Next Article in Journal
Hydration Habits and Water Balance in a Working Population in Greece
Previous Article in Journal
Maternal Vitamin D Deficiency Is a Risk Factor for Infants’ Epigenetic Gestational Age Acceleration at Birth in Japan: A Cohort Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Prevalence of and Factors Associated with Disordered Eating Among Adult Athletes in Italy and Lebanon
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Tying Food Addiction to Uncontrolled Eating: The Roles of Eating-Related Thoughts and Emotional Eating

by
Alessandro Alberto Rossi
1,2
1
Department of Philosophy, Sociology, Education, and Applied Psychology, Section of Applied Psychology, University of Padova, 35131 Padova, Italy
2
Center for Intervention and Research on Family Studies—CIRF, Department of Philosophy, Sociology, Education, and Applied Psychology, Section of Applied Psychology, University of Padova, 35131 Padova, Italy
Nutrients 2025, 17(3), 369; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17030369
Submission received: 5 January 2025 / Revised: 13 January 2025 / Accepted: 17 January 2025 / Published: 21 January 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Nutrition, Disordered Eating and Mental Health)

Highlights

  1. Cognitive and emotional mechanisms in food addiction: Symptoms of food addiction lead to overwhelming eating-related thoughts which in turn lead to emotional eating, indicating that cognitive and emotional factors are of paramount importance in the development of uncontrolled eating behaviors.
  2. Mediational insights: A structural equation model highlights that food addiction symptoms predict uncontrolled eating through a sequential mediation of eating-related thoughts and emotional eating, explaining 61.6% of the variance in food-intake eating behaviors.
  3. Clinical implications: These findings underline the importance of addressing dysfunctional thoughts and emotional regulation in therapeutic interventions for individuals showing symptoms of food addiction and eating-related disorders.

Abstract

:
Background. Food addiction is often linked to overeating and difficulty in controlling eating habits. At the same time, food addiction is often associated with intense eating-related thoughts and emotional eating behaviors. However, despite extensive research on food addiction, the psychological processes that contribute to these outcomes have not been fully examined. Consequently, this study aims to fill that gap by investigating the influence of eating-related thoughts, as well as emotional eating behaviors that may precede episodes of uncontrolled eating. Methods. A cross-sectional design was used. A sample of 467 individuals was enrolled from the general population. Participants completed a battery of self-report questionnaires. A sequential mediation analysis with latent variables (i.e., structural equation modeling; SEM) using 5000 bootstrap samples and observed variables was performed. Results. The proposed model provides good fit indices. Indeed, food addiction predicts uncontrolled eating behaviors through eating-related thoughts (p < 0.001), which were also significantly associated with the emotion-driven eating patterns (p < 0.001), revealing a fully mediated model explaining 61.6% of the outcome variance (R2 = 0.616). Discussion. The findings underscore the critical influence of cognitive factors (i.e., eating-related thoughts) in driving maladaptive coping mechanisms like emotional eating. Moreover, emotional eating may act as a precursor to behaviors associated with overeating, which are often rooted in food addiction. Conclusions. Recognizing the central role of thoughts and emotions can help clinicians develop more targeted psychological interventions for those experiencing food addiction symptoms.

1. Introduction

Obesity has become a global epidemic and is increasingly prevalent in the developed world, with estimates that more than 85% of adults will be diagnosed as being overweight/obese when using the correct criteria for overweight and obesity over the next few decades [1,2]. Obesity is a chronic clinical condition influenced by medical, psychological, and environmental factors that contribute to its development and maintenance [3,4]. One potentially crucial aspect implicated in its onset and persistence is the idea that certain foods have addictive properties [5,6], especially those high in sugar, fat, and salt [7,8,9]. The inherent addictive properties of these food products—namely, highly processed food (HPF) [7,8,10]—which are palatable and (thus) psychologically rewarding, may elicit cravings and withdrawal-like symptoms that may trigger and maintain a dependency response in some individuals [10,11,12,13,14], namely, food addiction (FA).
FA is a complex construct that can be conceptualized as a set of characteristics related to eating disorders (EDs) and substance use disorders (SUDs) [15,16,17,18] which mutually reinforce each other in a vicious cycle, leading the individual to become addicted to food. It should be noted that FA can manifest in several psychological conditions related to EDs [19,20], such as anorexia nervosa (AN), bulimia nervosa (BN), and binge eating disorder (BED), which involves recurrent episodes of uncontrollable eating, often resulting in discomfort or distress [21,22]. In fact, one of the most iconic characteristics of FA is its association with behavioral manifestations related to food consumption, which can range from constant nibbling behaviors (grazing) [23] to binge eating episodes and compulsive eating [24,25]. It thus emerges that FA primarily—although not exclusively [23,26,27]—manifests through uncontrolled food consumption.
These manifestations are often driven and sustained by extremely complex psychological components that are associated with SUDs. Indeed, besides that, HPFs can stimulate neural reward pathways [28,29]; the regular consumption of these foods can result in the development of tolerance, meaning that individuals may need to consume larger quantities of substance (i.e., food) to experience the same level of satisfaction [30]. Also, when the consumption of these HPFs goes unmet, withdrawal symptoms may emerge [31,32], prompting individuals to pursue the same sensations of pleasure and well-being [19,23]. This compulsive urge to consume HPFs may assume the shape of craving symptoms.
As a result, while individuals grapple with FA-related symptoms—such as tolerance, withdrawal, and cravings—these can lead to the development of (intrusive) thoughts specifically tied to food consumption, which influence their behavior, often leading to excessive food intake (e.g., grazing, overeating, and binge eating patterns) [30,33,34]. In fact, symptoms like withdrawal or craving often lead individuals to spend significant amounts of time thinking about the substance, such as HPFs, and these persistent eating-related thoughts may trigger uncontrolled eating behaviors [15,34,35]. Consequently, FA may lead to uncontrolled eating behaviors in response to (e.g., mediated by) these eating-related thoughts and mental states.
Furthermore, as a result of these FA-triggered thoughts, individuals may experience other kinds of negative feelings such as anger, anxiety, preoccupations, worries, and feelings of lack of control that may lead to compulsive overeating and binge/uncontrolled eating behaviors [15,36,37]. Consequently, the urgency to cope with these negative affective states could lead individuals to feel an ‘overwhelming impulse to eat’ [36,38], namely, emotional eating. Indeed, individuals with FA seem to use emotional eating as a coping strategy for psychological distress [36,39,40], suggesting that FA may lead to emotional eating behaviors.
Additionally, this dysfunctional coping strategy for emotion regulation does not seem to fully inhibit the negative and/or distressing emotions for the individual [41,42,43]. As a result, the individual often resorts to more intense and extreme solutions, leading to binge eating behaviors [43,44,45,46,47].
Summarizing, it appears that developing or experiencing symptoms related to food addiction first leads the individual to have thoughts about food consumption [23,30]. These thoughts elicit emotional responses that, in turn, drive emotional eating behaviors as a way to manage them [40,41]. However, even this coping strategy (i.e., dysfunctional coping strategy) appears insufficient to stop the negative and distressing thoughts and emotions, eventually resulting in more intense behaviors such as uncontrolled eating [46,47].
However, to date, although the scientific literature suggests a link between FA symptoms and uncontrolled eating behavior, the path described above does not yet appear to have been fully studied. Consequently, the purpose of the present study was to examine the mediating effect of eating-related thoughts and emotional eating on the relationship between symptoms of FA and uncontrolled eating in a community sample. Indeed, although the link between the previously described variables appears to be fully coherent and dynamically reasonable, it does not seem to have received a clear empirical validation yet; therefore, the objective of this study is to clearly test these connections.
Based on the above-mentioned scientific literature, it was expected that individuals who recall more symptoms of FA would report higher eating-related thoughts, which lead to greater emotional eating and to greater uncontrolled eating. Explicit hypotheses about each path (relationship) between variables were formulated as follows:
H1. 
Symptoms of FA, eating-related thoughts, emotional eating, and uncontrolled eating are positively associated with each other;
H2. 
Symptoms of FA predict uncontrolled eating via eating-related thoughts and emotional eating;
H3. 
Symptoms of FA directly predict uncontrolled eating.
In other words, it was hypothesized that the positive associations between symptoms of FA on uncontrolled eating behaviors would be partially mediated by the presence of binge eating thoughts and emotional eating.

2. Methods and Materials

2.1. Procedure

According to previous studies, the snowball sampling technique [48] was used to recruit participants from the general population via social media platforms such as Facebook, Twitter/X, or Instagram [49]. To begin, participants were required to click a link that led to a Qualtrics survey. The recruitment materials provided comprehensive information regarding eligibility requirements and other relevant details to help participants make informed decisions, including guarantees of anonymity for their responses. Participants who consented to take part completed the survey online. It was specified that no financial compensation was offered for completing the survey; therefore, only voluntary participants were enrolled.
The criteria for inclusion were as follows: (A) participants should be 18 years or older; (B) they should be native Italian speakers; (C) they need to provide complete responses; and (D) they must sign an online informed consent form. This study received approval from the Ethics Committee of the University of Padua (protocol n° 3558).

2.2. Sample Size Determination

The sample size was predetermined based on the primary statistical analysis, specifically using the “n:q criterion”, which refers to the ratio of participants (n) to model parameters (q) [50]. According to established guidelines, a minimum of 10 participants per parameter was required to ensure adequate statistical power for the proposed model. Given that the model tested had 45 parameters, this necessitated a minimum enrollment of 450 participants to meet this criterion.

2.3. Participants

A sample of 551 participants started the survey; however, 84 of them did not complete it, and were therefore excluded (inclusion criterion C). A total of 467 participants were recruited. The sample comprised 125 males (26.8%) and 342 females (73.2%), with ages ranging from 18 to 71 years (mean = 31.25, SD = 13.52) and BMIs ranging from 15.62 to 40.66 (mean = 23.92; SD = 5.38). Additional details can be found in Table 1.

2.4. Measures

The demographic information gathered included age, gender, education level, marital status, and employment status. Additionally, participants were requested to self-report their height and weight to facilitate the calculation of their Body Mass Index (BMI), as well as to indicate the presence of a diagnosis of an eating disorder [51].

2.4.1. The Modified Yale Food Addiction Scale 2 (mYFAS 2)

The mYFAS 2.0 [52] is a self-assessment instrument aimed at assessing the frequency of food addiction symptoms within the last year. It is composed of 13 items rated on an 8-point Likert-type scale. Among these items, 11 align with the DSM-5 criteria for substance use disorder (SUD), such as tolerance, withdrawal, and craving. The remaining two focus on food-related psychological and emotional distress experienced by the individual. To establish a diagnosis of food addiction (FA), two scoring methods are utilized: the symptom count score, which tallies the number of diagnostic criteria met, and the diagnostic score, which evaluates the degree of impairment or emotional distress, categorizing it as No FA, mild FA, moderate FA, or severe FA. In the present study, the symptom count score was used, which higher scores indicate a greater amount of symptoms endorsed. In this study, the Italian version of the mYFAS 2.0 was used [53], showing good internal consistency values with a McDonald’s omega for categorical data of 0.832.

2.4.2. The Binge Eating Scale (BES)

The BES [35] is a self-report tool designed to assess the severity of feelings, cognitions, and behaviors related to binge eating. It consists of 16 items that capture the core characteristics of binge eating disorder (BED), in which each item offers from three to four levels of symptom descriptions, and higher scores indicate higher severity levels. The BES is composed of two distinct scales: Feelings/Cognitions (F/C), and Behaviors (B). The B scale strictly evaluates binge eating behaviors such as consuming large amounts of food quickly. Opposite to this, the F/C scale evaluates thoughts, feelings, and cognitions that precede eating behaviors (not strictly limited to binge eating). In this study, the Italian version of the BES was used [54]. Although the semantic content of the two scales is different, given the potential strong correlation between them [55], only the F/C scale—assessing thoughts, feelings, and cognitions related to binge eating—was used for the main analysis in this study. In this research, the internal consistency measured by McDonald’s omega was found to be 0.801.

2.4.3. The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire—Revised—18 (TFEQ-R-18)

The TFEQ-R-18 [56] is a well-established and psychometrically robust questionnaire designed to evaluate three primary cognitive and behavioral aspects related to eating disorders: Cognitive Restraint (CR), Uncontrolled Eating (UE), And Emotional Eating (EE). It comprise 18 items rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale, and higher scores indicate greater levels in each of these areas. The CR dimension assesses individuals’ attempts and concerns regarding the regulation of their food consumption to manage body weight and shape. The EE dimension refers to the inclination to eat in reaction to emotional triggers, whether they are positive or negative, such as feelings of distress or other negative emotions. Meanwhile, the UE dimension evaluates the propensity to lose self-control and engage in overeating. Given the semantic content of the items of the CR scale are not of interest for the present study, it was not included in the statistical analyses. In this study, the Italian version of the TFEQ-R-18 was used [57]. In this research, the internal consistency, measured by McDonald’s omega, was found to be 0.864 for the EE scale and 0.877 for the UE scale.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using the software R (v. 4.3.2). In line with the inclusion and exclusion criteria, all observations were complete and contained no missing data. To meet the goals of this study, different phases were conducted [58,59,60,61]. (i) Prior to executing the main statistics, preliminary analyses were undertaken to evaluate the strength of the relationships among the observed variables [62,63]. Also, in line with previous studies, a multivariate multiple regression was conducted to explore the presence of potential socio-demographic factors that might influence the relationship between the variables. (ii) A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA; Diagonally Weighted Least Squares estimator, DWLS) was used to assess the measurement model of each questionnaire by applying traditional goodness-of-fit indices (χ2, RMSEA, CFI, and SRMR) and adhering to the recommended cutoff values: (A) a non-statistically significant χ2, (B) an RMSEA of less than 0.08, (C) a CFI greater than 0.95, and (D) an SRMR below 0.08 [50,64,65]. (iii) An evaluation of common method bias was conducted [63,66,67,68]. (iv) A structural equation model with latent variables (SEM) was specified: FA symptoms (X) were regressed on Uncontrolled Eating (Y) through binge eating thoughts (M1) and Emotional Eating (M2) (see Figure 1) [69,70,71,72]. (v) Latent factors were operationalized through the use of parcels as indicators [73,74]. Specifically, a partially disaggregated item–parcel method was employed [75,76], using a strategy focused on maintaining a balance between items and constructs (i.e., item-to-construct balance strategy) [73,77]. It should be noted that, since the EE scale of the TFEQ-18-R consists of only 3 items, it is not possible to create parcels due to the low number of items. Therefore, for the 3 items composing the EE scale, a fully disaggregated approach was used [68,73,74]. (vi) The SEM using parcels as indicators was conducted employing the maximum likelihood (ML) estimator along with a bootstrap resampling procedure of 5000 iterations using the Bollen–Stine method to address the non-ideal normality of the parcels. Model fit was evaluated using the χ2 test, RMSEA, CFI, and SRMR, in accordance with their recommended cutoff values [50,64]. All regression coefficients presented in the Results section were reported as unstandardized (β).

3. Results

3.1. Preliminary Analysis

Correlation analyses (H1) revealed a moderate to strong positive relationship among the psychological variables involved in the SEM. However, none of these factors appear to exceed critical thresholds. Similarly, for the socio-demographic variables, none seem to show markedly strong associations with the variables of interest (Table 2 and Table S1 in Supplementary Materials).

3.2. Structural Models

The mYFAS2-0 scale showed adequate goodness-of-fit indices: χ2 (44) = 37.368; p = 0.750; RMSEA = 0.000; 90%CI 0.000–0.023; p(RMSEA < 0.05) = 1, CFI = 1, SRMR = 0.074. Also, the BES F/C subscale showed adequate fit indices: χ2 (14) = 16.837; p = 0.265; RMSEA = 0.021; 90%CI 0.000–0.52; p(RMSEA < 0.05) = 0.938, CFI = 0.999, SRMR = 0.042. Lastly, the TFEQ-R-18 (‘Emotional Eating’ and ‘Uncontrolled Eating’ subscales) showed adequate goodness-of-fit indices: χ2 (53) = 155.534; p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.064; 90%CI 0.053–0.076; p (RMSEA < 0.05) = 0.021, CFI = 0.991, SRMR = 0.053.

3.3. Harman’s Single-Factor Test

Harman’s single-factor test showed an absence of the ‘common method bias’. Indeed, the CFA with four correlated factors provided adequate fit indices: χ2 (399) = 805.206; p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.047; 90%CI 0.042–0.051; p(RMSEA < 0.05) = 0.874, CFI = 0.988, SRMR = 0.078. Opposite, the single-factor CFA provided poor-fit indices: χ2 (405) = 1722.523; p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.084; 90%CI 0.080–0.088; p(RMSEA < 0.05) < 0.001, CFI = 0.960, SRMR = 0.111. The model comparison further suggested the absence of the ‘common method bias’: Δχ2 (6) = 917.32, p < 0.001; |ΔRMSEA| = 0.037, and |ΔCFI| = 0.028.

3.4. Latent Mediation Model with Parcels

Item parcels were almost normally distributed, and they were statistically significantly associated with the latent variable and exhibited standardized factor loading higher than 0.5, ranging from 0.628 to 0.885 (see Table S2; Supplementary Materials).
The model (see Figure 1) provided adequate goodness-of-fit indices: χ2 (59) = 132.909; p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.052; 90%CI 0.040–0.064; p(RMSEA < 0.05) = 0.384 ns, CFI = 0.978, SRMR = 0.035. Results are reported in Figure 2 and Table 3.
The hypothesized model (H2) showed that FA symptoms (X) were positively associated with Binge Eating Feelings/Cognitions (M1), path a1: β = 1.605 (SE = 0.231) [95%CI: 1.241; 2.154], z = 6.935, p < 0.001. At the same time, Binge Eating Feelings/Cognitions (M1) were positively associated with Emotional Eating (M2), path d: β = 0.512 (SE = 0.113) [95%CI: 0.314; 0.752], z = 4.534, p < 0.001. Also, Emotional Eating (M2) predicted Uncontrolled Eating behaviors (Y), path b2: β = 0.632 (SE = 0.103) [95%CI: 0.438; 0.844], z = 6.121, p < 0.001. Lastly, (H3) FA symptoms (X) were no longer associated with Uncontrolled Eating behaviors (Y), path c’: β = −0.259 (SE = 0.252) [95%CI: −0.868; 0.098], z = −1.026, p = 0.305, thus suggesting a totally mediated path.
Considering the other paths, interestingly, FA symptoms (X) were no longer associated with emotional eating (M2), path a2: β = −0.096 (SE = 0.218) [95%CI: −0.585; 0.264], z = −0.440, p = 0.660 ns. Opposite, in line with expectations, Binge Eating Feelings/Cognitions (M1) predicted Uncontrolled Eating behaviors (Y), path b1: β = 0.393 (SE = 0.136) [95%CI: 0.174; 0.712], z = 2.892, p = 0.004.
Moreover, an examination of the indirect path was conducted. The total indirect effect (FA symptoms → Binge Eating Feelings/Cognitions → Emotional Eating → Uncontrolled Eating behaviors) was statistically significant: β = 0.519 (SE = 0.130) [95%CI: 0.312; 0.827], z = 3.986, p < 0.001. Also, the total model effect was inspected, the results being statistically significant: β = 0.831 (SE = 0.116) [95%CI: 0.635; 1.083], z = 7.193, p < 0.001. The degree of explained variance (R2) was equal to 61.6% (R2 = 0.616).

4. Discussion

Over the years, numerous studies have highlighted the significant impact of FA on the physical and mental health of individuals. Consequently, the present research aimed to provide a deeper understanding of the association between FA symptoms and dysfunctional eating behaviors such as uncontrolled eating in order to design timely psychological interventions and to explore their effects on other constructs within research contexts.
Indeed, there has been a steady increase in the prevalence of obesity and EDs worldwide, particularly in industrialized countries where expenditure for medical and psychological conditions related to HPF consumption is markedly high [78,79]. The literature acknowledges that these foods [6,7,10]—highly palatable and psychologically rewarding—can lead some individuals to develop an addiction to food, known as FA [80]. FA appears to play a pivotal role in this epidemic, contributing to unhealthy eating behaviors such as grazing, binge eating, or uncontrolled eating, which, over time, promote the development of obesity [23,81].
This study is the first to enhance the understanding of how eating-related thoughts, along with emotional eating, contribute to the development of disordered eating behaviors starting from FA symptoms—examining the conceptual model illustrated in Figure 1 and Figure 2—in a sample of individuals enrolled from the general population. This research explores the relationship between (A) FA symptoms, (B) thoughts, (C) emotion-driven behaviors, and (D) uncontrolled eating.
The primary findings of this research indicate that FA symptoms do not have a direct effect on uncontrolled eating, as their relationship is fully mediated by binge eating thoughts and emotional eating, accounting for a notable portion of the variance in uncontrolled eating. This highlights the critical role of cognitive and emotional components in mediating the effects of FA symptoms on the tendency to engage in uncontrolled eating behaviors [24,34,82]. The mediating influence of these factors suggests that uncontrolled eating behaviors may be triggered by the development of food-focused thought processes in individuals exhibiting FA symptoms [34]. Indeed, FA includes symptoms such as withdrawal and craving for HPFs. The presence of these elements leads individuals to experience persistent and often intrusive thoughts, consuming much of their time [30]. These thoughts frequently orbit around food, concerns about it, and strategies for obtaining, consuming, and savoring it, despite awareness of the harm HPFs may cause. The prominence of food and eating-related worries, fears, desires, and cravings underscores the significant impact of dysfunctional eating-related thoughts, which are critical variables in various disordered eating behaviors and eating disorders [44,82,83]. At the same time, these thoughts contribute to the development of negative emotions. These emotions often include anxiety, anger, and frustration, stemming from both the condition of dependency and the inability to immediately satisfy the need to consume the desired substance [41,43]. Additionally, feelings of worry and fears of losing control are frequently present. Such emotions are often overwhelming, prompting the individual to employ emotional eating (e.g., a coping strategy) to manage them [42]. Finally, the model shows that emotional eating—preceded by FA symptoms and eating-related thoughts—drives individuals to engage in disordered eating behaviors that may lead to compulsive overeating, uncontrolled eating, and a loss of control over food consumption in a maladaptive effort to cope, further reinforcing and maintaining the cycle of addiction [49,84,85,86,87,88].
These findings suggest that FA symptoms and uncontrolled eating behaviors are not necessarily linearly connected, but that their association is totally mediated by thoughts and emotion-driven behaviors, with particular emphasis on the former. Indeed, it is important to highlight that, when controlling for eating-related thoughts, FA symptoms are no longer associated with either emotional eating or uncontrolled eating. This result highlights the crucial role of thoughts as a key element in developing coping strategies and dysfunctional eating behaviors [34]. On the one hand, these findings suggest that emotional eating must also rely on a cognitive component to manifest and that, consistent with the literature, emotions seem to follow the individual’s mental state. On the other hand, these results appear to indicate that symptoms such as withdrawal or craving, once the effect of thoughts is controlled for, do not seem capable of leading to maladaptive behaviors.
It is important to emphasize that, in this model, thoughts (related to food) were placed sequentially before emotion-driven behaviors. It is crucial to note that the relationship between emotions and thoughts has been the subject of numerous studies, revealing that they can influence each other [89,90]. Indeed, some theories suggest that thoughts often precede emotions, while others propose the opposite [90]. Indeed, there are theories that postulate emotional states as antecedents of thoughts, suggesting that emotions can manifest before conscious thoughts [91,92,93,94]. For example, physiological responses to stimuli can rapidly trigger emotions, often before an individual has the time to process or articulate their thoughts about the situation. This perspective is supported by studies indicating that emotional reactions are fundamental in decision making and cognitive processes, as individuals deprived of emotional responses often encounter difficulties in logical reasoning and decision making [91]. However, this is not the case for the variables involved in this research. The hypothesis described above might be valid in specific conditions, such as individuals experiencing severe food deprivation or intense craving states [34,85]. In fact, the participants evaluated in this study did not exhibit such characteristics. Instead, this study assessed the number of FA symptoms over the past year [52], being, therefore, in a chronic rather than an acute condition. Based on this chronicity of symptoms, a significant body of empirical evidence highlights the role of cognitive appraisal in shaping emotional experiences, leading to the formulation of feelings based on the cognitive interpretation of the situation [95,96,97]. According to these theories, thoughts can (also) influence how we experience and define emotions [95,97]. Thus, the relationship between emotions and thoughts is dynamic and context-dependent, suggesting an interconnectedness between the two. For example, some research indicates that children’s understanding of how thoughts influence emotions develops over time [89]. This suggests that, while emotions may arise first, conscious thought can later modify or influence these emotional states. This interaction underscores that both processes are essential in shaping human behavior and experiences [89,98]. Lastly, regarding the variables measured in this study, it is worth emphasizing once again that the variable ‘Emotional Eating’ is tied to a behavioral actions influenced by emotions rather than to simple/pure emotional states [41,42]. Therefore, the reasoning behind positioning these variables (thoughts and emotions related to food) in this way appears more plausible [99]: the FA symptoms from the past year lead to eating-related thoughts and emotional eating behaviors.
In summary, these findings enhance our understanding of eating-related thoughts and emotional-driven behaviors among individuals exhibiting core features of disordered eating behaviors [49,88]. Additionally, they align with existing research indicating that thoughts about food significantly contribute to the variance observed in uncontrolled eating. Specifically, studies have shown that individuals with eating-related craving symptoms demonstrate higher levels of overwhelming eating-related thoughts. Also, studies have shown that these thoughts may trigger emotional eating behaviors, which in turn lead to binge eating. However, prior studies did not specifically examine the relationship between these variables together starting from FA symptoms. This study provides new insights into the nature of FA symptoms by elucidating the mechanisms connecting eating-related thoughts and emotional eating to maladaptive patterns of eating behaviors. However, it is important to note that these findings are based on a moderately large sample recruited from the general population, and further studies on clinical samples are needed to provide additional confirmation.
Thus, the shift from FA to thoughts of uncontrolled eating represents a complex interplay between psychological, physiological, emotional, and behavioral elements. Increasingly, FA has been considered a major marker for a variety of eating disorders, mainly BED, characterized by compulsive eating behaviors and loss of control over eating despite adverse consequences. However, this association is fully mediated by psychological factors. Individuals with FA symptoms frequently report persistent thoughts of concern about food; thoughts about its availability, preparation, and anticipatory ideas related to its consumption leading to emotional eating. The incidence of emotional eating can further cycle FA and binge eating because temporary relief may be obtained through food consumption, but will ultimately result in negative psychological effects that continue to support disordered eating [49,84,85,86,87,88].

4.1. Strengths and Limitations

This research has several limitations. Firstly, it relies on self-report measures without clinical interviews or behavioral measures to properly evaluate the severity of FA symptoms as well as uncontrolled eating behaviors. Nonetheless, all assessment tools used for this study are well validated and consistently measure the target construct. Secondly, due to the cross-sectional design, causal relationships cannot be established; however, robust statistical methods enabled the testing of hypotheses regarding associations and influences among constructs. Future studies could employ longitudinal designs to explore the long-term effects of FA symptoms [100]. Thirdly, this study examined a limited set of variables, and other confounding factors—such as anxiety, impulsivity, and emotion regulation difficulties—may also play a role, warranting investigation in future research. Fourthly, the sample was slightly skewed towards females, suggesting that future studies should consider gender and ethnic differences. Furthermore, it is important to note that although the sample in the present study is moderately large, the results are only extendable to individuals drawn from the general Italian population, making them susceptible to potential representation bias. For this reason, future studies could aim to extend the sample—making it more representative of the Italian population—and/or test the same hypotheses in individuals with different clinical conditions, such as eating and feeding disorders and/or obesity. Lastly, it should be noted that since all the scales used are related to the domain of excessive food intake and overeating, there might be semantic redundancy among the items of the scales, potentially amplifying the correlations between the scales themselves. However, it is important to note that the correlations among the scales used do not exceed the critical threshold of 0.84 [62,83] and are all associated with different instruments. In this regard, the only potentially problematic correlations would be those between scales within the same instrument—in this case, the TFEQ-R-18. However, the correlation between the EE scale and the UE scale is 0.654, which is well below the critical threshold. For these reasons, there is no excessive concern, and the results can be considered sufficiently reliable.
Despite these limitations, this research has notable strengths. Firstly, it involved a large and diverse sample of individuals. Secondly, this study employed rigorous methodology and statistical analyses based on validated procedures. Third, this research also contributes to a deeply understanding of the relationships among the FA symptoms, binge eating-related thoughts, emotional eating, and uncontrolled eating. Additionally, it establishes a connection between some of the most important psychological variables upon which modern therapies are based, namely the centrality of thoughts and emotions in behavioral processes.

4.2. Implications for Clinical and Research Practice

The clinical implications of this study are significant for both research and clinical practice. Regarding the former, this study provides a solid experimental foundation to enhance our understanding of one of the most important and debated phenomena in recent years.
At the same time, the clinical applications align closely with what was outlined in the previous section. Indeed, it is worth emphasizing that modern evidence-based psychotherapeutic approaches aim to improve the management of (dysfunctional) thoughts, metacognitions, intrusive thoughts, and negative emotions [101,102,103,104,105]. This study highlights the key and central role of thoughts and emotions in behavioral processes related to uncontrolled eating, thus offering an initial guideline for clinicians working with individuals exhibiting FA symptoms [106].

5. Conclusions

This study contributes to the understanding of disordered eating behaviors associated with FA and its possible treatment. The findings indicate that both eating-related thoughts and emotional eating serve as mediators in the relationship between FA symptoms and uncontrolled eating. These results underscore the significant role that thoughts and emotions play in the development and persistence of dysfunctional eating behaviors.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu17030369/s1, Table S1: Multivariate multiple regression between socio-demographic variables and variables used in the SEM model; Table S2: Item parcels’ descriptive statistics and factor loadings (λ).

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Padua (no. 3558) on 10 April 2020.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available on a reasonable request due to privacy and ethical restrictions.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Liu, Y.; Song, Y.; Hao, Q.; Wu, J. Global Prevalence of Osteosarcopenic Obesity amongst Middle Aged and Older Adults: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Arch. Osteoporos. 2023, 18, 60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Heeren, F.A.N.; Darcey, V.L.; Deemer, S.E.; Menon, S.; Tobias, D.; Cardel, M.I. Breaking down Silos: The Multifaceted Nature of Obesity and the Future of Weight Management. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2023, 378, 20220215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Haslam, D.W.; James, W.P.T. Obesity. Lancet 2005, 366, 1197–1209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Mandlik, M.; Oetzel, J.G.; Kadirov, D. Obesity and Health Care Interventions: Substantiating a Multi-Modal Challenge through the Lens of Grounded Theory. Health Promot. J. Aust. 2021, 32, 274–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Neff, K.M.H.; Fay, A.; Saules, K.K. Foods and Nutritional Characteristics Associated with Addictive-Like Eating. Psychol. Rep. 2022, 125, 1937–1956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Gearhardt, A.N.; DiFeliceantonio, A.G. Highly Processed Foods Can Be Considered Addictive Substances Based on Established Scientific Criteria. Addiction 2023, 118, 589–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Schulte, E.M.; Avena, N.M.; Gearhardt, A.N. Which Foods May Be Addictive? The Roles of Processing, Fat Content, and Glycemic Load. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0117959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Schulte, E.M.; Smeal, J.K.; Lewis, J.; Gearhardt, A.N. Development of the Highly Processed Food Withdrawal Scale. Appetite 2018, 131, 148–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Monteiro, C.A.; Cannon, G.; Moubarac, J.-C.; Levy, R.B.; Louzada, M.L.C.; Jaime, P.C. The UN Decade of Nutrition, the NOVA Food Classification and the Trouble with Ultra-Processing. Public Health Nutr. 2018, 21, 5–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Gearhardt, A.N.; Schulte, E.M. Is Food Addictive? A Review of the Science. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 2021, 41, 387–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Manzoni, G.M.; Rossi, A.; Pietrabissa, G.; Mannarini, S.; Fabbricatore, M.; Imperatori, C.; Innamorati, M.; Gearhardt, A.N.; Castelnuovo, G. Structural Validity, Measurement Invariance, Reliability and Diagnostic Accuracy of the Italian Version of the Yale Food Addiction Scale 2.0 in Patients with Severe Obesity and the General Population. Eat. Weight Disord. Stud. Anorex. Bulim. Obes. 2021, 26, 345–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Gearhardt, A.N.; Corbin, W.R.; Brownell, K.D. Food Addiction: An Examination of the Diagnostic Criteria for Dependence. J. Addict. Med. 2009, 3, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Volkow, N.D.; Wang, G.-J.; Fowler, J.S.; Telang, F. Overlapping Neuronal Circuits in Addiction and Obesity: Evidence of Systems Pathology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2008, 363, 3191–3200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Wang, G.-J.; Volkow, N.D.; Thanos, P.K.; Fowler, J.S. Similarity Between Obesity and Drug Addiction as Assessed by Neurofunctional Imaging. J. Addict. Dis. 2004, 23, 39–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Davis, C. Compulsive Overeating as an Addictive Behavior: Overlap Between Food Addiction and Binge Eating Disorder. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2013, 2, 171–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Schulte, E.M.; Gearhardt, A.N. Attributes of the Food Addiction Phenotype within Overweight and Obesity. Eat. Weight Disord. -Stud. Anorex. Bulim. Obes. 2020, 26, 2043–2049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Schulte, E.M.; Joyner, M.A.; Potenza, M.N.; Grilo, C.M.; Gearhardt, A.N. Current Considerations Regarding Food Addiction. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2015, 17, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Schulte, E.M.; Wadden, T.A.; Allison, K.C. An Evaluation of Food Addiction as a Distinct Psychiatric Disorder. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2020, 53, 1610–1622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Meule, A.; Gearhardt, A.N. Ten Years of the Yale Food Addiction Scale: A Review of Version 2.0. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2019, 6, 218–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Meule, A.; Gearhardt, A.N. Five Years of the Yale Food Addiction Scale: Taking Stock and Moving Forward. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2014, 1, 193–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Brewerton, T.D. Food Addiction as a Proxy for Eating Disorder and Obesity Severity, Trauma History, PTSD Symptoms, and Comorbidity. Eat. Weight. Disord. 2017, 22, 241–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Gold, M.S.; Kimberly, F.-P.; Jacobs, W.S. Overeating, Binge Eating, and Eating Disorders as Addictions. Psychiatr. Ann. 2003, 33, 117–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Rossi, A.A.; Mannarini, S.; Semonella, M.; Castelnuovo, G.; Pietrabissa, G. The Association between Grazing and Food Addiction: The Italian Version of the Repetitive Eating Questionnaire (Rep(Eat)-Q) and Its Relationships with Food Addiction Criteria. Nutrients 2024, 16, 949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Rossi, A.A.; Pietrabissa, G.; Gearhardt, A.N.; Musetti, A.; Castelnuovo, G.; Mannarini, S. Eating Compulsivity in Inpatients with Severe Obesity and the General Population: The Italian Version of the Measure of Eating Compulsivity (MEC10-IT). Nutrients 2023, 15, 1378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Schroder, R.; Sellman, J.D.; Adamson, S. Development and Validation of a Brief Measure of Eating Compulsivity (MEC). Subst. Use Misuse 2017, 52, 1918–1924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Conceição, E.M.; Mitchell, J.E.; Machado, P.P.P.; Vaz, A.R.; Pinto-Bastos, A.; Ramalho, S.; Brandão, I.; Simões, J.B.; de Lourdes, M.; Freitas, A.C. Repetitive Eating Questionnaire [Rep(Eat)-Q]: Enlightening the Concept of Grazing and Psychometric Properties in a Portuguese Sample. Appetite 2017, 117, 351–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Conceição, E.M.; Mitchell, J.E.; Engel, S.G.; Machado, P.P.P.; Lancaster, K.; Wonderlich, S.A. What Is “Grazing”? Reviewing Its Definition, Frequency, Clinical Characteristics, and Impact on Bariatric Surgery Outcomes, and Proposing a Standardized Definition. Surg. Obes. Relat. Dis. 2014, 10, 973–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Volkow, N.D.; Wise, R.A.; Baler, R. The Dopamine Motive System: Implications for Drug and Food Addiction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017, 18, 741–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Davis, C.; Loxton, N.J.; Levitan, R.D.; Kaplan, A.S.; Carter, J.C.; Kennedy, J.L. ‘Food Addiction’ and Its Association with a Dopaminergic Multilocus Genetic Profile. Physiol. Behav. 2013, 118, 63–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Meule, A.; Gearhardt, A.N. Food Addiction in the Light of DSM-5. Nutrients 2014, 6, 3653–3671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Gilbert, D.G.; Gilbert, B.O.; Schultz, V.L. Withdrawal Symptoms: Individual Differences and Similarities across Addictive Behaviors. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1998, 24, 351–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Parnarouskis, L.; Leventhal, A.M.; Ferguson, S.G.; Gearhardt, A.N. Withdrawal: A Key Consideration in Evaluating Whether Highly Processed Foods Are Addictive. Obes. Rev. 2022, 23, e13507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Schulte, E.M.; Grilo, C.M.; Gearhardt, A.N. Shared and Unique Mechanisms Underlying Binge Eating Disorder and Addictive Disorders. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2016, 44, 125–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Schumacher, S.; Kemps, E.; Tiggemann, M. The Food Craving Experience: Thoughts, Images and Resistance as Predictors of Craving Intensity and Consumption. Appetite 2019, 133, 387–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Gormally, J.; Black, S.; Daston, S.; Rardin, D. The Assessment of Binge Eating Severity among Obese Persons. Addict. Behav. 1982, 7, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Whiteside, U.; Chen, E.; Neighbors, C.; Hunter, D.; Lo, T.; Larimer, M. Difficulties Regulating Emotions: Do Binge Eaters Have Fewer Strategies to Modulate and Tolerate Negative Affect? Eat. Behav. 2007, 8, 162–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. MacLean, P.S.; Blundell, J.E.; Mennella, J.A.; Batterham, R.L. Biological Control of Appetite: A Daunting Complexity. Obesity 2017, 25, S8–S16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Goldbacher, E.M.; Grunwald, H.E.; LaGrotte, C.A.; Klotz, A.A.; Oliver, T.L.; Musliner, K.L.; VanderVeur, S.S.; Foster, G.D. Factor Structure of the Emotional Eating Scale in Overweight and Obese Adults Seeking Treatment. Appetite 2012, 59, 610–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Gearhardt, A.N.; White, M.A.; Masheb, R.M.; Morgan, P.T.; Crosby, R.D.; Grilo, C.M. An Examination of the Food Addiction Construct in Obese Patients with Binge Eating Disorder. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2012, 45, 657–663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Poole, N.A.; Atar, A.A.; Kuhanendran, D.; Bidlake, L.; Fiennes, A.; McCluskey, S.; Nussey, S.; Bano, G.; Morgan, J.F. Compliance with Surgical After-Care Following Bariatric Surgery for Morbid Obesity: A Retrospective Study. Obes. Surg. 2005, 15, 261–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Frayn, M.; Knäuper, B. Emotional Eating and Weight in Adults: A Review. Curr. Psychol. 2018, 37, 924–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Reichenberger, J.; Schnepper, R.; Arend, A.-K.; Blechert, J. Emotional Eating in Healthy Individuals and Patients with an Eating Disorder: Evidence from Psychometric, Experimental and Naturalistic Studies. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2020, 79, 290–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Meule, A.; Richard, A.; Schnepper, R.; Reichenberger, J.; Georgii, C.; Naab, S.; Voderholzer, U.; Blechert, J. Emotion Regulation and Emotional Eating in Anorexia Nervosa and Bulimia Nervosa. Eat. Disord. 2021, 29, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Rossi, A.A.; Pietrabissa, G.; Tagliagambe, A.; Scuderi, A.; Montecchiani, L.; Castelnuovo, G.; Mannarini, S.; Dalla Ragione, L. Many Facets of Eating Disorders: Profiling Key Psychological Features of Anorexia Nervosa and Binge Eating Disorder. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Hilbert, A.; Pike, K.M.; Goldschmidt, A.B.; Wilfley, D.E.; Fairburn, C.G.; Dohm, F.-A.; Walsh, B.T.; Striegel Weissman, R. Risk Factors across the Eating Disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2014, 220, 500–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Klein, D.A.; Walsh, B.T. Eating Disorders: Clinical Features and Pathophysiology. Physiol. Behav. 2004, 81, 359–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Nasirzadeh, Y.; Kantarovich, K.; Wnuk, S.; Okrainec, A.; Cassin, S.E.; Hawa, R.; Sockalingam, S. Binge Eating, Loss of Control over Eating, Emotional Eating, and Night Eating After Bariatric Surgery: Results from the Toronto Bari-PSYCH Cohort Study. Obes. Surg. 2018, 28, 2032–2039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Johnson, T.P. Snowball Sampling: Introduction. In Wiley StatsRef: Statistics Reference Online; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2014; ISBN 978-1-118-44511-2. [Google Scholar]
  49. Pietrabissa, G.; Castelnuovo, G.; Semonella, M.; Mannarini, S.; Rossi, A.A. Measuring Motivations to Eat Palatable Foods: Adaptation and Psychometric Properties of the Italian Version of the Palatable Eating Motives Scale (PEMS-IT). Healthcare 2024, 12, 574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  51. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  52. Schulte, E.M.; Gearhardt, A.N. Development of the Modified Yale Food Addiction Scale Version 2.0. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2017, 25, 302–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Imperatori, C.; Fabbricatore, M.; Lester, D.; Manzoni, G.M.; Castelnuovo, G.; Raimondi, G.; Innamorati, M. Psychometric Properties of the Modified Yale Food Addiction Scale Version 2.0 in an Italian Non-Clinical Sample. Eat. Weight Disord. 2019, 24, 37–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Ricca, V.; Mannucci, E.; Moretti, S.; Di Bernardo, M.; Zucchi, T.; Cabras, P.L.; Rotella, C.M. Screening for Binge Eating Disorder in Obese Outpatients. Compr. Psychiatry 2000, 41, 111–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Escrivá-Martínez, T.; Galiana, L.; Rodríguez-Arias, M.; Baños, R.M. The Binge Eating Scale: Structural Equation Competitive Models, Invariance Measurement Between Sexes, and Relationships with Food Addiction, Impulsivity, Binge Drinking, and Body Mass Index. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Karlsson, J.; Persson, L.-O.; Sjöström, L.; Sullivan, M. Psychometric Properties and Factor Structure of the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) in Obese Men and Women. Results from the Swedish Obese Subjects (SOS) Study. Int. J. Obes. 2000, 24, 1715–1725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Rossi, A.A.; Pietrabissa, G.; Castelnuovo, G.; Mannarini, S. Cognitive Restraint, Uncontrolled Eating, and Emotional Eating. The Italian Version of the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire-Revised 18 (TFEQ-R-18): A Three-step Validation Study. Eat. Weight Disord. Stud. Anorex. Bulim. Obes. 2024, 29, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. MacKinnon, D. Introduction to Statistical Mediation Analysis; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2008; ISBN 978-0-203-80955-6. [Google Scholar]
  59. MacKinnon, D.P.; Kisbu-Sakarya, Y.; Gottschall, A.C. Developments in mediation analysis. Oxf. Handb. Quant. Methods 2013, 2, 338–360. [Google Scholar]
  60. Rucker, D.D.; Preacher, K.J.; Tormala, Z.L.; Petty, R.E. Mediation Analysis in Social Psychology: Current Practices and New Recommendations. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2011, 5, 359–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. McDonald, R.P.; Ho, M.-H.R. Principles and Practice in Reporting Structural Equation Analyses. Psychol. Methods 2002, 7, 64–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S. Using Multivariate Statistics; Pearson: Harlow, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  63. Rossi, A.A.; Mannarini, S.; Donini, L.M.; Castelnuovo, G.; Simpson, S.; Pietrabissa, G. Dieting, Obsessive-Compulsive Thoughts, and Orthorexia Nervosa: Assessing the Mediating Role of Worries about Food through a Structural Equation Model Approach. Appetite 2024, 193, 107164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Brown, T.A. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. xvii, 462. ISBN 978-1-4625-1779-4. [Google Scholar]
  65. Hoyle, R.H. Handbook of Structural Equation Modeling, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  66. Harman, H.H. Modern Factor Analysis; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1976; ISBN 978-0-226-31652-9. [Google Scholar]
  67. Podsakoff, P.M.; MacKenzie, S.B.; Lee, J.-Y.; Podsakoff, N.P. Common Method Biases in Behavioral Research: A Critical Review of the Literature and Recommended Remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 879–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Rossi, A.A.; Panzeri, A.; Fernández, I.; Invernizzi, R.; Taccini, F.; Mannarini, S. The Impact of Trauma Core Dimensions on Anxiety and Depression: A Latent Regression Model Through the Post-Traumatic Symptom Questionnaire (PTSQ). Sci. Rep. 2024, 14, 23036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Frazier, P.A.; Tix, A.P.; Barron, K.E. Testing Moderator and Mediator Effects in Counseling Psychology Research. J. Couns. Psychol. 2004, 51, 115–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Iacobucci, D. Mediation Analysis; SAGE Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2008; ISBN 978-1-4129-8496-6. [Google Scholar]
  71. MacKinnon, D.P.; Fairchild, A.J.; Fritz, M.S. Mediation Analysis. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007, 58, 593–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Wiedermann, W.; von Eye, A. Direction of Effects in Mediation Analysis. Psychol. Methods 2015, 20, 221–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Little, T.D.; Cunningham, W.A.; Shahar, G.; Widaman, K.F. To Parcel or Not to Parcel: Exploring the Question, Weighing the Merits. Struct. Equ. Model. 2002, 9, 151–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Little, T.D.; Rhemtulla, M.; Gibson, K.; Schoemann, A.M. Why the Items versus Parcels Controversy Needn’t Be One. Psychol. Methods 2013, 18, 285–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Bandalos, D.L.; Finney, S.J. Item Parceling Issues in Structural Equation Modeling. In New Developments and Techniques in Structural Equation Modeling; Psychology Press: East Sussex, UK, 2001; ISBN 978-1-4106-0185-8. [Google Scholar]
  76. Coffman, D.L.; MacCallum, R.C. Using Parcels to Convert Path Analysis Models Into Latent Variable Models. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2005, 40, 235–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Yang, C.; Nay, S.; Hoyle, R.H. Three Approaches to Using Lengthy Ordinal Scales in Structural Equation Models: Parceling, Latent Scoring, and Shortening Scales. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 2010, 34, 122–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Mokdad, A.H.; Serdula, M.K.; Dietz, W.H.; Bowman, B.A.; Marks, J.S.; Koplan, J.P. The Continuing Epidemic of Obesity in the United States. JAMA 2000, 284, 1650–1651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Wang, Y.; Beydoun, M.A.; Liang, L.; Caballero, B.; Kumanyika, S.K. Will All Americans Become Overweight or Obese? Estimating the Progression and Cost of the US Obesity Epidemic. Obesity 2008, 16, 2323–2330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Krupa, H.; Gearhardt, A.N.; Lewandowski, A.; Avena, N.M. Food Addiction. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Aloi, M.; Liuzza, M.T.; Rania, M.; Carbone, E.A.; de Filippis, R.; Gearhardt, A.N.; Segura-Garcia, C. Using Latent Class Analysis to Identify Different Clinical Profiles According to Food Addiction Symptoms in Obesity with and without Binge Eating Disorder. J. Behav. Addict. 2024, 13, 262–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Rossi, A.A.; Mannarini, S.; Castelnuovo, G.; Pietrabissa, G. Disordered Eating Behaviors Related to Food Addiction/Eating Addiction in Inpatients with Obesity and the General Population: The Italian Version of the Addiction-like Eating Behaviors Scale (AEBS-IT). Nutrients 2023, 15, 104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Panzeri, A.; Castelnuovo, G.; Spoto, A. Assessing Discriminant Validity through Structural Equation Modeling: The Case of Eating Compulsivity. Nutrients 2024, 16, 550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Meule, A.; Hermann, T.; Kübler, A. Food Addiction in Overweight and Obese Adolescents Seeking Weight-Loss Treatment. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2015, 23, 193–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Parylak, S.L.; Koob, G.F.; Zorrilla, E.P. The Dark Side of Food Addiction. Physiol. Behav. 2011, 104, 149–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Orford, J. Addiction as Excessive Appetite. Addiction 2001, 96, 15–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Meye, F.J.; Adan, R.A.H. Feelings about Food: The Ventral Tegmental Area in Food Reward and Emotional Eating. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 2014, 35, 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Bozkurt, O.; Çamli, A.; Kocaadam-Bozkurt, B. Factors Affecting Food Addiction: Emotional Eating, Palatable Eating Motivations, and BMI. Food Sci. Nutr. 2024, 12, 6841–6848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Lagattuta, K.H.; Elrod, N.M.; Kramer, H.J. How Do Thoughts, Emotions, and Decisions Align? A New Way to Examine Theory of Mind during Middle Childhood and Beyond. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2016, 149, 116–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Dalgleish, T.; Power, M. Handbook of Cognition and Emotion; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2000; ISBN 978-0-470-84221-8. [Google Scholar]
  91. Izard, C.E. Emotion Theory and Research: Highlights, Unanswered Questions, and Emerging Issues. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2009, 60, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Taylor, S.E.; Stanton, A.L. Coping Resources, Coping Processes, and Mental Health. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2007, 3, 377–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Gross, J.J.; Levenson, R.W. Emotional Suppression: Physiology, Self-Report, and Expressive Behavior. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 64, 970–986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Mitchell, R.L.C.; Phillips, L.H. The Overlapping Relationship between Emotion Perception and Theory of Mind. Neuropsychologia 2015, 70, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Beck, A.T.; Rush, J.A.; Shaw, B.F.; Emery, G.; DeRubeis, R.J.; Hollon, S.D. Cognitive Therapy of Depression, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2024. [Google Scholar]
  96. Lagattuta, K.H. Linking Past, Present, and Future: Children’s Ability to Connect Mental States and Emotions Across Time. Child Dev. Perspect. 2014, 8, 90–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Lazarus, R.S. Thoughts on the Relations between Emotion and Cognition. Am. Psychol. 1982, 37, 1019–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Dillon, D.G.; Pizzagalli, D.A. Inhibition of Action, Thought, and Emotion: A Selective Neurobiological Review. Appl. Prev. Psychol. 2007, 12, 99–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Wood, W.; Quinn, J.M.; Kashy, D.A. Habits in Everyday Life: Thought, Emotion, and Action. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 83, 1281–1297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Pietrabissa, G.; Sorgente, A.; Rossi, A.; Simpson, S.; Riva, G.; Manzoni, G.M.; Prochaska, J.O.; Prochaska, J.M.; Cattivelli, R.; Castelnuovo, G. Stages of Change in Obesity and Weight Management: Factorial Structure of the Italian Version of the University of Rhode Island Change Assessment Scale. Eat. Weight. Disord. 2017, 22, 361–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Kaya Cebioğlu, İ.; Dumlu Bilgin, G.; Kavsara, H.K.; Gül Koyuncu, A.; Sarioğlu, A.; Aydin, S.; Keküllüoğlu, M. Food Addiction among University Students: The Effect of Mindful Eating. Appetite 2022, 177, 106133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Robertson, S.; Strodl, E. Metacognitive Therapy for Binge Eating Disorder: A Case Series Study. Clin. Psychol. 2020, 24, 143–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Aloi, M.; Rania, M.; Carbone, E.A.; Caroleo, M.; Calabrò, G.; Zaffino, P.; Nicolò, G.; Carcione, A.; Coco, G.L.; Cosentino, C.; et al. Metacognition and Emotion Regulation as Treatment Targets in Binge Eating Disorder: A Network Analysis Study. J. Eat. Disord. 2021, 9, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Laghi, F.; Bianchi, D.; Pompili, S.; Lonigro, A.; Baiocco, R. Metacognition, Emotional Functioning and Binge Eating in Adolescence: The Moderation Role of Need to Control Thoughts. Eat. Weight. Disord. 2018, 23, 861–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Clyne, C.; Blampied, N.M. Training in Emotion Regulation as a Treatment for Binge Eating: A Preliminary Study. Behav. Chang. 2004, 21, 269–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. O’Hea, E.L.; Edwards-Hampton, S.A.; Beall Brown, D.L.; Sonneville, K.R.; Ziedonis, D.M.; Gearhardt, A.N. The Food Addiction Clinical Treatment (FACT) Manual: A Harm Reduction Treatment Approach. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Structural equation model—conceptual representation.
Figure 1. Structural equation model—conceptual representation.
Nutrients 17 00369 g001
Figure 2. Structural equation model with unstandardized regression coefficients, standard errors, and associated p-values.
Figure 2. Structural equation model with unstandardized regression coefficients, standard errors, and associated p-values.
Nutrients 17 00369 g002
Table 1. Sample’s descriptive statistics.
Table 1. Sample’s descriptive statistics.
Variable Descriptive Statistics
Age (M. SD)31.2513.52
Sex (n. %)
Male 12526.8%
Female34273.2%
Marital status (n. %)
Single18239.0%
In a relationship18439.4%
Married7516.1%
Separated/divorced234.9%
Widowed30.6%
Education (n. %)
Middle school337.1%
High school22448.0%
University 18840.3%
Ph.D. 224.7%
Work status (n. %)
Student22548.2%
Full-time worker15232.5%
Part-time worker183.9%
Entrepreneur 4810.3%
Housewife 30.6%
Unemployed153.2%
Retired 61.3%
BMI Class (n, %)
Severely underweight (<16)20.4%
Underweight (16–18.49)4910.5%
Normal weight (18.5–24.99)26957.6%
Overweight (25–29.44)7415.8%
Class I obesity (30–34.99)5812.4%
Class II obesity (35–39.99)122.6%
Class III obesity (>40)30.6%
Eating disorder (n. %)
No eating disorder40185.9%
Anorexia Nervosa214.5%
Bulimia Nervosa143.0%
Binge eating disorder306.4%
Eating disorder not otherwise specified10.2%
Note: N = 467. M = mean; SD = standard deviation.
Table 2. Study 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations among variables.
Table 2. Study 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations among variables.
Descriptive StatisticsCorrelations
MSDSKK123456
1mYFAS2 Symptom Count1.001.892.567.03-
2BES total score8.517.361.271.770.712-
3BES Feelings/Cognitions3.973.821.271.420.6890.919-
4BES Behaviors4.554.131.301.920.6310.9310.713-
5TFEQ-R-18 Emotional eating6.632.630.34−0.820.4740.6080.5590.567-
6TFEQ-R-18 Uncontrolled Eating17.495.620.770.230.4600.6510.5680.6340.654-
Note: all correlations are statistically significant with p < 0.001. M = mean, SD = standard deviation, SK = skewness, K = kurtosis.
Table 3. Summary of standardized parameter estimates (Beta) with 95% confidence intervals of the tested model (Figure 2).
Table 3. Summary of standardized parameter estimates (Beta) with 95% confidence intervals of the tested model (Figure 2).
β*β (SE)95%CI [L - U]z-Valuep-ValueR2
FA symptoms (X) → Feelings/Cognitions (M1)a10.8491.605 (0.231)[1.241; 2.154]6.935p < 0.0010.720
Feelings/Cognitions (M1) → Emotional Eating (M2)d0.7240.512 (0.113)[0.314; 0.752]4.534p < 0.0010.441
Emotional Eating (M2) → Uncontrolled Eating (Y)b20.5240.632 (0.103)[0.438; 0.844]6.121p < 0.0010.616
FA symptoms (X) → Uncontrolled Eating (Y)c’−0.160−0.259 (0.252)[−0.868; 0.098]−1.0260.305
FA symptoms (X) → Emotional Eating (M2)a2−0.072−0.096 (0.218)[−0.585; 0.264]−0.4400.660
Feelings/Cognitions (M1) → Uncontrolled Eating (Y)b10.4610.393 (0.136)[0.174; 0.712]2.8920.004
Indirect effect of X on Y via M1a1*b10.3910.631 (0.286)[0.256; 1.340]2.2090.027
Indirect effect of X on Y via M2a2*b2−0.038−0.061 (0.132)[−0.346; 0.178]−0.4580.647
Indirect effect of X on Y via M1 and M2a1*d*b20.3220.519 (0.130)[0.312; 0.827]3.986p < 0.001
Total effect of X on Y 0.5150.831 (0.116)[0.635; 1.083]7.193p < 0.001
Note: β* = standardized beta; β = unstandardized beta; SE = standard error; 95%CI = 95% confidence intervals (lower/upper) for the unstandardized beta; R2 = explained variance; p(…) = item parcel; FA symptoms = symptom count of the mYFAS 2; Feelings/Cognitions = Binge Eating Feelings/Cognitions subscale of BES; Emotional Eating = Emotional Eating scale of the TFEQ-18-R; Uncontrolled Eating = Uncontrolled Eating scale of the TFEQ-18-R.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Rossi, A.A. Tying Food Addiction to Uncontrolled Eating: The Roles of Eating-Related Thoughts and Emotional Eating. Nutrients 2025, 17, 369. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17030369

AMA Style

Rossi AA. Tying Food Addiction to Uncontrolled Eating: The Roles of Eating-Related Thoughts and Emotional Eating. Nutrients. 2025; 17(3):369. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17030369

Chicago/Turabian Style

Rossi, Alessandro Alberto. 2025. "Tying Food Addiction to Uncontrolled Eating: The Roles of Eating-Related Thoughts and Emotional Eating" Nutrients 17, no. 3: 369. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17030369

APA Style

Rossi, A. A. (2025). Tying Food Addiction to Uncontrolled Eating: The Roles of Eating-Related Thoughts and Emotional Eating. Nutrients, 17(3), 369. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17030369

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop