much do you know about health risks associated with road-traffic noise?12345
15In your opinion, how much do you know about health risks associated with road-traffic exhausts?12345
Environmental AttitudesStrongly AgreeAgreeNeutralDisagreeStrongly DisagreeUnable to Answer
16People needlessly worry that developmental activities cause damage to the environment123458
17There are more important things in life than environmental protection123458
18Many arguments regarding environmental threats are exaggerated123458
No.Nature OrientednessResponse Scale
Strongly AgreeAgreeNeutralDisagreeStrongly DisagreeUnable to Answer
19I enjoy being in the city123458
20I often feel distressed in a crowded and busy city123458
21I feel that city centres are just the place for me123458
22Green areas within cities do not fulfil my need to be in the nature123458
23Sometimes I feel an urge to be in natural settings123458
24I greatly appreciate areas with cafeterias, restaurants, museums, and theatres123458
25I feel more comfortable in green areas and parks than in built environments123458
26Noise SensitivityStrongly AgreeAgreeNeutralDisagreeStrongly DisagreeUnable to Answer
I get irritated when my neighbours cause noise123458
27I am good at concentrating whatever happens around me
28It is difficult for me relax in a noisy place123458
29I am sensitive to noise123458
Table A2. The distribution of some explored covariates between the strata of noise annoyance (high to extreme and others).
Table A2. The distribution of some explored covariates between the strata of noise annoyance (high to extreme and others).
VariablesNo Annoyance to Some AnnoyanceHigh to Extreme Annoyance
NumberMean (Standard Deviation)RangeNumberMean (Standard Deviation)Range
Perceived exposure to traffic noise at home8321.39 (0.60)1–31882.61 (0.65)1–3
Worried about personal and family risks from home environment8101.50 (0.70)1–31862.11 (0.77)1–3
Noise sensitivity8118.42 (3.40)1–1618710.05 (3.47)2–16
Environmental attitude7514.70 (2.76)0–121703.79 (2.89)0–11
City-nature orientedness80917.43 (5.35)0–2818118.33 (5.15)5–28
Knowledge of health risk from Traffic noise exposure8241.89 (0.73)1–31852.26 (0.73)1–3
VariablesNo Annoyance to Some AnnoyanceHigh to Extreme Annoyance
Number%Number%
Knowledge of health risk from Traffic noise exposureno knowledge271333117
some knowledge371457440
much knowledge182228043

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. European Environment Agency (EEA). Europe’s Environment: The Third Assessment; 2003 Contract No.: EAR10; European Environment Agency: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  2. Yoshida, T.; Osada, Y.; Kawaguchi, T.; Hoshiyama, Y.; Yoshida, K.; Yamamoto, K. Effects of Road traffic noise on inhabitants of Tokyo. J. Sound Vib. 1997, 205, 517–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Berglund, B.; Lindvall, T. Community Noise. Available online: http://www.noisesolutions.com/uploads/images/pages/resources/pdfs/WHO%20Community%20Noise.pdf (accessed on 28 March 2015).
  4. WHO Regional Office for Europe. Concern for Europe’s Tomorrow: Health and Environment in the WHO European Region; 1999 Contract No.: Report; WHO Regional Office for Europe: København, Denmark, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  5. Paunović, K.; Jakovljević, B.; Belojević, G. Predictors of noise annoyance in noisy and quiet urban streets. Sci. Total Environ. 2009, 407, 3707–3711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Kono, S.; Sone, T. Residents’ response to environmental and neighborhood noise. J. Sound Vib. 1988, 127, 573–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Aydin, Y.; Kaltenbach, M. Noise perception, heart rate and blood pressure in relation to aircraft noise in the vicinity of the Frankfurt Airport. Clin. Res. Cardiol. 2007, 96, 347–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Ouis, D. Annoyance from road traffic noise: A review. J. Environ. Psychol. 2001, 21, 101–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Shepherd, D.; Welch, D.; Dirks, K.N.; Mathews, R. Exploring the relationship between noise sensitivity, annoyance and health-related quality of life in a sample of adults exposed to environmental noise. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7, 3579–3594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Van Gerven, P.W.; Vos, H.; van Boxtel, M.P.; Janssen, S.A.; Miedema, H.M. Annoyance from environmental noise across the lifespan. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2009, 126, 187–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Babisch, W.; Beule, B.; Schust, M.; Kersten, N.; Ising, H. Traffic Noise and risk of myocardial infarction. Epidemiology 2005, 16, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Gan, W.Q.; Davies, H.W.; Koehoorn, M.; Brauer, M. Association of long-term exposure to community noise and traffic-related air pollution with coronary heart disease mortality. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2012, 175, 898–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Selander, J.; Nilsson, M.E.; Bluhm, G.; Rosenlund, M.; Lindqvist, M.; Nise, G.; Pershagen, G. Long-term exposure to road traffic noise and myocardial infarction. Epidemiology 2009, 20, 272–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Dora, C. A Different route to health: Implication of transport policies. BMJ 1999, 318, 1686–1689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Guski, R.; Felscher-Suhr, U.; Schuemer, R. The Concept of noise annoyance: How international experts see it. J. Sound Vib. 1999, 223, 513–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. ISO. Acoustics—Assessment of Noise Annoyance by Means of Social and Socio-Acoustic Surveys. ISO/TS 15666:2003; ISO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  17. Waye, K.P.; Öhrström, E. Psycho-acoustic characters of relevance for annoyance of wind turbine noise. J. Sound Vib. 2002, 250, 65–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Raw, G.J.; Griffiths, I.D. Individual differences in response to road traffic noise. J. Sound Vib. 1988, 121, 463–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Thomas, J.R.; Jones, D.M. Individual differences in noise annoyance and the uncomfortable loudness level. J. Sound Vib. 1982, 82, 289–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Pathak, V.; Tripathi, B.D.; Mishra, V.K. Evaluation of traffic noise pollution and attitudes of exposed individuals in working place. Atmos. Environ. 2008, 42, 3892–3898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Dratva, J.; Zemp, E.; Felber Dietrich, D.; Bridevaux, P.O.; Rochat, T.; Schindler, C.; Gerbase, M.W. Impact of road traffic noise annoyance on health-related quality of life: Results from a population-based study. Qual. Life Res. 2010, 19, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Langdon, F.J. Noise nuisance caused by road traffic in residential areas: Part III. J. Sound Vib. 1976, 49, 241–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Meijer, H.; Knipschild, P.; Sallé, H. Road traffic noise annoyance in amsterdam. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 1985, 56, 285–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Schreckenberg, D.; Griefahn, B.; Meis, M. The associations between noise sensitivity, reported physical and mental health, perceived environmental quality, and noise annoyance. Noise Health 2010, 12, 7–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Van Kamp, I.; Job, R.F.; Hatfield, J.; Haines, M.; Stellato, R.K.; Stansfeld, S.A. The role of noise sensitivity in the noise-response relation: A comparison of three international airport studies. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2004, 116, 3471–3479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Smith, A. The concept of noise sensitivity: Implications for noise control. Noise Health 2003, 5, 57–59. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  27. Miedema, H.M.; Vos, H. Noise sensitivity and reactions to noise and other environmental conditions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2003, 113, 1492–1504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Hatfield, J.; Job, R.F.; Hede, A.J.; Carter, N.L.; Peploe, P.; Taylor, R.; Morrell, S. Human response to environmental noise: The role of perceived control. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2002, 9, 341–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Weinstein, N.D. Individual differences in critical tendencies and noise annoyance. J. Sound Vib. 1980, 68, 241–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ryu, J.K.; Jeon, J.Y. Influence of noise sensitivity on annoyance of indoor and outdoor noises in residential buildings. Appl. Acoust. 2011, 72, 336–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Stansfeld, S.A.; Sharp, D.; Gallacher, J.; Babisch, W. Road traffic noise, noise sensitivity and psychological disorder. Schriftenr. Ver. Wasser- Boden- Lufthyg. 1993, 88, 167–188. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  32. Watson, D.; Pennebaker, J.W. Health complaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychol. Rev. 1989, 96, 234–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Berglund, B.; Lindvall, T.; Schwela, D.H. Guidelines for Community Noise; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  34. Uppenkamp, S.; Röhl, M. Human auditory neuroimaging of intensity and loudness. Hear. Res. 2014, 307, 65–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. UN. World Population Prospects: The 2012 Revision; United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  36. Intenational Social Survey Programme. Finnish Social Science Data Archive: Environment III (FSD2620 ISSP 2010). Available online: https://services.fsd.uta.fi/catalogue/FSD2620/PIP/quF2620_fin.pdf (accessed on 14 August 2011).
  37. Korpela, K.; Ylen, M.; Tyrvainen, L.; Silvennoinen, H. Determinants of restorative experiences in everyday favorite places. Health Place 2008, 14, 636–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Weinstein, N.D. Individual differences in reactions to noise: A longitudinal study in a college dormitory. J. Appl. Psychol. 1978, 63, 458–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Neitzel, R.; Daniell, W.; Sheppard, L.; Davies, H.; Seixas, N. Comparison of perceived and quantitative measures of occupational noise. Ann. Occup. Hyg. 2009, 53, 41–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Schlaefer, K.; Schlehofer, B.; Schüz, J. Validity of self-reported occupational noise exposure. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2009, 24, 269–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Dzhambov, A.H.; Dimitrova, D.D. Development and feasibility of perceived noise exposure scale. Noise Control Eng. 2014, 62, 102–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Wardman, M.; Bristow, A.L. Traffic related noise and air quality valuations: Evidence from stated preference residential choice models. Transp. Res. D: Transp. Environ. 2004, 9, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Jakovljevic, B.; Paunovic, K.; Belojevic, G. Road-traffic noise and factors influencing noise annoyance in an urban population. Environ. Int. 2009, 35, 552–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Ali, S.A.; Tamura, A. Road traffic noise levels, restrictions and annoyance in Greater Cairo, Egypt. Appl. Acoust. 2003, 64, 815–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Heinonen-Guzejev, M.; Vuorinen, H.S.; Kaprio, J.; Heikkilä, K.; Mussalo-Rauhamaa, H.; Koskenvuo, M. Self-report of transportation noise exposure, annoyance and noise sensitivity in relation to noise map information. J. Sound Vib. 2000, 234, 191–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Baruch, Y. Response rate in academic studies—A comparative analysis. Hum. Relat. 1999, 52, 421–438. [Google Scholar]
  47. Job, R.F. Community response to noise: A review of factors influencing the relationship between noise exposure and reaction. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1988, 83, 991–1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Moreira, N.M.; Bryan, M.E. Noise annoyance susceptibility. J. Sound Vib. 1972, 21, 449–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Miedema, H.M.; Vos, H. Demographic and attitudinal factors that modify annoyance from transportation noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1999, 105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Li, H.N.; Chau, C.K.; Tse, M.S.; Tang, S.K. On the study of the effects of sea views, greenery views and personal characteristics on noise annoyance perception at homes. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2012, 131, 2131–2140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Van Den Berg, A.E.; Agnes, E.; Hartig, T.; Staats, H. Preference for nature in urbanized societies: Stress, restoration, and the pursuit of sustainability. J. Soc. Issues 2007, 63, 79–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Heinonen-Guzejev, M.; Vuorinen, H.S.; Mussalo-Rauhamaa, H.; Heikkilä, K.; Koskenvuo, M.; Kaprio, J. The association of noise sensitivity with coronary heart and cardiovascular mortality among Finnish adults. Sci. Total Environ. 2007, 372, 406–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Björk, J.; Ardö, J.; Stroh, E.; Lövkvist, H.; Ostergren, P.O.; Albin, M. Road traffic noise in Southern Sweden and its relation to annoyance, disturbance of daily activities and health. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2006, 32, 392–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Bluhm, G.; Nordling, E.; Berglind, N. Road traffic noise and annoyance—An increasing environmental health problem. Noise Health 2004, 6, 43–49. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  55. Bodin, T.; Bjork, J.; Ohrstrom, E.; Ardo, J.; Albin, M. Survey context and question wording affects self reported annoyance due to road traffic noise: A comparison between two cross-sectional studies. Environ. Health 2012, 11, 2–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Job, R.F.S. Noise sensitivity as a factor influencing human reaction to noise. Noise Health 1999, 1, 57–68. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  57. Kjellberg, A.; Muhr, P.; Skoldstrom, B. Fatigue after work in noise—An epidemiological survey study and three quasi-experimental field studies. Noise Health 1998, 1, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Okokon, E.O.; Turunen, A.W.; Ung-Lanki, S.; Vartiainen, A.-K.; Tiittanen, P.; Lanki, T. Road-Traffic Noise: Annoyance, Risk Perception, and Noise Sensitivity in the Finnish Adult Population. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 5712-5734. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph120605712

AMA Style

Okokon EO, Turunen AW, Ung-Lanki S, Vartiainen A-K, Tiittanen P, Lanki T. Road-Traffic Noise: Annoyance, Risk Perception, and Noise Sensitivity in the Finnish Adult Population. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2015; 12(6):5712-5734. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph120605712

Chicago/Turabian Style

Okokon, Enembe Oku, Anu W. Turunen, Sari Ung-Lanki, Anna-Kaisa Vartiainen, Pekka Tiittanen, and Timo Lanki. 2015. "Road-Traffic Noise: Annoyance, Risk Perception, and Noise Sensitivity in the Finnish Adult Population" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 12, no. 6: 5712-5734. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph120605712

APA Style

Okokon, E. O., Turunen, A. W., Ung-Lanki, S., Vartiainen, A. -K., Tiittanen, P., & Lanki, T. (2015). Road-Traffic Noise: Annoyance, Risk Perception, and Noise Sensitivity in the Finnish Adult Population. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 12(6), 5712-5734. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph120605712

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop