Functional Ego States, Behavior Patterns, and Social Interaction of Bulgarian Adolescents and Their Parents
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Models for Representing Personality Structure and Behavior in Transactional Analysis
1.2. Functional Ego States Model
1.3. Life Positions
1.4. Strokes and Stroke Economy
1.5. Family Environment and Parent-Adolescent Relationships
- Is there any difference in functional ego states, life positions, and strokes economy between the adolescents and their parents?
- Do ego states and life position predict stroke economies?
- Establishing the expression of functional ego states, life positions, and stroke economy in adolescents and their parents.
- Establishing differences between ego states, life positions, and stroke economy in adolescents and their parents.
- Examining the impact of ego states and life positions on the stroke economy in the adolescents and parents studied.
- In the adolescents and parents studied, Nurturing Parent ego state, Adult ego state and the life position “I am OK—you are OK” will be expressed. We also assume that in adolescents “Don’t give yourself” and “Don’t ask” will be more expressed and in parents ““Don’t ask“ strokes economy” will be more expressed.
- We allow for significant differences in functional ego states, life positions, and strokes economy between the adolescents and parents studied.
- We expect the significant predictors of strokes economy in parents and adolescents to be the Normative Parent and the Adapted Child ego states, the life position “I am not OK—you are OK” and “I am not OK—You are not OK”.
2. Methods
- (1)
- Self-assessment questionnaire for the study of functional ego states [19], adapted for Bulgarian conditions [44]. The questionnaire contains 25 statements and scores are on a five-level Likert-type scale (from 1 = “It is not true in general for me” to 5 = “It is completely true for me”). The Normative Parent ego state is measured by items such as “I impose my opinion in solving problems”, the Nurturing Parent is measured by items such as “I am dedicated to the needs of others” and the Adult ego state is measured by items such as “I consider things through before I act”. The Adapted Child scale includes statements such as “I easily agree with the opinion of others” and the Natural Child ego state is measured by items such as “I take time for my own needs”. Testing the reliability of the questionnaire using Cronbach’s alpha shows that the reliability coefficient for adolescents it is 0.81 and for parents is 0.85, indicating an acceptable internal consistency of the items for the purposes of the study.
- (2)
- A self-report life position questionnaire developed by Bekir and Tair [24] with 24 items. Scores are on a five-level Likert-type scale (from 1 = “It is not true in general for me” to 5 = “It is completely true for me”). The life position “I’m OK—you are OK” is measured by adjectives such as “respectful”, the position “I’m OK—you are not OK” includes adjectives such as “aggressive”, the life position “I’m not OK—you are OK” is measured by statements like “yielding” and “I’m not OK—you are not OK” position is measured by adjectives such as “dissatisfied”. The resulting Cronbach’s α reliability coefficient of the questionnaire for adolescents was 0.64 and for parents was 0.67, indicating acceptable psychometric properties and internal consistency of the items.
- (3)
- Self-report questionnaire for the study of stroke economy with 15 items [19], adapted for Bulgarian conditions [27]. Scores are on a five-level Likert-type scale (from 1 = “Totally disagree” to 5 = “Totally agree”). The “Don’t Accept” stroke economy is measured by statements such as “I feel uncomfortable when others express their thoughts about me”, the “Don’t Give Yourself” is measured by statements such as “I do not reward myself for my success” and the “Don’t Ask” stroke economy consists items such as “I don’t seek compliment for something when other people have helped me to do it”. The “Don’t Give” stroke economy is measured with items such as “If you praise someone, he/she stops making effort” and the “Don’t Reject” stroke economy is measured by items such as “Not accepting a compliment is a manifestation of lack of manners”. Testing the reliability of the questionnaire using Cronbach’s alpha indicates that the reliability coefficient for adolescents it is 0.58 and for parents is 0.76, indicating acceptable psychometric properties and internal consistency of the items.
Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics of Ego States
Ego States Differences between the Adolescents and Their Parents
3.2. Descriptive Statistics of Life Position
Life Positions Differences between the Adolescents and Their Parents
3.3. Descriptive Statistics of Stroke Economy
Stroke Economy’s Differences between the Adolescents and Their Parents
3.4. Results of Linear Regression Analyses
4. Discussion
Strengths and Limitations of the Study
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Brecard, F.; Hawkes, L. Big Book on Transactional Analysis; ARS Publishing House: Sofia, Bulgaria, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Bowlby, J. Attachment. In Attachment and Loss; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1969; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
- Collins, W.A.; Repinski, D.J. Parents and adolescents as transformers of relationships: Dyadic adaptations to developmental change. In Dynamics of Parenting: International Perspectives on Nature and Sources of Parenting; Gerris, J.R.M., Ed.; Garant: Leuven, The Netherlands, 2001; pp. 429–443. [Google Scholar]
- Erikson, E.H. Childhood and Society; W. W. Norton: New York, NY, USA, 1950. [Google Scholar]
- Holmbeck, G.N.; Paikoff, R.L.; Brooks-Gunn, J. Parenting adolescents. In Handbook of Parenting; Bornstein, M.H., Ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1995; pp. 91–118. [Google Scholar]
- Collins, W.A.; Steinberg, L. Adolescent Development in Interpersonal Context. In Handbook of Child Psychology, 6th ed.; Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., Lerner, R.M., Eds.; Social, Emotional, and Personality Development; Chapter 16; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2006; Volume 3, pp. 1003–1067. [Google Scholar]
- Noller, P. Parent-adolescent relationships. In Explaining Family Interactions; Fitzpatrick, M.A., Vangelisti, A., Eds.; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1995; pp. 77–111. [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg, L. We know some things: Adolescent-parent relationships in retrospect and prospect. J. Res. Adolesc. 2001, 11, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berne, E. Ego states in psychotherapy. Am. J. Psychother. 1957, 11, 293–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- žvelc, G. Between Self and Others: Relational Schemas as an Integrating Construct in Psychotherapy. Trans. Anal. J. 2009, 39, 22–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mortimer, J. Working and Growing up in America; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, B. Adolescents’ relationships with peers. In Handbook of Adolescent Psychology, 2nd ed.; Lerner, R., Steinberg, L., Eds.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2004; pp. 363–394. [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg, L.; Lamborn, S.D.; Darling, N.; Mounts, N.S.; Dornbusch, S.M. Over-Time Changes in Adjustment and Competence among Adolescents from Authoritative, Authoritarian, Indulgent, and Neglectful Families. Child Dev. 1994, 65, 754–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laursen, B.; Collins, W.A. Parent–Child Relationships During Adolescence. In Handbook of Adolescent Psychology; Richard, M.L., Laurencem, S., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2009; Chapter 1; pp. 3–42. [Google Scholar]
- Allen, J.P.; McElhaney, K.B.; Land, D.J.; Kuperminc, G.P.; Moore, C.W.; O’Beirne-Kelly, H.; Kilmer, S.L. A Secure Base in Adolescence: Markers of Attachment Security in the Mother–Adolescent Relationship. Child Dev. 2003, 74, 292–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ohlsson, T. Scientific evidence base for transactional analysis in the year 2010 Annex 1–the big list: References to transactional analysis research 1963–2010. Int. J. Trans. Anal. Res. 2010, 1, 12–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vos, J.; Van Rijn, B. A Systematic Review of Psychometric Transactional Analysis Instruments. Trans. Anal. J. 2021, 51, 127–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stewart, I.; Joines, V. TA Today: A New Introduction to Transactional Analysis; Lifespace Publishing: Nottingham and Chapel Hill, UK, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Hay, J. Transactional Analysis for Trainers; Sherwood Publishing: Hertford, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Levin, P. Ego States and Emotional Development in Adolescence. Trans. Anal. J. 2015, 45, 228–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levkova, I. Stsenarno Razvitie v Zrelostta; Faber Publishing House: Veliko Tarnovo, Bulgaria, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Kaygusuz, C.; Ozpolat, A.R. An analysis of university students’ levels of selfcontrol according to their ego states. Eurasian J. Educ. Res. 2015, 64, 197–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bekir, S. The ego states and “big-five” personality traits relationships. In Proceedings of the 21th International Scientific Conference “Applied Psychology–Possibilities and Perspectives”, Varna Free University “Chernorizets Hrabar”, Varna, Bulgaria, 1–3 July 2022; pp. 182–190. [Google Scholar]
- Bekir, S. Family environment and professional development: Personality predictors of subjective career success in parents of adolescents. In Proceedings of the 10th Jubilee International Scientific Conference “Applied Psychology—Possibilities and Perspectives”, Varna Free University “Chernorizets Hrabar”, Varna, Bulgaria, 25–27 June 2021; pp. 484–502. [Google Scholar]
- Ciucur, D. The Ego States and the “Big Five” Personality Factors. Procedia—Soc. Behav. Sci. 2013, 78, 581–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Steiner, C. Scripts People Live: Transactional Analysis of Life Scripts, 2nd ed.; Grove Press: New York, NY, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Bekir, S. Style of social relationships among 7th grade students. In Proceedings of the National Scientific Conference with International Participation “Science and Society 2019”, Kardzhali, Bulgaria, 2–3 October 2019; RKR Print Ltd. Publishing House: Kardzhali, Bulgaria, 2019; Volume 7, pp. 608–616. (In Bulgarian). [Google Scholar]
- Newell, S.; Jeffery, D. Behaviour Management in the Classroom. In A Transactional Analysis Approach; David Fulton Publishers: London, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Williams, A. Adolescents’ Relationships With Parents. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 22, 58–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gonzalez-De Hass, A.R.; Willems, P.P.; Doan Holbein, M.F. Examining the Relationship Between Parental Involvement and Student Motivation. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2005, 17, 99–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jodl, K.M.; Michael, A.; Malanchuk, O.; Eccles, J.E.; Sameroff, A. Parents’ Roles in Shaping Early Adolescents’ Occupational Aspirations. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 1247–1265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lee, B.; Porfeli, E.J. Youths’ socialization to work and school within the family. Int. J. Educ. Vocat. Guid. 2015, 15, 145–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Isley, S.L.; O’Neil, R.; Clatfelter, D.; Parke, R.D. Parent and child expressed affect and children’s social competence: Modeling direct and indirect pathways. Dev. Psychol. 1999, 35, 547–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eisenberg, N.; Cumberland, A.; Spinrad, T.L.; Fabes, R.A.; Shepard, S.A.; Reiser, M.; Murphy, B.C.; Losoya, S.H.; Guthrie, I.K. The relations of regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and internalizing problem behavior. Child Dev. Psychopathol. 2001, 72, 1112–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Putallaz, M. Maternal behavior and sociometric status. Child Dev. 1987, 58, 324–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carson, J.L.; Parke, R.D. Reciprocal negative affect in parent-child interactions and children’s peer competency. Child Dev. 1996, 67, 2217–2226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eisenberg, N.; Valiente, C.; Morris, A.S.; Fabes, R.A.; Cumberland, A.; Reiser, M.; Gershoff, E.T.; Shepard, S.A.; Losoya, S. Longitudinal relations among parental emotional expressivity, children’s regulation, and quality of socioemotional functioning. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 39, 3–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cassidy, J.; Parke, R.D.; Butkovsky, L.; Braungart, J.M. Family-peer connections: The roles of emotional expressiveness within the family and children’s understanding of emotions. Child Dev. 1992, 63, 603–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Belsky, J. Variation in susceptibility to rearing influences: An evolutionary argument. Psychol. Inq. 1997, 8, 182–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dimitrova, E.; Kotzeva, T.; Alexandrova-Karamanova, A. Psychosocial school environment and health risk behaviours of adolescents in Bulgaria: Results from multilevel analysis. Int. J. Public Health 2020, 65, 1331–1344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Branje, S.J.T.; Hale, W.W., III; Frijns, T.; Meeus, W.H.J. Longitudinal Associations Between Perceived Parent-Child Relationship Quality and Depressive Symptoms in Adolescence. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2010, 38, 751–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Saphir, M.N.; Chaffee, S.H. Adolescents’ contribution to family communication patterns. Hum. Commun. Res. 2002, 28, 86–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bekir, S.; Tair, E. Self-esteem of pre-secondary school students (grade 7): Ego states and parental education ifluences. Filos.–Philos. 2020, 29, 184–198. [Google Scholar]
- Bekir, S.; Tair, E. The Role of Ego States in Subjective Career Success in Different Occupations. Psychol. Res. 2019, 22, 799–814. [Google Scholar]
- Bekir, S.; Tair, E. Students—Teachers relationship style in school environment. Filos.—Philos. 2022, 31, 159–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ganeva, Z. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics; Elestra Publishing: Sofia, Bulgaria, 2016; ISBN 978-619-7292-01-5. [Google Scholar]
- Jackson, S. Statistics Plain and Simple, 4th ed.; Cengage Learning: Boston, MA, USA, 2006; ISBN 978-1305638907. [Google Scholar]
- Spss Inc. SPSS Version 13.0; SPSS Incorporated: Chicago, IL, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Morad, A.M.T. A Research Study into the impact on Emotional Stability of a Transactional Analysis Training Programme intended to develop increased levels of Adult Ego State in Adolescents in Syria. Int. J. Trans. Anal. Res. Pract. 2020, 11, 4–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Collins, W.A.; Laursen, B. Parent-Adolescent Relationships And Influences. In Handbook of Adolescent Psychology, 2nd ed.; Richard, M.L., Laurence, S., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2004; Chapter 11; pp. 331–361. ISBN 0-471-20948-1. [Google Scholar]
- Koepke, S.; Denissen, J.J. Dynamics of identity development and separation-individuation in parent—child relationships during adolescence and emerging adulthood—A conceptual integration. Dev. Rev. 2012, 32, 67–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Noack, P.; Buhl, H.M. Relations with parents and friends during adolescence and early adulthood. Marriage Fam. Rev. 2005, 36, 31–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bucx, F.; Van Wei, F. Parental bond and life course transitions from adolescence to young adulthood. Adolescence 2008, 43, 71–88. [Google Scholar]
- Englund, M.M.; Kuo, S.I.; Puig, J.; Collins, W.A. Early roots of adult competence: The significance of close relationships from infancy to early adulthood. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2011, 35, 490–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Shearer, C.L.; Crouter, A.C.; McHale, S.M. Parents’ perceptions of changes in mother–child and father–child relationships during adolescence. J. Adolesc. Res. 2005, 20, 662–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Dimensions | Respondents | N | Mean | SD | Skewness | Kurtosis | t-Test | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Normative Parent | Parents | 215 | 15.21 | 4.69 | −0.032 | −0.773 | 2.907 | 0.004 |
Adolescents | 215 | 13.97 | 4.11 | 0.305 | −0.494 | |||
Nurturing Parent | Parents | 215 | 20.31 | 3.30 | −0.752 | 0.200 | 3.822 | 0.000 |
Adolescents | 215 | 19.05 | 3.51 | −0.856 | 0.770 | |||
Adult | Parents | 215 | 20,73 | 3.34 | −0.812 | 0.399 | 6.880 | 0.000 |
Adolescents | 215 | 18.47 | 3.44 | −0.462 | −0.260 | |||
Adapted Child | Parents | 215 | 17.47 | 3.98 | −0.433 | −0.116 | 5.540 | 0.000 |
Adolescents | 215 | 15.37 | 3.87 | −0.032 | −0.601 | |||
Natural Child | Parents | 215 | 18.31 | 3.43 | −0.514 | −0.783 | −1.114 | 0.266 |
Adolescents | 215 | 18.67 | 3.24 | −0.501 | −0.058 |
Dimensions | Respondents | N | Mean | SD | Skewness | Kurtosis | t-Test | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
“I am OK—you are OK” | Parents | 183 | 24.98 | 3.56 | −1.313 | 0.917 | 5.108 | 0.000 |
Adolescents | 199 | 23.10 | 3.59 | −0.675 | 0.200 | |||
“I am OK—you are not OK” | Parents | 183 | 15.35 | 4.40 | 0.180 | −0.367 | 1.507 | 0.133 |
Adolescents | 199 | 14.70 | 3.98 | 0.150 | −0.475 | |||
“I am not OK—you are OK” | Parents | 183 | 16.58 | 3.97 | 0.294 | 0.457 | −0.690 | 0.491 |
Adolescents | 199 | 16.85 | 3.78 | 0.326 | −0.063 | |||
“I am not OK—you are not OK” | Parents | 183 | 12.42 | 4.39 | 0.805 | 0.711 | −2.727 | 0.007 |
Adolescents | 199 | 13.67 | 4.57 | 0.718 | 0.366 |
Dimensions | Respondents | N | Mean | SD | Skewness | Kurtosis | t-Test | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Don’t Give | Parents | 213 | 10.17 | 2.67 | −0.133 | −0.585 | 3.904 | 0.000 |
Adolescents | 215 | 9.19 | 2.54 | 0.195 | −0.413 | |||
Don’t Ask | Parents | 213 | 10.54 | 2.69 | −0.133 | −0.551 | 1.078 | 0.282 |
Adolescents | 215 | 10.27 | 2.48 | −0.194 | −0.310 | |||
Don’t Accept | Parents | 213 | 10.16 | 2.57 | −0.100 | −0.527 | 2.675 | 0.008 |
Adolescents | 215 | 9.50 | 2.55 | −0.386 | 0.087 | |||
Don’t Reject | Parents | 213 | 8.93 | 2.86 | 0.089 | −0.419 | 1.376 | 0.170 |
Adolescents | 215 | 8.57 | 2.61 | 0.041 | −0.255 | |||
Don’t Give Yourself | Parents | 213 | 10.03 | 2.85 | −0.163 | −0.439 | 0.253 | 0.800 |
Adolescents | 215 | 9.96 | 2.48 | −0.316 | −0.152 |
Dimensions | Don’t Give Yourself | Don’t Give | Don’t Reject | Don’t Ask | Don’t Accept | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | t | β | t | β | t | β | t | β | t | |
Normative Parent | 0.023 | 0.274 | 0.219 ** | 2.629 | −0.033 | −0.393 | −0.053 | −0.618 | 0.005 | 0.061 |
Nurturing Parent | −0.106 | −1.166 | 0.023 | 0.247 | −0.006 | −0.065 | 0.148 | 1.566 | 0.184 * | 1.933 |
Adult | 0.255 ** | 3.086 | 0.134 | 1.612 | −0.060 | −0.710 | 0.005 | 0.059 | 0.092 | 1.063 |
Adapted Child | 0.061 | 0.733 | 0.162 | 1.944 | 0.087 | 1.037 | 0.200 * | 2.320 | 0.007 | 0.084 |
Natural Child | 0.192 ** | 2.451 | 0.045 | 0.573 | 0.211 ** | 2.631 | 0.041 | 0.499 | 0.132 | 1.609 |
p | p < 0.001 | p < 0.001 | p < 0.001 | p < 0.01 | p ≤ 0.01 | |||||
R2 | 0.19 | 0.19 | 0.17 | 0.13 | 0.12 | |||||
AR2 | 0.15 | 0.15 | 0.13 | 0.09 | 0.07 | |||||
F | 4.859 | 4.657 | 4.067 | 2.977 | 2.691 |
Dimensions | Don’t Ask | Don’t Reject | Don’t Accept | Don’t Give | Don’t Give Yourself | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | t | β | t | β | t | β | t | β | t | |
Normative Parent | −0.209 * | −2.295 | 0.028 | 0.309 | −0.147 | −1.581 | 0.108 | 1.161 | −0.037 | −0.388 |
Nurturing Parent | 0.072 | 0.749 | 0.151 | 1.559 | 0.183 | 1.870 | 0.112 | 1.145 | 0.027 | 0.269 |
Adult | 0.081 | 0.945 | −0.018 | −0.204 | 0.025 | 0.278 | 0.011 | 0.130 | −0.043 | −0.472 |
Adapted Child | 0.306 *** | 3.310 | 0.147 | 1.571 | 0.199 * | 2.098 | 0.087 | 0.914 | 0.189 | 1.933 |
Natural Child | −0.044 | −0.516 | 0.216 * | 2.500 | −0.039 | −0.444 | −0.096 | −1.094 | −0.052 | −0.573 |
p | p ≤ 0.001 | p ≤ 0.01 | p ≤ 0.01 | p = 0.01 | p > 0.05 | |||||
R2 | 0.16 | 0.14 | 0.12 | 0.12 | 0.06 | |||||
AR2 | 0.12 | 0.09 | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.01 | |||||
F | 3.643 | 3.059 | 2.605 | 2.505 | 1.244 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bekir, S.; Tair, E. Functional Ego States, Behavior Patterns, and Social Interaction of Bulgarian Adolescents and Their Parents. Societies 2023, 13, 154. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc13070154
Bekir S, Tair E. Functional Ego States, Behavior Patterns, and Social Interaction of Bulgarian Adolescents and Their Parents. Societies. 2023; 13(7):154. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc13070154
Chicago/Turabian StyleBekir, Sezgin, and Ergyul Tair. 2023. "Functional Ego States, Behavior Patterns, and Social Interaction of Bulgarian Adolescents and Their Parents" Societies 13, no. 7: 154. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc13070154
APA StyleBekir, S., & Tair, E. (2023). Functional Ego States, Behavior Patterns, and Social Interaction of Bulgarian Adolescents and Their Parents. Societies, 13(7), 154. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc13070154