Next Article in Journal
Use of E-Health in Dutch General Practice during the COVID-19 Pandemic
Next Article in Special Issue
Perception of Competence as Mediator between Motor Competence and Physical Activity
Previous Article in Journal
Efficiency and Productivity of Public Hospitals in Serbia Using DEA-Malmquist Model and Tobit Regression Model, 2015–2019
Previous Article in Special Issue
Accelerometer-Measured Daily Step Counts and Adiposity Indicators among Latin American Adults: A Multi-Country Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Linking Emotional Intelligence, Physical Activity and Aggression among Undergraduates

by
José Luis Ubago-Jiménez
1,
Mar Cepero-González
1,
Asunción Martínez-Martínez
2 and
Fátima Chacón-Borrego
3,*
1
Department of Didactics of Musical, Plastic and Corporal Expression, University of Granada, 18071 Granada, Spain
2
Department of Research and Diagnosis Methods in Education, University of Granada, 18071 Granada, Spain
3
Department of Physical Education and Sports, University of Sevilla, 41013 Sevilla, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(23), 12477; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182312477
Submission received: 22 October 2021 / Revised: 24 November 2021 / Accepted: 25 November 2021 / Published: 26 November 2021

Abstract

:
Several indicators are strongly related to health and well-being in university students, such as emotional intelligence and physical activity. At the same time, some qualities threaten it and are incompatible with students’ adaptation to society in general, such as aggressive behaviours. The aim of this research is to analyse the relationship established between emotional intelligence using TMMS-24, physical activity, using IPAQ, and aggression behaviours in university students. For this purpose, a descriptive, cross-sectional and non-experimental study was developed with 932 undergraduates (M = 20.55; SD = 3.673). The findings highlight how emotional intelligence and physical activity practice decrease violent behaviour in university students. In addition, men tend to have more aggressive behaviours than women as well as the relation between physical activity and emotional intelligence is stronger in all its dimensions. Results highlight the importance of including emotional intelligence programs in order to enable undergraduate well-being.

1. Introduction

University is a stage of psychophysical transition, where adolescents tend to adopt new habits in order to feel socially included [1]. Likewise, the university period itself generates stressful situations caused by academic performance [2,3].
These stressful situations often induce actions and behaviours that, according to Appelqvist-Schmidlechner et al. [4], are determined by the perception of the individual’s self-concept. The effects of attachment or emotional engagement have also been found in adolescence on their self-confidence, their ability to establish and maintain relationships with others, and their importance and influence in the peer group [5]. Aggressive behaviour in youth is a global public health problem due to its emotional, social, and economic consequences [6].
In this context, physical activity plays a key role in promoting an active and healthy lifestyle in students, with multiple academic, psychosocial, and sport-physical benefits [7,8]. Moderate physical activity generates a multitude of benefits such as reducing cardiovascular diseases [9,10] and gradually lowering levels of obesity and overweight [11]. In addition, in the cognitive sphere, active lifestyles significantly reduce stress and anxiety levels, improving mood and promoting socioaffective and emotional self-regulation, self-esteem, and well-being [12,13,14,15].
These behaviours provoke conflicts like bullying, which is an issue of growing incidence in recent years [16]. This has led to a proliferation in the number of research projects looking for better strategies to address this violence phenomenon. Placing a focus on physical activity, it is found how recent literature reviews [17,18,19] have concluded that through the practice of physical activity, levels of aggression among this population can be significantly reduced.
Sport practice is a way of transmitting values, showing respect for others, as well as compliance with a system or rules [20]. In addition to the mentioned sporting benefits, studies such as Zurita-Ortega et al. [21] have shown how physical activity is a factor in the regulation of aggression among adolescents.
Authors like Singh and Sachdev [22], Vaquero-Solís et al. [23] and Rodríguez-Romo et al. [24] confirm how emotional intelligence (EI) and physical activity are closely related. In fact, the influence exerted by emotional intelligence improves sport performance, which helps to increase levels of satisfaction and decision making among participants in physical activity. On the other hand, although it is known to be important in sports performance [25,26], EI is not seen as a key factor due to insufficient knowledge generated by EI interventions, limited dedication to training, or mistaken thinking about the term [27,28].
External factors such as the place of residence, culture, or society itself can affect all of this. Indeed, some of the aggressive responses of young people are a consequence of the behaviour promoted in the media and social networks.
Taking into account the above, the aim of this research is to analyse the established relationship between EI, physical activity, and aggression in university students. In order to achieve this, the following hypotheses are considered: (a) there is an inverse relationship between EI and aggression; and (b) there is an inverse relationship between physical activity and aggression.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects and Design

Research was conducted following a descriptive, cross-sectional, and non-experimental design with a sample of 932 students from the universities of Almería, Cádiz, Córdoba, Granada, Huelva, Jaén, Málaga, and Sevilla (Spain) using convenience sampling. The departments of corporal expression of different universities were contacted to provide questionnaires to the students. After permission was obtained, students were invited to participate in the research, and 958 students participated, with the inclusion criteria being that the participants had to be signed up in any of the four university courses in the universities described. Its sex-specific distribution is 68.3% (n = 637) for women and 31.7% (n = 295) for men.
More specifically, the average age of the participants was between 18 and 28 years old (20.55 ± 3.673). It is also to be noted that 26 questionnaires were invalidated due to some errors in their completion, missing items, or duplicate answers.

2.2. Instruments

Four instruments were used in this investigation. An ad hoc questionnaire was used to collect sociodemographic data (sex and age).
The second one is the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ), which was developed in Geneva in 1998 [29] and it is one of the most widely used questionnaires to measure physical activity levels in a specific sample. This questionnaire is divided into four domains (work, transport, home activities, and leisure time), where the frequency and duration of physical activity lasts for more than 10 min. The final result is obtained in three categories: vigorous (VPA), moderate (MPA), and light (LPA) activities. For the present study, the results obtained a reliability of α = 0.891.
The Trait Meta-Mood Scale-24 (TMMS-24), by Fernández-Berrocal et al. [30], was used to measure EI. Its original version is the one developed by Salovey et al. [31]. This tool has been used in many social science research contexts. The reliability obtained in our study is of α = 0.901 for the dimension ‘Emotional Attention’ (EA); α = 0.887 and α = 0.915 for ‘Clarity feelings’ (CF); and for ‘Emotional Repair’ (ER) some indexes of α = 0.871.
For the measurement of violent behaviour, the “Violent Behavior at School Scale”, proposed in its original version by Little et al. [32] and adapted to Spanish by Estévez [33] was used. This questionnaire is divided into two categories: manifest aggression and relational aggression, which are further subdivided into three subscales: pure, reactive, and instrumental. It is also answered by means of a 25-item Likert-type scale ranging from 1 = never to 4 = always. Once the scale is completed, two types of violent behaviour are obtained: manifest aggression, which is generated in a personal encounter between the aggressor and the victim; and relational aggression, which is considered when the aggressor remains anonymous. Reliability in the present study was obtained with a Cronbach’s Alpha of α = 0.798 for items measuring manifest aggression and α = 0.735 for relational aggression.

2.3. Procedure

In order to conduct the study, the procedure was carried out in several stages. The first step was to request a research authorisation and ethics committee from the Faculty of Education Sciences of the University of Granada (Granada, Spain), obtaining the permission code 1478/CEIH/2020. The second phase was to draft a research document explaining scientific aims and research topics, and to request consent for the students’ participation. After they agreed to participate in the study, the questionnaire was sent by email for honest completion.
The study was completed by 958 undergraduate students, and 26 questionnaires had to be eliminated because they were not properly completed. Data collection was carried out between May and June 2021, ensuring that the confidentiality of the answers was guaranteed. Data collection and analysis were carried out in accordance with the ethical principles established by the Declaration of Helsinki in 1975 and its update carried out in Brazil in 2013.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

IBM SPSS 25.0 statistical software (International Business Machines Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA) was used during the data analysis process. The means and frequencies of the variables were calculated. To obtain the effect size, Cohen’s standardised d was used, which can be interpreted as zero effect (0–0.19); low effect (0.20–0.49); medium effect (0.50–0.79); or high effect (≥0.80). The 95% confidence interval was also determined.
For testing and understanding differences between correlations, the effect size was calculated with Cohen’s q. In addition, Fisher’s Z was calculated by subtracting both correlations from each other. Furthermore, to understand this measurement, the parameters: no effect (≤0.1); small effect (0.1–0.3); medium effect (0.3–0.5); and large effect (0.5) were established. Differences between categorical variables were calculated using contingency tables. On the other hand, ANOVA and Student’s t-test were used to analyse differences between categorical and interval variables. In addition, Bonferroni’s test was used to check for intergroup differences. Finally, the linear regression analysis was designed to establish and understand the association between EI and physical activity (independent variables) and aggressive behaviours (dependent variable), adjusted for sex.

3. Results

Table 1 shows the results achieved in the variables of the present investigation in relation to the sex of the participants. Statistically significant relationships were found in MPA and VPA (p = 0.001; p = 0.013) for men. For EI, men showed higher average values for ER (M = 3.72; SD = ±0.721) (p = 0.004) and CF (M = 3.66; SD = ±0.729) (p = 0.007) than women. There are also higher mean values for women in EA, (M = 4.08; SD = 0.712) (p = 0.001).
In relation to aggressive behaviours, statistically significant relationships are found in all dimensions with higher values for men. MA values (M = 1.30; SD = 0.330 vs. M = 1.24; SD = 0.270) representing a small effect size (d = 0.207); PMA (M = 1.54; SD = 0.490 vs. M = 1.43; SD = 0.430) representing a small effect size (d = 0.245); RMA (M = 1.08; SD = 0.209 vs. M = 1.05; SD = 0.177) representing a small effect size (d = 0.160); IMA (M = 1.31; SD = 0.353 vs. M = 1.26; SD = 0.312) representing a small effect size (d = 0.154); RA (M = 1.51; SD = 0.449 vs. M = 1.43; SD = 0.405) representing a small effect size (d = 0.191); PRA (M = 1.16; SD = 0.315 vs. M = 1.09; SD = 0.247) representing a small effect size (d = 0.259); RRA (M = 1.29; SD = 0.259 vs. M = 1.23; SD = 0.220) representing a small effect size (d = 0.257); and IRA (M = 1.33; SD = 0.304 vs. M = 1.26; SD = 0.267) representing a small effect size (d = 0.251).
The correlational analysis between the research variables for women is shown in Table 2. The strongest relationships are found between the dimensions of VPA and EI, CF (r = 0.722), EA (r = 0.704), and GEI (r = 0.686). Likewise, in terms of MPA, a moderate relationship is found with GEI (r = 0.612) and EA (r = 0.478). On the other hand, a strong relationship is found between LPA and GEI (r = 0.627).
The correlational analysis between the research variables for men is shown in Table 3. Negative and direct relationships were obtained for physical activity and aggressive behaviours. For VPA practice, a negative relationship was found with RMA (r = −0.134), IMA (r = −0.118), PMA (−0.111), MA (r = −0.105), and RA (r = −0.058). For MPA practice with MA (r = −0.097), RMA (r = −0.072), PMA (r = −0.057), RA (r = −0.041), PRA (r = −0.033), and IMA (r = −0.029). Additionally, negative relationships were also found between LPA and MA (r = −0.068), IMA (r = −0.054), RMA (r = −0.041), PMA (r = −0.033) and RA (r = −0.015).
The correlations between EI and aggressive behaviours also reported negative and direct relationships between GEI and IMA (r = −0.126), MA (r = −0.118), and RMA (r = −0.099). The dimension of ER was related to IMA (r = −0.154), MA (r = −0.137), and PMA (r = −0.130). For the CF dimension, a negative relationship was found with RRA (r = −0.118).
The correlations for men reported slightly higher levels than for women. The relationships between the VPA and EI dimensions reported strong correlations with CF (r = 0.489), EA (r = 0.766), GEI (r = 0.712), and ER (r = 0.521). Similarly, MPA reported correlations with GEI (r = 0.697), EA (r = 0.511), ER (r = 0.389), and CF (r = 0.324); while LPA correlated more strongly with GEI (r = 0.658) and ER (r = 0.350).
In relation to physical activity and aggressive behaviours, negative and direct relationships were obtained. VPA was related to MA (r = −0.176), RMA (r = −0.136), PMA (r = −0.124), IMA (r = −0.105), and RA (r = −0.092). MPA was correlated with MA (r = −0.115), RMA (r = −0.098), PMA (r = −0.065), IMA (r = −0.078), RA (r = −0.063), and PRA (r = −0.028). LPA correlated with MA (r = −0.124), IMA (r = −0.089), RMA (r = −0.047), and PMA (r = −0.025).
Finally, the relationships between EI and aggressive behaviours were negatively presented. GEI was correlated with MA (r = −0.112), PMA (r = −0.123), RMA (r = −0.071), and IMA (r = −0.073). RE was correlated with PMA (r = −0.154), MA (r = −0.127), IMA (r = −0.113), PRA (r = −0.086), RA (r = −0.080) and RRA (r = −0.066). Finally, CF was correlated with PMA (r = −0.097) and MA (r = −0.078).
Linear regression analyses were performed to verify the association between EI, physical activity, and aggressive behaviours. Regression was also carried out to differentiate between men and women (Table 4). For predictive models in relation to men, EI and physical activity are a predictive variable of MA (β = 0.177; p = 0.015; β = 0.184; p = 0.018), which explains 32% of the variance of the response variables; for the PMA dimension (β = 0.155; p = 0.009; β = 0.188; p = 0.016) explaining 20% of the variance; RMA (β = 0.028; p = 0.008; β = 0.034; p = 0.020) predictive value decreases to 15%; and EI is a predictor (2%) of the IMA dimension (β = 0.151; p = 0.024).
On the other hand, in the case of women, physical activity was a predictive variable of PMA (β = 0.263; p = 0.025), explaining 17% of the variance, and EI was a predictive variable of RMA (β = 0.375; p = 0.002) explaining 15% of the variance. For the remaining dimensions of aggressive behaviours, none of the introduced variables were significant predictors.

4. Discussion

According to the purpose of the study, the relationship between EI, physical activity, and aggressive behaviours in university students has been analysed; highlighting how EI and the practice of physical activity decrease violent behaviour in university students.
As a general observation, based on the results obtained, it has been observed that men have higher values in all dimensions of aggressive behaviour than women. Likewise, men have higher aggressive levels than women in both their manifest and relational expressions. Similar results are consistent with most research studies on sex differences related to aggressive behaviour [21,34,35,36,37,38,39,40]. In contrast, these findings are opposed to those reflected in the study by Blasco and Orgilés [41], who found that women were more inclined to physical aggression than men in a population of football players between 7 and 17 years old.
Considering relationships between EI and aggressive behaviours, weak negative relationships were found according to previous studies [42]. Students with high levels of GEI have lower rates of manifest aggression and its dimensions. It can also be observed that students with higher scores on the ER dimension have lower scores for both manifest and relational aggression. These data are consistent with studies such as Bibi et al. [43] and Antoñanzas [44], which found inverse relationships between EI and aggressive behaviours. Alvarado et al. [45] and Segura et al. [46] have shown that young people with a higher level of EI are less likely to show any type of aggression.
The relationship between physical activity practice and aggressive behaviours shows how physical activity practice in its three modalities (VPA, MPA, and LPA) reduces aggressive behaviours. Likewise, it shows that students’ aggressive behaviour decreases with higher levels of physical activity. The findings show that adherence to physical activity helps reduce aggressive behaviour [47,48,49]. Taking into account that physical activity practice is a factor to reduce stress and release tension, it could also be said to reduce aggressive behaviour. Furthermore, studies such as Park et al. [50] and Jenkins et al. [51] show how physical activity reduces aggression among adolescents.
In addition, the relationship between EI and PA was found to be direct and strong for the sample surveyed. In particular, it revealed that students who practiced more physical activity had higher levels of general emotional intelligence, as well as all its dimensions. These results are consistent with previous studies such as those conducted by Wang et al. [28] and Acebes et al. [52] or the one developed by Roy et al. [53].
Moreover, it is necessary to point out the limitations associated with this research. First of all, it was conducted with undergraduates in order to match a sample with similar research projects. Furthermore, it was a descriptive and cross-sectional design and convenience sampling which gives valuable information concerning current issues, but also avoids casual conclusions. Another limitation concerns the instruments used, since, although they have a high reliability, it would have been interesting to measure physical activity with accelerometry instead of a reported test. Recognising the above limitations, the following suggestions are made for future research. The research should be replicated in and extended to other countries with similar socio-economic characteristics. In addition, it would be interesting to develop an intervention program to test a combined influence of EI and PA on the reduction of aggressive behaviour in university students.

5. Conclusions

Principal findings showed how physical activity and EI help reduce aggressive behaviours. Additionally, IE was negatively associated with aggressive behaviours, suggesting a greater ability to control emotions, which means lower problems associated with aggressive behaviours. These negative relationships found between EI point to an important point to address. According to initial hypotheses, it was found that the higher the physical activity intensity, the lower the aggressiveness indexes. It is necessary to emphasize physical activity not only to improve or maintain physical health, but also to improve one’s own behaviour, since it releases accumulated stress. In fact, the main implications of the present study are oriented to the consolidation of evidence for designing and developing psychophysical-healthy programs in higher education.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.L.U.-J. and A.M.-M.; methodology, A.M.-M.; formal analysis, J.L.U.-J., M.C.-G. and F.C.-B.; data curation, J.L.U.-J.; writing—original draft preparation, J.L.U.-J., A.M.-M., M.C.-G. and F.C.-B.; writing—review and editing, J.L.U.-J., A.M.-M., M.C.-G. and F.C.-B.; supervision, J.L.U.-J., A.M.-M., M.C.-G. and F.C.-B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Granada (1478/CEIH/2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Cooper, K.M.; Gin, L.E.; Barnes, E.; Brownell, S.E. An Exploratory Study of Students with Depression in Undergraduate Research Experiences. CBE—Life Sci. Educ. 2020, 19, ar19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. De La Fuente, J.; Amate, J.; González-Torres, M.C.; Artuch, R.; García-Torrecillas, J.M.; Fadda, S. Effects of Levels of Self-Regulation and Regulatory Teaching on Strategies for Coping With Academic Stress in Undergraduate Students. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Yusufov, M.; Nicoloro-SantaBarbara, J.; Grey, N.E.; Moyer, A.; Lobel, M. Meta-analytic evaluation of stress reduction interventions for undergraduate and graduate students. Int. J. Stress Manag. 2019, 26, 132–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Appelqvist-Schmidlechner, K.; Vaara, J.; Häkkinen, A.; Vasankari, T.; Mäkinen, J.; Mäntysaari, M.; Kyröläinen, H. Relationships Between Youth Sports Participation and Mental Health in Young Adulthood Among Finnish Males. Am. J. Health Promot. 2017, 32, 1502–1509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Marshall, S.L.; Parker, P.; Ciarrochi, J.; Sahdra, B.; Jackson, C.J.; Heaven, P.C. Self-compassion protects against the negative effects of low self-esteem: A longitudinal study in a large adolescent sample. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2015, 74, 116–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. World Health Organization. Preventing Youth Violence: An Overview of the Evidence. 2021. Available online: www.who.int (accessed on 29 September 2021).
  7. Cuervo, C.; Cachón, J.; Zagalaz, M.L.; de Mesa González-González, C. Cómo me veo: Estudio diacrónico de la imagen cor-poral. Instrum. Eval. Magister 2017, 29, 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Emeljanovas, A.; Valantine, I.; Zaicenkoviene, K.; Misigoj-Durakovic, M.; Kreivyte, R.; Gómez-Ruano, M.A. Associations between physical activity and health-related physical fitness in 17 years-old girls. R. Psico. Dep. 2017, 26, 157–166. [Google Scholar]
  9. Garn, A.C.; Morin, A.J.S.; White, R.L.; Owen, K.B.; Donley, W.; Lonsdale, C. Moderate-to-vigorous physical activity as a predictor of changes in physical self-concept in adolescents. Health Psychol. 2020, 39, 190–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Newby, K.; Varnes, L.; Yorke, E.; Meisel, S.F.; Fisher, A. Illness Risk Representation beliefs underlying adolescents’ cardiovascular disease risk appraisals and the preventative role of physical activity. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2019, 25, 171–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Lutz, N.; Clarys, P.; Koenig, I.; Deliens, T.; Taeymans, J.; Verhaeghe, N. Health economic evaluations of interventions to increase physical activity and decrease sedentary behavior at the workplace: A systematic review. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health 2019, 46, 127–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Hakun, J.G.; Findeison, M.A. Cognitive control moderates the health benefits of trait self-regulation in young adults. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2019, 152, 109572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Martland, R.; Mondelli, V.; Gaughran, F.; Stubbs, B. Can high-intensity interval training improve physical and mental health outcomes? A meta-review of 33 systematic reviews across the lifespan. J. Sports Sci. 2019, 38, 430–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Spring, B.; Champion, K.E.; Acabchuk, R.; Hennessy, E.A. Self-regulatory behaviour change techniques in interventions to promote healthy eating, physical activity, or weight loss: A meta-review. Health Psychol. Rev. 2020, 15, 508–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Tweed, L.M.; Rogers, E.N.; Kinnafick, F.-E. Literature on peer-based community physical activity programmes for mental health service users: A scoping review. Health Psychol. Rev. 2020, 15, 287–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Carretero, R.; Nolasco, A. Acoso escolar y diversidad. Relación del acoso escolar con la percepción de normalidad en víctimas y agresores. Rev. Educ. 2021, 392, 155–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Jiménez-Barbero, J.A.; Jiménez-Loaisa, A.; González-Cutre, D.; Beltrán-Carrillo, V.J.; Llor-Zaragoza, L.; Ruiz-Hernández, J.A. Physical education and school bullying: A systematic review. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedag. 2019, 25, 79–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Thompson, I.; Hong, J.S.; Lee, J.M.; Prys, N.A.; Morgan, J.T.; Udo-Inyang, I. A review of the empirical research on weight-based bullying and peer victimisation published between 2006 and 2016. Educ. Rev. 2018, 72, 88–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Melguizo-Ibáñez, E.; García-Pérez, L.; Polo-Rodríguez, A. Incidence of bullying and loneliness behaviors on the diet followed and the practice of physical activity in primary school students. ESHPA Educ. Sport Health Phys. Act. 2021, 5, 227–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Bermejo, J.; Borrás, P.; Ponseti, F. El fairplay en edad escolar: Programa “ponemos valores al deporte”. Rev. Ibero. Cienc. Act. Fís. Deporte 2019, 8, 59–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Zurita-Ortega, F.; Castro-Sánchez, M.; Ubago-Jiménez, J.L.; Chacón-Cuberos, R.; González-Valero, G. La conducta violenta y actividad física de los adolescentes en acogimiento residencial. SporTK 2018, 7 (Suppl. S2), 97–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Singh, M.; Sachdev, S. Correlation between General Intelligence, Emotional Intelligence and Stress Response after One Month Practice of Moderate Intensity Physical Exercise. J. Exerc. Physiol. 2020, 23, 38–47. [Google Scholar]
  23. Vaquero-Solís, M.; Amado-Alonso, D.; Sánchez-Oliva, D.; Sánchez-Migue, P.A.; Iglesias-Gallego, D. Emotional intelligence in adolescence: Motivation and physical activity. Rev. Int. Med. Cienc. Act. Fís. Deporte 2020, 20, 119–131. [Google Scholar]
  24. Rodriguez-Romo, G.; Blanco-Garcia, C.; Diez-Vega, I.; Acebes-Sánchez, J. Emotional Intelligence of Undergraduate Athletes: The Role of Sports Experience. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 609154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Arribas-Galarraga, S.; Cecchini, J.A.; Luis-De-Cos, I.; Saies, E.; Luis-De-Cos, G. Influence of emotional intelligence on sport performance in elite canoeist. J. Hum. Sport Exer. 2020, 15, 772–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Merino-Fernández, M.; Dal Bello, F.; Mota-Barreto, L.B.; Brito, C.J.; Miarka, B.; López-Díaz-de-Durana, A. State-trait anxiety and reduced emotional intelligence in combat sport athletes of different genders and competitive levels. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 2019, 19 (Suppl. S2), 363–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Adamakis, M.; Dania, A. Validity of Emotional Intelligence scale in pre-service Physical Education teachers. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 2021, 21, 54–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Wang, K.; Yang, Y.; Zhang, T.; Ouyang, Y.; Liu, B.; Luo, J. The Relationship Between Physical Activity and Emotional Intelligence in College Students: The Mediating Role of Self-Efficacy. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. IPAQ Research Committee. Guidelines for Data Processing and Analysis of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ)—Short and Long Forms. 2005. Available online: https://sites.google.com/site/theipaq/scoring-protocol (accessed on 14 June 2021).
  30. Fernández-Berrocal, P.; Extremera, N.; Ramos, N. Validity and reliability of the Spanish modified version of the Trait Me-ta-Mood Scale. Psychol. Rep. 2004, 94, 751–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Salovey, P.; Mayer, J.D.; Goldman, S.L.; Turvey, C.; Palfai, T.P. Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. In Emotion, Disclosure, & Health; Pennebaker, J.W., Ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1995; pp. 125–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Little, T.D.; Henrich, C.C.; Jones, S.M.; Hawley, P.H. Disentangling the “whys” from the “whats” of aggressive behaviour. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2003, 27, 122–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Estévez, E. Violencia, Victimización y Rechazo Escolar en la Adolescencia. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  34. Domínguez-Alonso, J.; López-Castedo, A.; Nieto-Campos, B. Violencia escolar: Diferencias de género en estudiantes de secundaria. Rev. Complut. Educ. 2019, 30, 1031–1044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Rodríguez, V.D.; Deaño, M.D.; González, F.T. Incidencia de los distintos tipos de violencia escolar en Educación Primaria y Secundaria. Aula Abiert 2020, 49, 373–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Maquibar, A.; Estalella, I.; Vives-Cases, C.; Hurtig, A.-K.; Goicolea, I. Analysing training in gender-based violence for undergraduate nursing students in Spain: A mixed-methods study. Nurse Educ. Today 2019, 77, 71–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Sánchez-Zafra, M.; Zagalaz-Sánchez, M.L.; Cachón-Zagalaz, J. Análisis de las Conductas Violentas en la escuela en función del género y el tipo de Centro. ESHPA-Educ. Sport Health Phys. Act. 2018, 2, 16–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Strøm, I.F.; Hjemdal, O.K.; Myhre, M.C.; Wentzel-Larsen, T.; Thoresen, S. The Social Context of Violence: A Study of Repeated Victimization in Adolescents and Young Adults. J. Interpers. Violence 2017, 35, 2210–2235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Walters, G.D. School-Age Bullying Victimization and Perpetration: A Meta-Analysis of Prospective Studies and Research. Trauma Violence Abus. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Zych, I.; Viejo, C.; Vila, E.; Farrington, D.P. School Bullying and Dating Violence in Adolescents: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Trauma Violence Abus. 2019, 22, 397–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Blasco, M.; Orgilés, M. Agresividad en menores de 18 años jugadores de fútbol: Diferencias en función del sexo y edad y en comparación con los jugadores de baloncesto. Cuad. Psicol. Deporte 2014, 14, 21–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Vega, A.; Cabello, R.; Megías-Robles, A.; Gómez-Leal, R.; Fernández-Berrocal, P. Emotional Intelligence and Aggressive Behaviors in Adolescents: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Trauma Violence Abus. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Bibi, A.; Saleem, A.; Khalid, M.A.; Shafique, N. Emotional Intelligence and Aggression among University Students of Pakistan: A Correlational Study. J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma 2020, 29, 1189–1203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Antoñanzas, J. The Relationship of Personality, Emotional Intelligence, and Aggressiveness in Students: A Study Using the Big Five Personality Questionnaire for Children and Adults (BFQ-NA). Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2020, 11, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Alvarado, J.M.; Jiménez-Blanco, A.; Artola, T.; Sastre, S.; Azañedo, C.M. Emotional Intelligence and the Different Manifestations of Bullying in Children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 8842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Segura, L.; Estévez, J.F.; Estévez, E. Empathy and Emotional Intelligence in Adolescent Cyberaggressors and Cybervictims. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Herazo-Beltrán, Y.; Campo-Ternera, L.; García-Puello, F.; Méndez, O.; Suarez-Villa, M.; Vásquez-De la Hoz, F.; Núñez-Bravo, N. Relationship between physical activity and emotional intelligence and bullying among school children. J. Sport Psychol. 2019, 28, 97–103. [Google Scholar]
  48. Georgiev, G.; Gontarev, S. Impact of Physical Activity on the Aggressiveness, Deviant Behavior and Self-esteem with School Children Aged 11-15. J. Anthr. Sport Phys. Educ. 2019, 3, 21–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Pálvölgyi, Á.; Ács, P.; Betlehem, J.; Morvay-Sey, K. The effect of differing levels of physical activity on dispositional mindfulness, trait anxiety, and trait aggression. Health Probl. Civiliz. 2020, 14, 183–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Park, S.; Chiu, W.; Won, D. Correction: Effects of physical education, extracurricular sports activities, and leisure satisfaction on adolescent aggressive behavior: A latent growth modeling approach. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0251221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Jenkins, M.; Hargreaves, E.A.; Hodge, K. Examining the Relationships Among Cognitive Acceptance, Behavioral Commitment, Autonomous Extrinsic Motivation, and Physical Activity. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2020, 42, 177–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Acebes-Sánchez, J.; Diez-Vega, I.; Esteban-Gonzalo, S.; Rodriguez-Romo, G. Physical activity and emotional intelligence among undergraduate students: A correlational study. BMC Public Health 2019, 19, 1241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Roy, B.; Chandrasekaran, S.K.; Manzoor, S.; Ramesh, S.A.; Quan, C.W.; Deshan Bhagya Pathirana, G.P.; Susan Shenes Jayasekara, K.D. The association between physical activity and emotional intelligence: A cross sectional study from Quest International University, Perak. Quest I J. Med. Health Sci. 2021, 4, 4–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. EI, aggressive behaviours and PA practice according to participants’ sex.
Table 1. EI, aggressive behaviours and PA practice according to participants’ sex.
VariableMenWomen Levene’s TestSig.Es (d)95% CI
MS.D.MS.D.FSig.(Bilateral)
LPA754.21198542.8994.35.2380.0750.8540.199(0.061; 0.337)
MPA1045.61587.2631.51240.86.3240.0010.001 *0.305(0.166; 0.443)
VPA264.5793.182.3421.72.5490.1040.013 *0.322(0.183; 0.461)
GEI3.720.5373.700.5751.6250.2030.4770.036(−0.103; 0.174)
ER3.720.7213.570.8115.1100.0240.004 *0.202(0.063; 0.34)
CF3.660.7293.520.8144.4680.0350.007 *0.178(0.039; 0.316)
EA3.780.7454.080.7120.6490.4210.001 *−0.415(−0.554; −0.276)
MA1.300.3301.240.27022.0130.0010.009 *0.207(0.068; 0.345)
PMA1.540.4901.430.43013.6830.0010.001 *0.245(0.106; 0.383)
RMA1.080.2091.050.17718.3250.0010.016 *0.160(0.022; 0.298)
IMA1.310.3531.260.31210.2510.0010.035 *0.154(0.015; 0.292)
RA1.510.4491.430.4051.9510.1630.010 *0.191(0.052; 0.329)
PRA1.160.3151.090.24725.3500.0010.001 *0.259(0.120; 0.397)
RRA1.290.2591.230.22021.2960.0010.001 *0.257(0.119; 0.396)
IRA1.330.3041.260.2679.5920.0020.001 *0.251(0.112; 0.389)
Note: low physical activity (LPA); moderate physical activity (MPA); vigorous physical activity (VPA); general emotional intelligence (GEI); emotional repair (ER); clarity feelings (CF); emotional attention (EA); manifest aggression (MA); pure manifest aggression (PMA); reactive manifest aggression (RMA); instrumental manifest aggression (IMA); relational aggression (RA); pure relational aggression (PRA); reactive relational aggression (RRA); instrumental relational aggression (IRA) * p < 0.05.
Table 2. Bivariate correlations between PA, EI, and aggressive behaviours for women.
Table 2. Bivariate correlations between PA, EI, and aggressive behaviours for women.
LPAMPAVPAGEIERCFEAMAPMARMAIMARAPRARRAIRA
LPA-0.394 **0.455 **0.627 **0.338 **0.210 **0.124 **−0.068 **−0.033 **−0.041 **−0.054 **−0.015 **−0.014−0.018−0.057
MPA -0.502 **0.612 **0.299 **0.358 **0.478 **−0.097 **−0.057 **−0.072 **−0.029 **−0.041 **−0.033 **−0.019−0.015
VPA -0.686 **0.493 **0.722 **0.704 **−0.105 **−0.111 **−0.134 **−0.118 **−0.058 **−0.022−0.004−0.050
GEI -0.697 **0.823 **0.682 **−0.118 **−0.055−0.099 *−0.126 *−0.070−0.048−0.094−0.015
ER -0.438 **0.109−0.137 **−0.130 **−0.066−0.154 **−0.081−0.112−0.0900.018
CF -0.378 **−0.086−0.046−0.060−0.108−0.082−0.035−0.118 *−0.030
EA -−0.038−0.052−0.092−0.0170.0070.039−0.002−0.020
MA -0.767 **0.830 **0.695 **0.713 **0.579 **0.612 **0.546 **
PMA -0.376 **0.502 **0.547 **0.496 **0.408 **0.445 **
RMA -0.353 **0.527 **0.361 **0.554 **0.341 **
IMA -0.607 **0.478 **0.450 **0.611 **
RA -0.819 **0.878 **0.757 **
PRA -0.558 **0.474 **
RRA -0.502 **
IRA -
Note: low physical activity (LPA); moderate physical activity (MPA); vigorous physical activity (VPA); general emotional intelligence (GEI); emotional repair (ER); clarity feelings (CF); emotional attention (EA); manifest aggression (MA); pure manifest aggression (PMA); reactive manifest aggression (RMA); instrumental manifest aggression (IMA); relational aggression (RA); pure relational aggression (PRA); reactive relational aggression (RRA); instrumental relational aggression (IRA) **: correlation significant at the 0.01 level (bilateral); *: correlation significant at the 0.05 level (bilateral).
Table 3. Bivariate correlations between PA, EI, and aggressive behaviours for men.
Table 3. Bivariate correlations between PA, EI, and aggressive behaviours for men.
LPAMPAVPAGEIERCFEAMAPMARMAIMARAPRARRAIRA
LPA-0.462 **0.345 **0.658 **0.350 **0.125 **0.103 **−0.124 **−0.025 **−0.047 **−0.089 **−0.005−0.007−0.005−0.102
MPA -0.568 **0.697 **0.389 **0.324 **0.511 **−0.115 **−0.065 **−0.098 **−0.078 **−0.063 **−0.028 **−0.009−0.022
VPA -0.712 **0.521 **0.789 **0.766 **−0.176 **−0.124 **−0.136 **−0.105 **−0.092 **−0.031−0.012−0.037
GEI -0.735 **0.821 **0.634 **−0.112 **−0.123 **−0.071 *−0.073 *−0.050−0.046−0.044−0.032
ER -0.466 **0.119 **−0.127 **−0.154 **−0.056−0.113 *−0.080 *−0.086 **−0.066 *−0.044
CF -0.315 **−0.078 *−0.097 **−0.053−0.031−0.031−0.022−0.045−0.002
EA -−0.037−0.014−0.047-0.0140.0040.0100.016-0.025
MA -0.746 **0.839 **0.704 **0.705 **0.545 **0.634 **0.547 **
PMA -0.360 **0.487 **0.536 **0.480 **0.438 **0.408 **
RMA -0.353 **0.527 **0.361 **0.554 **0.341 **
IMA -0.607 **0.478 **0.450 **0.611 **
RA -0.819 **0.878 **0.757 **
PRA -0.558 **0.474 **
RRA -0.502 **
IRA -
Note: low physical activity (LPA); moderate physical activity (MPA); vigorous physical activity (VPA); general emotional intelligence (GEI); emotional repair (ER); clarity feelings (CF); emotional attention (EA); manifest aggression (MA); pure manifest aggression (PMA); reactive manifest aggression (RMA); instrumental manifest aggression (IMA); relational aggression (RA); pure relational aggression (PRA); reactive relational aggression (RRA); instrumental relational aggression (IRA) **: correlation significant at the 0.01 level (bilateral); *: correlation significant at the 0.05 level (bilateral).
Table 4. Regression model for EI, PA and aggressive behaviours.
Table 4. Regression model for EI, PA and aggressive behaviours.
VariablesStandardized βtp95% CIR2Adjusted R2
MenWomenMenWomenMenWomenMenWomenMenWomenMenWomen
Manifest Aggression 0.2440.1470.3270.107
EI0.1770.125−1.127−1.0960.0150.078(−4.05; 0.46)(−3.87; 0.35)
PA0.1840.322−0.251−0.2850.0180.095(−1.74; 1.68)(−1.55; 1.22)
Pure Manifest Aggression 0.2560.1220.2070.174
EI0.1550.215−0.742−0.7180.0090.257(−3.83; 0.21)(−3.64; 0.31)
PA0.1880.2630.4590.4260.0160.025(−1.08; 1.98)(−1.14; 1.33)
Reactive Manifest Aggression 0.1270.1020.1520.148
EI0.0280.3750.1080.1060.0080.002(−1.51; 1.58)(−1.28; 1.42)
PA0.0340.2040.1640.0910.0200.075(−1.97; 0.73)(−1.84; 0.85)
Instrumental Manifest Aggression 0.1920.1300.1560.025
EI0.1810.284−0.401−0.6190.0240.119(−1.67; 0.59)(−1.74; 0.62)
PA0.1580.312−0.318−0.5210.0620.317(−1.87; 1.52)(−1.48; 1.05)
Relational Aggression 0.1080.1610.1980.119
EI0.4790.2520.7150.7210.6510.322(−1.09; 3.11)(−1.12; 2.54)
PA0.5770.5470.6570.6430.3460.218(−1.23; 1.97)(−1.08; 1.67)
Pure Relational Aggression 0.1410.1290.1230.097
EI0.8040.4860.7410.6280.1630.412(−2.87; 1.54)(−1.91; 1.23)
PA0.4540.1980.9030.3520.1980.235(−2.46; 0.89)(−2.32; 1.43)
Reactive Relational Aggression 0.1330.1960.0880.103
EI0.6420.1620.1950.2240.7810.741(−1.82; 2.20)(−1.84; 2.01)
PA0.5110.2731.7510.7140.2960.263(−0.55; 2.51)(−0.84; 1.88)
Instrumental Relational Aggression 0.2470.2670.0650.010
EI0.3860.2780.133−0.2420.6830.801(−1.78; 2.12)(−1.66; 2.03)
PA0.8910.8710.264−0.1030.8740.172(−1.33; 1.63)(−1.42; 1.52)
Note: p < 0.05; physical activity (PA); emotional intelligence (EI).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ubago-Jiménez, J.L.; Cepero-González, M.; Martínez-Martínez, A.; Chacón-Borrego, F. Linking Emotional Intelligence, Physical Activity and Aggression among Undergraduates. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 12477. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182312477

AMA Style

Ubago-Jiménez JL, Cepero-González M, Martínez-Martínez A, Chacón-Borrego F. Linking Emotional Intelligence, Physical Activity and Aggression among Undergraduates. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(23):12477. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182312477

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ubago-Jiménez, José Luis, Mar Cepero-González, Asunción Martínez-Martínez, and Fátima Chacón-Borrego. 2021. "Linking Emotional Intelligence, Physical Activity and Aggression among Undergraduates" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 23: 12477. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182312477

APA Style

Ubago-Jiménez, J. L., Cepero-González, M., Martínez-Martínez, A., & Chacón-Borrego, F. (2021). Linking Emotional Intelligence, Physical Activity and Aggression among Undergraduates. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(23), 12477. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182312477

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop