Survey Results on Using Nudges for Choice of Green-Energy Supplier
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Literature Review
2.1. Choice Architecture and Nudges
2.2. Green Nudges
3. Materials and Methods
3.1. Research Description
3.2. Survey Sample
3.3. Variables and Method of Analysis
- 1.
- Do you approve or disapprove of the following hypothetical policy?
- 2.
- Do you approve or disapprove of the following hypothetical policy?
- -
- How much are you concerned about the environment?
- -
- How much do you trust the following institutions?
4. Results
- in the first stage of early adulthood (25–34 years old),
- living in rural areas,
- with the lowest level of education,
- that have no children,
- whose monthly net income in the household was within the first income bracket or between PLN 5000 and PLN 7500,
- who voted in the last elections for the KON (a grouping of R-GAL parties) (To determine the position of the Polish parties was used Chapel Hill Expert Survey [94] which contains expert ratings of the parties’ positions in 31 countries (all European Union members, Norway, Switzerland and Turkey). The position of each party is considered in two dimensions. One is economic: the left–right spectrum. One is cultural: the GAL-TAN spectrum, which contrasts green, alternative and libertarian (some say postmodern) values with traditional, authoritarian and nationalistic ones. The position of the coalition in Poland was determined as the average of the positions of the parties included in the coalition.)
- with very conservative political views.
- during late adolescence (18–24 years) and the first stage of early adulthood (25–34 years),
- living in big cities,
- having a secondary education,
- having no children,
- whose monthly net income in the household was within the first income bracket and those with an income between PLN 5000 and PLN 7500,
- in the last election voting for DLW (a grouping of L-GAL parties),
- showing very liberal (left-wing) political beliefs show less support for both green defaults.
- in early adulthood (25–34 years),
- residing in rural areas,
- those with the lowest level of education,
- having no children,
- whose monthly net household income was within the first income bracket,
- who in the last election voted for the coalition PSLK (a grouping of R-GAL parties),
- with liberal political views.
5. Discussion
6. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Ghazali, E.M.; Nguyen, B.; Mutum, D.S.; Yap, S.-F. Pro-Environmental Behaviours and Value-Belief-Norm Theory: Assessing Unobserved Heterogeneity of Two Ethnic Groups. Sustainability 2019, 11, 3237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brosch, T. Behavioral Insights for Climate Action; GPSI Policy Brief: Genewa, Switzerland, 2020; Available online: https://gspi.ch/activities/behavioral-insights-for-climate-action/ (accessed on 25 January 2021).
- Bodansky, D. The Paris climate change agreement: A new hope? Am. J. Int. Law. 2016, 110, 288–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Low, M.; Bea, E.; Lu, S. Katowice Climate Package: Operationalising the Climate Change Regime in the Paris Agreement. ESI Policy Brief 2019, 27, 1–4. Available online: https://esi.nus.edu.sg/docs/default-source/esi-policy-briefs/katowice-climate-package.pdf?sfvrsn=2 (accessed on 11 January 2022).
- Limiting Member States’ Carbon Emissions. Briefing EU Legislation in Progress, 12 January 2022. Available online: https://www.europarl.europa.eu/RegData/etudes/BRIE/2016/589799/EPRS_BRI%282016%29589799_EN.pdf (accessed on 12 February 2022).
- Energy Policy of Poland until 2040; Ministry of Climate and Environment: Warsaw, Poland, 2021. Available online: https://www.gov.pl/web/klimat/polityka-energetyczna-polski (accessed on 6 February 2022).
- Kaiser, M.; Bernauer, M.; Sunstein, C.R.; Reisch, L.A. The power of green defaults: The impact of regional variation of opt-out tariffs on green energy demand in Germany. Ecol. Econ. 2020, 174, 106685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- EPA. Sources of Greenhouse Gas Emissions. Available online: https://www.epa.gov/ghgemissions/sources-greenhouse-gas-emissions (accessed on 8 February 2022).
- Lehmann, A.; Masò, J.; Nativi, S.; Giuliani, G. Towards integrated essential variables for sustainability. Int. J. Digit. Earth 2020, 13, 158–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knez, S.; Šimić, G.; Milovanović, A.; Starikova, S.; Županič, F. Prices of conventional and renewable Energy as determinants of sustainable and secure energy development: Regression model analysis, Energy. Sustain. Soc. 2022, 12, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seto, K.C.; Davis, S.J.; Mitchell, R.; Stokes, E.C.; Unruh, G.; Ürge-Vorsatz, D. Carbon Lock-In: Types, Causes, and Policy Implications. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 2016, 41, 425–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stephenson, J.; Sovacool, B.; Håkon, T.; Inderberg, J. Energy cultures and national decarbonisation pathways. Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 2020, 137, a110592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baur, D.; Emmerich, P.; Baumann, M.J.; Weil, M. Assessing the social acceptance of key technologies for the German energy transition. Energy Sustain. Soc. 2022, 12, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Niamir, L.; Ivanova, O.; Filatova, T. Economy-wide impacts of behavioral climate change mitigation: Linking agent-based and computable general equilibrium models. Environ. Model. Softw. 2020, 134, 104839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nielsen, K.S.; van der Linden, S.; Stern, P.C. How Behavioral Interventions Can Reduce the Climate Impact of Energy Use. Joule 2020, 4, 1613–1620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nielsen, K.S.; Clayton, S.; Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T.; Capstick, S.; Whitmarsh, L. How psychology can help limit climate change. Am. Psychol. 2021, 76, 130–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Słupik, S.; Kos-Łabędowicz, J.; Trzęsiok, J. How to Encourage Energy Savings Behaviours? The Most Effective Incentives from the Perspective of European Consumers. Energies 2021, 14, 8009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stankuniene, G.; Streimikiene, D.; Kyriakopoulos, G.L. Systematic Literature Review on Behavioral Barriers of Climate Change Mitigation in Households. Sustainability 2020, 12, 7369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sunstein, C.R.; Reisch, L.A. Automatically green: Behavioral economics and environmental protection. Harv. Environ. Law Rev. 2014, 38, 127–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jakučionyte-Skodienė, M.; Dagiliūtė, R.; Liobikienė, G. Do general pro-environmental behaviour, attitude, and knowledge contribute to energy savings and climate change mitigation in the residential sector? Energy 2020, 193, 116784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Njoku, H.O.; Omeke, O.M. Potentials and financial viability of solar photovoltaic power generation in Nigeria for greenhouse gas emissions mitigation. Clean Technol Env. Policy 2020, 22, 481–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van der Linden, S.; Pearson, A.; Van Boven, L. Behavioural climate policy. Behav. Public Policy 2021, 5, 430–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kollmuss, A.; Agyeman, J. Mind the Gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 2010, 8, 239–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- John, P. How Far to Nudge? Assessing Behavioral Public Policy; Edward Elgar: Cheltenham, UK, 2018; p. 22. [Google Scholar]
- Reynoldsa, J.P.; Stautz, K.; Piling, M.; van der Linden, S.; Marteau, T.M. Communicating the effectiveness and ineffectiveness of government policies and their impact on public support: A systematic review with meta-analysis. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2020, 7, 190522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van der Linden, S. The social-psychological determinants of climate change risk perceptions, attitudes, and behaviours: A national study. Environ. Educ. Res. 2016, 22, 434–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tummers, L. Public Policy and Behavior Change. Public Adm. Rev. 2019, 79, 925–930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palm, R.; Bolsen, T.; Kingsland, J.T. “Don’t Tell Me What to Do”: Resistance to Climate Change Messages Suggesting Behavior Changes. Weather Clim. Soc. 2020, 12, 827–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sunstein, C.R. Behavioural economics, consumption and environmental protection. In Handbook of Research on Sustainable Consumption; Reisch, L.A., Thøgersen, J., Eds.; Edward Elgar Publishing: Cheltenham, UK, 2015; pp. 313–327. [Google Scholar]
- Santos Silva, M. Nudging and Other Behaviourally Based Policies as Enablers for Environmental Sustainability. Laws 2022, 11, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thaler, R.H.; Sunstein, C.R. Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- John PBehavioral Approaches: How Nudges Lead to More Intelligent Policy Design. In Contemporary Approaches to Public Policy. Theories, Controverses and Perspectives; Peters, G.B.; Zittoun, P. (Eds.) Palgrave: Macmillan, UK, 2016; pp. 113–132. [Google Scholar]
- Sunstein, C.R. Nudging: A Very Short Guide. Bus. Econ. 2019, 54, 127–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sunstein, C.R.; Reisch, L.A.; Kaiser, M. Trusting nudges? Lessons from an international survey. J. Eur. Public Policy 2019, 26, 1417–1443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sunstein, C.R.; Reisch, L.A.; Rauber, J. A worldwide consensus on nudging? Not quite, but almost. Regul. Gov. 2018, 12, 3–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sunstein, C.R.; Reisch, L.A. Trusting Nudges: Toward A Bill of Rights for Nudging, Routledge, Now York. Routledge Adv. Behav. Econ. Financ. 2019, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Khadzhyradieva, S.; Hrechko, T.; Savkov, A. Behavioral Insights in Public Policy: Ukrainian Case. Public Policy Adm. 2019, 18, 85–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cialdini, R.B.; Martin, S.J.; Goldstein, N.J. Small behavioral science–informed changes can produce large policy-relevant effects. Behav. Sci. Policy 2015, 1, 21–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stern, P.C. A reexamination on how behavioral interventions can promote household action to limit climate change. Nat. Commun. 2020, 11, 918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hertwig, R.; Grüne-Yanoff, T. Nudging and boosting: Steering or empowering good decisions. Perspect. Psych. Sci. 2017, 12, 973–986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Thaler, R.; Sunstein, C.R.; Baltz, J.P. Choice Architecture. In The Behavioral Foundations of Public Policy; Shafir, E., Ed.; Princeton University Press: Princeton, UK; Oxford, UK, 2012; pp. 428–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow; Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Halpern, D. Inside the Nudge Unit. How Small Changes Can Make a Big Difference; WH Allen: London, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Beshears, J.; Kosowsky, H. Nudging: Progress to date and future directions. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2020, 161, 3–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ghesla, C.; Grieder, M.; Schubert, R. Nudging the poor and the rich—A field study on the distributional effects of green electricity defaults. Energy Econ. 2020, 86, 104616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- John, P.; Stoker, G. Rethinking the Role of Experts and Expertise in Behavioural Public Policy. Policy Polit. 2019, 47, 209–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sunstein, C.R. Nudging: A Very Short Guide. J. Consum. Policy 2014, 37, 583–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Munscher, R.; Vetter, M.; Scheuerle, T. A Review and Taxonomy of Choice Architecture Techniques. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2016, 29, 511–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Osman, M.; Fenton, N.; Pilditch, T.; Lagnado, D.; Neil, M. Whom do we trust on social policy interventions? Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 40, 249–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hausman, D.; Welch, B. Debate: To Nudge or Not to Nudge. J. Political Philos. 2010, 18, 123–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grüne-Yanoff, T. Old wine in new casks: Libertarian paternalism still violates liberal principles. Soc. Choice Welf. 2012, 38, 635–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rebonato, R. A Critical Assessment of Libertarian Paternalism. J. Consum. Policy 2014, 37, 357–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, M.D. The Manipulation of Choice: Ethics and Libertarian Paternalism; Palgrave Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Heilmann, C. Success conditions for nudges: A methodological critique of libertarian paternalism. Eur. J. Philos. Sci. 2014, 4, 75–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vallier, K. On the inevitability of nudging. Georget. J. Law Public Policy 2016, 14, 817–828. [Google Scholar]
- MacKay, D.; Robinson, A. The ethics of organ donor registration policies: Nudges and respect for autonomy. Am. J. Bioeth. 2016, 16, 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mills, C. The heteronomy of choice architecture. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 2015, 6, 495–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nys, T.R.; Engelen, B. Judging nudging: Answering the manipulation objection. Polit. Stud. 2017, 65, 199–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schubert, C. Green nudges: Do they work? Are they ethical? Ecol. Econ. 2017, 132, 329–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Engelen, B.; Nys, T. Nudging and Autonomy: Analyzing and Alleviating the Worries. Rev. Phil. Psych. 2020, 11, 137–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmidt, A.T.; Engelen, B. The ethics of nudging: An overview. Philos. Compass 2020, 15, e12658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carlsson, F.; Gravert, C.A.; Kurz, V.; Johansson-Stenman, O. Nudging as an Environmental Policy Instrument. Rev. Environ. Econ. Policy 2021, 15, 216–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nagatsu, M. Social Nudges: Their Mechanisms and Justification. Rev. Phil. Psych. 2015, 6, 481–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wensing, J.; Caputo, V.; Carraresi, L.; Bröring, S. The effects of green nudges on consumer valuation of bio-based plastic packaging. Ecol. Econ. 2020, 178, 106783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sunstein, C.R. Green defaults can combat climate change. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021, 5, 548–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nielsen, A.S.E.; Sand, H.; Sørensen, P.; Knutsson, M.; Martinsson, P.; Persson, E.; Wollbrant, C. Nudging and Pro-Environmental Behaviour. 2016. Available online: https://norden.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1065958/FULLTEXT01.pdf (accessed on 23 January 2022).
- Jachimowicz, J.M.; Duncan, S.; Weber, E.U.; Johnson, E.J. When and why defaults influence decisions: A meta-analysis of defaults effects. Behav. Public Policy 2019, 3, 159–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willis, L.E. When nudges fail: Slippery defaults. Univ. Chic. Law Rev. 2013, 80, 1155–1230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sunstein, C.R.; Reisch, L.A. Green by default. Kyklos 2013, 66, 398–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sintov, N.D.; Schultz, P.W. Adjustable Green Defaults Can Help Make Smart Homes More Sustainable. Sustainability 2017, 9, 622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Michaelsen, P.; Johansson, L.-O.; Hedesström, M. Experiencing default nudges: Autonomy, manipulation, and choice-satisfaction as judged by people themselves. Behav. Public Policy 2021, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ebeling, F.; Lotz, S. Domestic uptake of green energy promoted by opt-out tariffs. Nat. Clim. Change 2015, 5, 868–871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McKenzie, C.R.; Liersch, M.J.; Finkelstein, S.R. Recommendations implicit in policy defaults. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 414–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tankard, M.E.; Paluck, E.L. Norm perception as a vehicle for social change. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2016, 10, 181–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa, D.L.; Kahn, M.E. Energy conservation “nudges” and environmentalist ideology: Evidence from a randomized residential electricity field experiment. J. Eur. Econ. Assoc. 2013, 11, 680–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farrow, K.; Grolleau, G.; Ibanez, L. Social norms and pro-environmental behavior: A review of the evidence. Ecol. Econ. 2017, 140, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loschelder, D.D.; Siepelmeyer, H.; Fischer, D.; Rubel, J.A. Dynamic norms drive sustainable consumption: Norm-based nudging helps café customers to avoid disposable to-go-cups. J. Econ. Psychol. 2019, 75, 102146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zorell, C.V. Nudges, Norms, or Just Contagion? A Theory on Influences on the Practice of (Non-)Sustainable Behavior. Sustainability 2020, 12, 10418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hale, L.A. At Home with Sustainability: From Green Default Rules to Sustainable Consumption. Sustainability 2018, 10, 249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pichert, D.; Katsikopoulos, K.V. Green defaults: Information presentation and proenvironmental behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 2008, 28, 63–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Momsen, K.; Stoerk, T. From intention to action: Can nudges help consumers to choose renewable energy? Energy Policy 2014, 74, 376–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hedlin, S.; Sunstein, C.R. Does active choosing promote green energy use? Experimental evidence. Ecol. Law Q. 2016, 43, 107–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ghesla, C. Behavioral Economics and Public Policy: The Case of Green Electricity Defaults. Ph.D. Thesis, ETH, Zurich, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Ghesla, C. Defaults in green electricity markets: Preference match not guaranteed. J. Assoc. Environ. Resour. Econ. 2017, 4, 37–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liebe, U.; Gewinner, J.; Diekmann, A. Large and persistent effects of green energy defaults in the household and business sectors. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021, 5, 576–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sunstein, C.R. Do people like nudges? Admin. L. Rev. 2016, 68, 177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reisch, L.A.; Sunstein, C.R. Do Europeans like nudges? Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2016, 11, 310–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- GUS. Mały Rocznik Statystyczny. 2020. Available online: www.stat.gov.pl (accessed on 19 February 2022).
- Manstead, A.S.R. The psychology of social class: How socioeconomic status impacts thought, feeling and behaviour. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 57, 267–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Han, H.; Hwang, J.; Lee, M.J. The value-belief-emotion-norm model: Investigating customers’ eco-friendly behaviour. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 2016, 34, 590–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dursun, I.; Kabadayi, E.T.; Tuger, A.T. Application of Value-Belief-Norm Theory to Responsible Post Consumption Behaviour: Recycling and Reuse. In Proceedings of the International Congress of the New Approaches and Technologies for Sustainable Development, Isparta, Turkey, 21–24 September 2017; Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/320087119_Application_of_Value-Belief-Norm_Theory_to_Responsible_Post_Consumption_Behaviors_Recycling_and_Reuse (accessed on 1 March 2022).
- Milfont, T.; Duckitt, J. The environmental attitudes inventory: A valid and reliable measure to assess the structure of environmental attitudes. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 80–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lahno, B. Institutional Trust: A Less Demanding Form of Trust? Rev. Latinoam. De Estud. Av. 2001, 15, 19–58. [Google Scholar]
- Jolly, S.; Bakker, R.; Hooghe, L.; Marks, G.; Polk, J.; Rovny, J.; Steenbergen, M.; Vachudova, M.A. Chapel Hill Expert Survey Trend File, 1999–2019. Elect. Stud. 2021, 75, 102420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- 2021–2022 EIB Climate Survey. Tackling the Climate Crisis during the Post-Pandemic Recovery. 2022. Available online: https://www.eib.org/en/surveys/climate-survey/4th-climate-survey/index.htm (accessed on 15 January 2022).
- Jakiej Energii chcą Polacy. PSEW Listopada 2019. Available online: http://psew.pl/jakiej-energii-chca-polacy-badania-opinii-spolecznej-indicator/ (accessed on 25 February 2022).
- Badania Opinii Polaków na Temat Różnych Zródeł Energii. Stowarzyszenie Indikator. 2020. Available online: https://stowarzyszeniepv.pl/2020/05/10/badanie-opinii-polakow-na-temat-roznych-zrodel-energii/ (accessed on 28 February 2022).
- Almqvist, G.; Andersson, P. Low support for nudging among Swedes in a population-representative sample. Behav. Public Policy 2021, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zaufanie Społeczne. Komunikat z Badań. 2020. Available online: https://www.cbos.pl/SPISKOM.POL/2020/K_043_20.PDF (accessed on 28 February 2022).
- Drożdż, W.; Mróz-Malik, O.; Kopiczko, M. The Future of the Polish Energy Mix in the Context of Social Expectations. Energies 2021, 14, 5341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fischhoff, B. Making behavioral science integral to climate science and action. Behav. Public Policy 2021, 5, 439–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hertwig, R. When to consider boosting: Some rules for policy-makers. Behav. Public Policy 2017, 1, 143–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Niamir, L.; Kiesewetter, G.; Wagner, F.; Schöpp, W.; Filatova, T.; Voinov, A.; Bressers, H. Assessing the macroeconomic impacts of individual behavioral changes on carbon emissions. Clim. Change 2020, 158, 141–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Choi, H.; Jang, J.; Kandampully, J. Application of the extended VBN theory to understand consumers’ decisions about green hotels. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2015, 51, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kumar, B.; Manrai, A.K.; Manrai, L.A. Purchasing behaviour for environmentally sustainable products: A conceptual framework and empirical study. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2017, 34, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cilo, T.S. Psychological and Social Factors Predicting Pro-Environmental Behaviour in The South African Context. 2020. Available online: https://uir.unisa.ac.za/bitstream/handle/10500/27708/dissertation_cilo_ts.pdf?sequence=3 (accessed on 19 January 2022).
Study | Kinds/Types | Description |
---|---|---|
[62] | pure nudges/ defaults, provision and simplification of information, changes to the physical environment and reminders moral nudges/ inter-personal motivations and social comparisons, moral suasion, and goal setting and commitment | lead to “do the right thing”; in order to nudge people into pro-social behaviour that may be in their own interest, use their cognitive limitations but do not assume that in their decisions they make errors; usually work through rearranging the existing choice environment; they reward “doing the right thing” with psychological utility; they intentionally trigger psychological reactions such as fun, fear, shame or pride; they use e.g., social proof, comparisons with others; lead to conscious psychological reactions of people who are nudged and compared to pure nudges they are more prone to boomerang effect or backlash; usually stay noticed because they may not be adjusted to the preferences of the individual or because the individual does not want to be nudged |
[30,59] | appeal to people’s self-image or self-identity as “pro-environmental consumers” or harness people’s private sense of “social identity” appeal to social conformism involve the modification of defaults | people may be nudged to be more pro-ecological by simplifying the way the information on a product’s characteristics is provided; increasing the salience of certain futures (for example by using eco-labels) can simplify the way of communicating information about selected product features and increase awareness among consumers they are based on the tendency of people to imitate their peers (“follow the herd” tendency); sometimes they convey specific norms by comparison; other work by stimulating people to compete for social status, for example, nudging people to demonstrate ecological behavior to others carefully setting the default value is considered a very effective nudge because it seizes two biases: the force of inertia (abandoning the default option requires commitment and reflection) and suggestion (the default rules take into account an informational signal that can trick people into complying with the rules). |
[71] | provision of information changes in the physical environment green default social norms and regular feedback | the use of available but reliable information leads to less complexity of choice, the most popular type of green support is often combined with changes to the default option; in this case it is very important to provide real-time information that can be used as a nudge targeting energy consumption or energy efficiency, and provides transparency about current energy are particularly suitable for motivating behaviors related to waste reduction, sorting, management and recycling, as well as reducing food waste, e.g., reducing the size of the dishes used in hotels and restaurants or using innovative sorting devices to improve waste segregation a very perspective type of nudge, used successfully primarily to motivate to reduce energy consumption and to encourage energy-saving behavior; social norms through peer comparisons are used to reduce energy and water consumption and to encourage waste sorting; providing information in the form of social feedback (on current energy consumption patterns) combined with frequent information can lead to a reduction in energy consumption; linking feedback reports and peer comparisons becomes more effective; |
Study | Outcome |
---|---|
[80] | Using the case study method, the research was conducted in three countries. Significant effects of the default options were found, including green energy in the default contract increasing the number of people using this type of energy. |
[81] | Results presented for an original survey experiment on which nudges affect the choice of contract for renewable energy or conventional energy. Defaults seem to have worked as expected and, in comparison to the other analyzed nudges (mental accounting, priming, decoy, framing, social norms), had a significant impact; choice of green energy increased by 44.6%. |
[72] | The results of a randomized controlled trial in Germany indicate that consumers are more likely to buy ‘green’ energy contracts (nearly tenfold) despite the higher price of this energy, if opting out requires them to be proactive in rejecting the default option and it seems this effect is not the result of unawareness. |
[82] | The results of the online experiment indicate that (when green energy cost extra) active choosing had larger effects in promoting green energy use (82% of the participants) than did green defaults (76% stayed with the default) due to the interaction between people’s feelings of guilt and reactance. |
[83] | The analyses based on experimental and empirical approaches and their results show that the alignment of default intentions and ‘pure’ individual preferences are influenced by the relative price of green electricity; green defaults have diverging costs and benefits for different groups in society, therefore significant impact on consumer welfare and well-intentioned default choice, stimulating initial “good” behavior, does not interfere with subsequent “other” individual choices. |
[84] | Using two design elements from the existing electricity markets and laboratory experiment, it was investigated whether the preferences of electricity consumers are the same in the conditions of no default option and in the conditions of its occurrence, and it was found that green defaults at a low price premium did not match subjects’ preferences while those with higher premiums were found to be a better fit. |
[44] | As a result of a field study in the residential electricity market, it was found that the green default settings are effective, but when set at a relatively cheap level they do not correspond to consumer’s preferences and have distributional effects. They make poorer households pay more than they would like to in order to use green electricity, and they do not increase willingness of the richer households to pay for it. |
[7] | Using macroeconomic and microeconomic data, based on real (not experimental) evidence, it was found in Germany that green defaults that automatically include consumers in green energy sources persist, especially among consumers who are concerned about unfavorable climate change. |
[85] | Two large-scale field studies conducted in Sweden have shown that green energy defaults are effective in influencing both businesses and households. The presentation of green energy as a standard option has meant that around 80% of customers (from both groups) have stayed with green energy for at least four years. Acceptance of green defaults was slightly more frequent among women than among men participating in the research. |
Characteristic | Frequency | Percentage | |
---|---|---|---|
Gender (Gen) | Male | 558 | 52.4 |
Female | 506 | 47.6 | |
Age (years) (Age) | 18–24 25–34 35–44 45–54 55 and more | 145 215 169 195 340 | 13.6 20.2 3.0 18.3 32.0 |
Education (Edu) | Primary/vocational school (P/VS) Secondary school (SS) Higher education (HE) | 213 445 406 | 20.0 38.2 37.6 |
Size of place of residence 1 (PoRes) | Village Small town Medium town Big town Very big town | 400 137 209 189 129 | 37.6 12.8 19.6 17.8 12.2 |
Net monthly income (NIn) (in PLN) 2 | <1000 1001–2000 2001–3000 3001–4000 4001–5000 5001–7500 7501–10,000 >10,000 refusal to answer | 42 93 159 146 145 144 73 31 233 | 5.0 11.2 19.1 17.6 17.5 17.4 8.8 3.7 21.8 |
Children (Child) | Yes No | 692 373 | 65.0 35.0 |
Political preferences (PPref) | Law and Justice Party + United Poland + Agreement (PiS) | 307 | 28.9 |
Civic Coalition (Civic Platform + Modern + Polish Initiative + The Greens) (PO) | 274 | 25.8 | |
Democratic Left Alliance + Spring + Left Together (DLW) | 132 | 12.4 | |
Polish People’s Party + Kukiz 15 (PSLK) | 82 | 7.7 | |
Confederation Liberty and Independence (KON) | 67 | 6.3 | |
Other | 34 | 3.2 | |
Political beliefs (PBel) | Very conservative (right wing) (VCon) Conservative (Con) Rather conservative (RCon) It’s hard to say, a bit conservative and a bit liberal (HtoS) Rather liberal (RLib) Liberal (Lib) Very liberal (VLib) Refusal to answer | 56 97 84 446 105 136 66 74 | 5.7 9.8 8.5 45.1 10.6 13.7 6.7 7.0 |
Variables | DF1 | DF2 | CoEnv | InTru | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Approve | p-Value | Hypotheses | Approve | p-Value | Hypotheses | M | SD | M | SD | ||
Sample | 69% | - | - | 66% | - | - | 5.1 | 1.42 | 2.5 | 1.67 | |
Gen | Male | 71% | 0.16684 | H0 | 67% | 0.41030 | H0 | 5.4 | 1.29 | 2.4 | 1.67 |
Female | 68% | 65% | 4.9 | 1.46 | 2.6 | 1.67 | |||||
Age | 18–24 | 73% | 0.27206 | H0 | 66% | 0.0002 | H1 | 4.8 | 1.34 | 2.4 | 1.55 |
25–34 | 63% | 58% | 4.7 | 1.38 | 2.4 | 1.58 | |||||
35–44 | 69% | 61% | 5.0 | 1.41 | 2.5 | 1.68 | |||||
45–54 | 72% | 62% | 5.2 | 1.46 | 2.5 | 1.60 | |||||
55 and more | 70% | 77% | 5.5 | 1.27 | 2.5 | 1.81 | |||||
Edu | P/VS | 65% | 0.19348 | H0 | 60% | 0.18120 | H0 | 4.8 | 1.49 | 2.6 | 1.64 |
SS | 68% | 66% | 5.2 | 1.40 | 2.5 | 1.71 | |||||
HE | 72% | 68% | 5.1 | 1.35 | 2.6 | 1.66 | |||||
PoRes | Village | 65% | 0.36353 | H0 | 62% | 0.04682 | H1 | 5.0 | 1.38 | 2.3 | 1.75 |
Small town | 71% | 61% | 5.1 | 1.39 | 2.2 | 1.58 | |||||
Medium town | 70% | 70% | 5.2 | 1.42 | 2.7 | 1.65 | |||||
Big town | 72% | 69% | 5.2 | 1.33 | 2.5 | 1.72 | |||||
Very big town | 72% | 72% | 5.3 | 1.47 | 2.3 | 1.62 | |||||
NInc | <1000 | 58% | 0.29267 | H0 | 61% | 0.06213 | H0 | 5.0 | 1.46 | 2.2 | 1.61 |
1001–2000 | 65% | 67% | 5.2 | 1.30 | 2.5 | 1.70 | |||||
2001–3000 | 69% | 73% | 5.3 | 1.34 | 2.3 | 1.68 | |||||
3001–4000 | 70% | 66% | 5.1 | 1.40 | 2.6 | 1.68 | |||||
4001–5000 | 78% | 75% | 5.2 | 1.29 | 2.7 | 1.79 | |||||
5001–7500 | 69% | 62% | 5.0 | 1.47 | 2.2 | 1.55 | |||||
7501–10,000 | 66% | 66% | 5.3 | 1.67 | 2.7 | 1.79 | |||||
>10,000 | 65% | 68% | 5.6 | 1.35 | 2.4 | 1.67 | |||||
refusal to answer | 69% | 59% | 5.0 | 1.36 | 2.5 | 1.59 | |||||
Child | Yes | 70% | 0.33552 | H0 | 68% | 0.06822 | H0 | 5.6 | 1.37 | 2.5 | 1.70 |
No | 67% | 63% | 5.5 | 1.41 | 2.4 | 1.60 |
Variables | DF1 | DF2 | CoEnv | InTru | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Approve | p-Value | Hypotheses | Approve | p-Value | Hypotheses | M | SD | M | SD | ||
Sample | 69% | - | 66% | 5.1 | 1.42 | 2.5 | 1.67 | ||||
PPref | PiS | 71% | 0.9003 | H0 | 69% | 0.00183 | H1 | 5.1 | 1.39 | 3.9 | 1.74 |
PO | 73% | 69% | 5.4 | 1.42 | 1.7 | 1.12 | |||||
DLW | 70% | 74% | 5.4 | 1.24 | 1.7 | 1.09 | |||||
PSLK | 66% | 55% | 5.2 | 1.31 | 2.1 | 1.33 | |||||
KON | 63% | 57% | 4.5 | 1.34 | 1.9 | 1.35 | |||||
Other | 68% | 53% | 5.2 | 1.23 | 2.2 | 1.45 | |||||
PBel | VCon | 77% | 0.80374 | H0 | 63% | 0.46885 | H0 | 4.8 | 1.59 | 3.7 | 2.25 |
Con | 74% | 72% | 4.9 | 1.35 | 3.8 | 1.84 | |||||
RCon | 69% | 70% | 4.8 | 1.44 | 3.3 | 1.77 | |||||
HtoS | 67% | 66% | 5.1 | 1.38 | 2.5 | 1.54 | |||||
RLib | 67% | 60% | 5.3 | 1.42 | 1.8 | 1.15 | |||||
Lib | 71% | 70% | 5.5 | 1.27 | 1.6 | 1.15 | |||||
VLib | 68% | 67% | 5.5 | 1.22 | 1.5 | 1.04 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Miłaszewicz, D. Survey Results on Using Nudges for Choice of Green-Energy Supplier. Energies 2022, 15, 2679. https://doi.org/10.3390/en15072679
Miłaszewicz D. Survey Results on Using Nudges for Choice of Green-Energy Supplier. Energies. 2022; 15(7):2679. https://doi.org/10.3390/en15072679
Chicago/Turabian StyleMiłaszewicz, Danuta. 2022. "Survey Results on Using Nudges for Choice of Green-Energy Supplier" Energies 15, no. 7: 2679. https://doi.org/10.3390/en15072679
APA StyleMiłaszewicz, D. (2022). Survey Results on Using Nudges for Choice of Green-Energy Supplier. Energies, 15(7), 2679. https://doi.org/10.3390/en15072679