Expert Perspectives on the Performance of Explosive Detection Canines: Operational Requirements
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Study Design and Participants
2.2. Data Collection
2.3. Data Analysis
2.4. Security Review and Omitted Data
3. Results
3.1. Characteristics of Study Participants
3.2. EDC Utilization
3.3. EDC Team Performance Requirements
3.4. Physical Environment Requirements
- Indoor–residential, industrial, storage, educational, commercial, entertainment venues, sporting venues, places of worship, and transportation terminals;
- Outdoor–exterior of indoor environments, damaged manmade structures, culverts and ditches, vehicle paths and tunnels, pedestrian paths and tunnels, transportation areas, and access control points;
- Vehicles.
- Small passenger–motorcycles, sedans, pickup trucks, and vans;
- Commercial–semi-trucks with flatbed or enclosed trailers, maritime vessels, and cargo planes;
- Mass transit–buses, trains, passenger planes, and ferries.
3.5. Climate Environment Requirements
3.6. Operational Environment Requirements
- People (types)—bystanders, criminal suspects or detainees, familiar or unfamiliar supporting personnel, and healthy, injured, or deceased people;
- People (responses to EDC)—neutral, avoidant, anxious, and confrontational;
- Animals—domestic, feral, and wild;
- Sound—novel, sudden, sustained, loud, and high-pitched;
- Novel objects—rolling baggage, mobility devices (e.g., wheelchairs or walkers), and heavy machinery (e.g., forklifts);
- Odors—noxious (e.g., diesel fuel), distracting (e.g., food and animal urine), and similar to explosives (e.g., fertilizer).
3.7. Explosive Odor Environment Requirements
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Penn-Barwell, J.G.; Bennett, P.M.; Kay, A.; Sargeant, I.D.; Severe Lower Extremity Combat Trauma (SeLECT) Study Group. Acute bilateral leg amputation following combat injury in UK servicemen. Injury 2014, 45, 1105–1110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barzilai, L.; Harats, M.; Wiser, I.; Weissman, O.; Domniz, N.; Glassberg, E.; Stavrou, D.; Zilinsky, I.; Winkler, E.; Hiak, J. Characteristics of Improvised Explosive Device Trauma Casualties in the Gaza Strip and Other Combat Regions: The Israeli Experience. Wounds 2015, 27, 209–214. [Google Scholar]
- Ferreri, T.G.; Weir, A.J. EMS Improvised Explosive Devices and Terrorist Activity; StatPearls: Treasure Island, FL, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Harper, R.J.; Almirall, J.R.; Furton, K.G. Identification of dominant odor chemicals emanating from explosives for use in developing optimal training aid combinations and mimics for canine detection. Talanta 2005, 67, 313–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Furton, K. The scientific foundation and efficacy of the use of canines as chemical detectors for explosives. Talanta 2001, 487–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gazit, I.; Goldblatt, A.; Terkel, J. The role of context specificity in learning: The effects of training context on explosives detection in dogs. Anim. Cogn. 2005, 143–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Porritt, F.; Shapiro, M.; Waggoner, P.; Mitchell, E.; Thomson, T.; Nicklin, S.; Kacelnik, A. Performance decline by search dogs in repetitive tasks, and mitigation strategies. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 112–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lazarowski, L.; Waggoner, L.P.; Krichbaum, S.; Singletary, M.; Haney, P.; Rogers, B.; Angle, C. Selecting Dogs for Explosives Detection: Behavioral Characteristics. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020, 7, 597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jamieson, L.T.J.; Baxter, G.S.; Murray, P.J. Identifying suitable detection dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Johnen, D.; Heuwieser, W.; Fischer-Tenhagen, C. An approach to identify bias in scent detection dog testing. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swap, W.C.; Leonard, D.A.; Shields, M.; Abrams, L. Using Mentoring and Storytelling to Transfer Knowledge in the Workplace. Manag. Knowl. Assets Creat. Innov. 2011, 137–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wasonga, T.A.; Murphy, J.F. Learning from tacit knowledge: The impact of the internship. Int. J. Educ. Manag. 2006, 153–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farrar, N.; Trorey, G. Maxims, tacit knowledge and learning: Developing expertise in dry stone walling. J. Vocat. Educ. Train. 2008, 35–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polanyi, M.; Sen, A. The Tacit Dimension; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Nonaka, I. The Knowledge-Creating Company. Econ. Impact Knowl. 1998, 175–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lamberton, D. The Knowledge-based Economy: A Sisyphus Model. Prometheus 1997, 73–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boiral, O. Tacit Knowledge and Environmental Management. Long Range Plan. 2002, 291–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nunes, M.B.; Annansingh, F.; Eaglestone, B.; Wakefield, R. Knowledge management issues in knowledge-intensive SMEs. J. Doc. 2006, 101–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Johannessen, J.-A.; Olaisen, J.; Olsen, B. Mismanagement of tacit knowledge: The importance of tacit knowledge, the danger of information technology, and what to do about it. Int. J. Inf. Manag. 2001, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fricke, C.; Faust, B. Knowledge transfer acquiring implicit knowledge. Int. J. Nucl. Knowl. Manag. 2006, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mitchell, L.; Flin, R.; Yule, S.; Mitchell, J.; Coutts, K.; Youngson, G. Thinking ahead of the surgeon. An interview study to identify scrub nurses’ non-technical skills. Int. J. Nurs. Stud 2011, 48, 818–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Russano, M.B.; Narchet, F.M.; Kleinman, S.M.; Meissner, C.A. Structured Interviews of Experienced HUMINT Interrogators. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2014, 847–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Qualitative Data Analysis Software. Available online: https://www.qsrinternational.com/nvivo-qualitative-data-analysis-software/home (accessed on 4 March 2021).
- Deterding, N.M.; Waters, M.C. Flexible coding of in-depth interviews: A twenty-first-century approach. Sociol. Methods Res. 2018, 004912411879937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, T.G.; Burgman, M.A.; Fidler, F.; Kuhnert, P.M.; Low-Choy, S.; McBride, M.; Mengersen, K. Eliciting expert knowledge in conservation science. Conserv. Biol. 2012, 26, 29–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Abernethy, M.A.; Horne, M.; Lillis, A.M.; Malina, M.A.; Selto, F.H. A multi-method approach to building causal performance maps from expert knowledge. Manag. Account. Res. 2005, 135–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morse, J.M. Strategies for Sampling. Qualitative Nursing Research: A Contemporary Dialogue; Sage: Newbury Park, CA, USA, 1991; pp. 127–146. [Google Scholar]
- Lazarowski, L.; Krichbaum, S.; DeGreeff, L.E.; Simon, A.; Singletary, M.; Angle, C.; Waggoner, L. Methodological Considerations in Canine Olfactory Detection Research. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020, 7, 408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hayes, J.E.; McGreevy, P.D.; Forbes, S.L.; Laing, G.; Stuetz, R.M. Critical review of dog detection and the influences of physiology, training, and analytical methodologies. Talanta 2018, 185, 499–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gruen, M.E.; Case, B.C.; Foster, M.L.; Lazarowski, L.; Fish, R.E.; Landsberg, G.; DePuy, V.; Dorman, D.; Sherman, B. The use of an open-field model to assess sound-induced fear and anxiety-associated behaviors in Labrador retrievers. J. Vet. Behav. 2015, 338–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sherman, B.L.; Gruen, M.E.; Case, B.C.; Foster, M.L.; Fish, R.E.; Lazarowski, L.; DePuy, V.; Dorman, D. A test for the evaluation of emotional reactivity in Labrador retrievers used for explosives detection. J. Vet. Behav. 2015, 94–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rooney, N.J.; Gaines, S.A.; Bradshaw, J.W.S.; Penman, S. Validation of a method for assessing the ability of trainee specialist search dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 90–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lit, L.; Schweitzer, J.B.; Oberbauer, A.M. Handler beliefs affect scent detection dog outcomes. Anim. Cogn. 2011, 14, 387–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- DeChant, M.T.; Ford, C.; Hall, N.J. Effect of Handler Knowledge of the Detection Task on Canine Search Behavior and Performance. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020, 7, 250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duranton, C.; Gaunet, F. Canis sensitivus: Affiliation and dogs’ sensitivity to others’ behavior as the basis for synchronization with humans? J. Vet. Behav. 2015, 513–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jezierski, T.; Adamkiewicz, E.; Walczak, M.; Sobczyńska, M.; Górecka-Bruzda, A.; Ensminger, J.; Papet, E. Efficacy of drug detection by fully-trained police dogs varies by breed, training level, type of drug and search environment. Forensic. Sci. Int. 2014, 237, 112–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zubedat, S.; Aga-Mizrachi, S.; Cymerblit-Sabba, A.; Shwartz, J.; Leon, J.F.; Rozen, S.; Varkovitzky, I.; Eshed, Y.; Grinstein, D.; Avital, A. Human–animal interface: The effects of handler’s stress on the performance of canines in an explosive detection task. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 69–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rutter, N.J.; Howell, T.J.; Stukas, A.A.; Pascoe, J.H.; Bennett, P.C. Diving in Nose First: The Influence of Unfamiliar Search Scale and Environmental Context on the Search Performance of Volunteer Conservation Detection Dog-Handler Teams. Animals 2021, 11, 1177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Obama, B.B. Executive Order 13526: Classified National Security Information Memorandum. White House, Office of the Press Secretary. Available online: https://www.archives.gov/isoo/policy-documents/cnsi-eo.html?_ga=2.35794016.1133502615.1620783446-1620139500.1615685658 (accessed on 11 May 2021).
- U.S. Department of the Army. Army Regulation 530-1: Operations Security. 2014. Available online: https://armypubs.army.mil/epubs/DR_pubs/DR_a/pdf/web/r530_1.pdf (accessed on 11 May 2021).
- Rooney, N.J.; Bradshaw, J.W.S. Breed and sex differences in the behavioural attributes of specialist search dogs—A questionnaire survey of trainers and handlers. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004, 123–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Musa, M.; Isha, A.S.N. Holistic view of safety culture in aircraft ground handling: Integrating qualitative and quantitative methods with data triangulation. J. Air Transp. Manag. 2021, 102019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallivan, M.J. Value in Triangulation: A Comparison of Two Approaches for Combining Qualitative and Quantitative Methods. Inf. Syst. Qual. Res. 1997, 417–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
High probability clearance | Quote 1. Any time that they’re doing a building entry for like a SWAT (Special Weapons and Tactics) team, a lot of times we’ll have the dogs pre-deployed to make sure. Here we had [adverse event prior to building entry]. So now beforehand, if a department has a dog, they’ll deploy the dog to the outside of that building to clear for the threat of any kind of explosive devices before the SWAT team goes into the breach. That’s a very common one here. |
Low probability clearance | Quote 2. So, the difference in this one is that, if we’re going to do a facility sweep for dignitaries, then we evacuate the facility of personnel, so it’s a sensory free environment I should say, when it comes to mobile human stimulus or conversation stimulus. So, it’s usually quiet. It’s usually just the handler and the dog, and there can be somewhat of a playful environment in there. The dog just maneuvers through what is an otherwise dead space. |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
EDC team composition | Quote 3. Well, you never have a thing as just a dog. So, there’s a problem with this. It took us years to get non-dog people to think about, “Well I got my dog, the dogs are over here, we’re good.” It’s like, “No, you don’t have anything.” If you have a dog and you don’t have a qualified team, you don’t have anything. You have something that’s eating dog food. So, every dog is different, every handler is different. You have to ensure that the handler is properly qualified. He really knows what he’s doing with the dog. And then you’ve got to put the dog and the handler together and run assessments against that team. Once you say, “Okay, this team is qualified”, then you can say you have something. But until you get to that step, you don’t have anything. Or you have something, but you don’t know what you have. |
Physical fitness and energy | Quote 4. Now, I’m not asking for a spaz, but I want to see some energy and some physical ability. Yeah, so the perception of physical fitness, I guess. If I see these dogs that are just kind of coming in and just very lazily approaching the search, although I’ve seen them be successful from time to time, that doesn’t instill a lot of confidence in me if I were forming an opinion about a dog or a team. I guess that’s energy slash drive slash physical conditioning. |
Effective searching | Quote 5. Well, I think you’re seeing the drive of the dog. Is the dog actively searching? Is the dog searching in a systematic pattern? Is the dog searching productively? Unfortunately, it’s dictated often by the environment, but is the dog panting so you know that it’s not actively searching? Is the dog distracted by other things within the environment? I think all of those things would be important. |
Quote 6. I want to see a dog that can work an entire problem and not find anything, and then come back in and still be searching the same way. There’s no drop in their ability to want to go to work, do another search. | |
Quote 7. Probably the one that stands out the most is active sniffing behavior. I’ve seen a lot of dogs over the years and all that, there’s nuances and differences between them, but the basic is, that the dog has to have active sniffing behavior. Sometimes just by watching the dog, you can see whether or not the dog has drive for the task. | |
Quote 8. So, if it’s a desk, an office desk, if you will, there’s a lot of drawers, cabinets, whatever the case may be, the dog has to make sure that they’re sniffing all those drawer seams or cabinet door seams and all that to make sure that that’s where the odor’s going to be coming out of. The handler has to be observant in making sure the dog is hitting those productive areas. If he doesn’t, that’s where the handler has to come in and actually present with his hands, or her hands, those productive areas, those door seams or drawer seams or whatever the case may be to make sure the dog is actively sniffing in those areas and having the best opportunity to find that explosive odor in this case. | |
Quote 9. If I’m looking for large hides, like you’re looking for 50-pound explosives in the trunk of a vehicle, dogs can be moving pretty quickly, and they can be sort of air searching and questing. If you’re looking for explosives that are hidden in laptops, the dog can’t be running real fast. He has to be deep down, he has to be moving at a slower pace, he has to be at a high rate of inhalation. I mean a very detailed search for that target, and I can tell that the dog is fully investigating each target of opportunity. If he runs by a suitcase, and this depends on the size of the target zone, if you’re trying to clear packages in an airport and they’re running on a conveyor belt and the dog is walking along just sniffing bags as they go by, he’s not going to find something that’s really hidden well, unless he just lucks into it. Now, big stinky hide that’s been permeating through the containers, he can catch that going by. So, the search that I want to see in the dog needs to be relative to the target that we’re seeking. | |
Interaction with handler | Quote 10. They can be high drive and fast, but I like to see a dog that will take direction easily and work away from the handler very well, but still doesn’t rely on the handler. So, either on or off lead, it doesn’t change the way they work, you shouldn’t see a change. They shouldn’t be relying on the handler at all is one of the things that I look for. They shouldn’t check back in too much with the handler. So, if I’m watching a dog work, I don’t particularly like when they keep checking back in with the handler, or back to people looking for those cues. |
Handler requirements | Quote 11. It’s incumbent upon the individual handlers to ensure that they know their individual dogs. Not only in terms of how they respond necessarily to odor, but also the effectiveness of their dogs’ searching abilities. In other words, are there other factors that are leading these dogs to either perform well or perform poorly. So, are they tired? Have they been broken? Have they been fed? Have they been exercised? All those things go into it. And if the dog is lackadaisical and not showing any sort of interest at all in the searching activity, is there something medically wrong with the dog? That sort of thing. So, all those factors build into the dog’s ability to perform the searching task... The idea being the handlers, especially in these repetitive tasks, the handlers know what the clues are that the dog is interested in the search. |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Required to search nearly everywhere | Quote 12. It’s any type of building to include warehouses, barracks. They’ve got to be able to search vehicles. They’ve got to be able to search roadways, open areas. Really, the only areas that we don’t want them to get used to search in are places that would be hazardous to the dog, i.e., some sort of chemical environment or we’re not going to run a dog through some toxic sludge heap, something along those lines. If you got a ton of raw sewage laying around. Something that could make the dog sick potentially or do damage to it short and long term, that’s what we stay out of, but yeah, just something that’s unsafe to the dog is really the only limit that we have on that. |
Specific locations | Quote 13. Basically, anything and everything is in play for that dog to deploy in. So, it’s just like basically any type of environment, that dog should be able to function in. I live in the mountains of (U.S. state). I’m looking at the side of a mountain right now. How many, actually outside of probably the military in Afghanistan back in the day in the mountainous regions, can work a steep grade? And how many dogs that can’t work a large grassy area without taking a piss on something? Because they have to be able to identify odor can come from any environment, not just particular ones like cars. Like they strictly do cars. Some of these private companies, all they do is cars. And then I will hear them say, “He just does cars; he can’t do rooms.” Why not? See what I’m saying? It is broad. |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Required to search in nearly every climate | Quote 14. I cannot picture any environments where we are not already asking dogs to work. We have dogs working outside in the winter here in various parts of the United States where it is cold. Then we’ve got them working in the swamps in Florida or in Miami, I guess, not the physical swamps but the high humidity, high temperature in Miami to the dry heat out West. I think we are already deploying them in all weather conditions from one extreme to the next. |
Role of handler and leadership | Quote 15. Just because the weather is ugly, it doesn’t mean that you’re not going to have to carry on with that search. I think you need to take into consideration both as a handler and as a program manager, whoever has control of that operational environment, to look at different factors and plan for that humidity and heat. |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Required to settle and refocus | Quote 16. Some of us can work well with stress, some of us might be overwhelmed with whatever it is we’re feeling anxious about. It’s going to have some effect. Whether it has enough effect so the dog can’t overcome it, that’s what we want to determine. When we’re looking at dogs in our selection process, we’re putting that stress on them. We’re putting those surprises. I don’t mind the dog basically startling, but what I’m doing when I’m testing it, I’m intentionally startling the dog, but then I’m trying to see how long it takes this dog to appear to overcome it. |
Manage tactical demands | Quote 17. When deployed overseas, you actually worry about somebody trying to blow you up any second. Not only are you having to work the dog, but you’re having to watch other vehicles in the area. You should have overwatch, and that’s the one nice thing about working in the military type environment, you generally have overwatch, whereas in law enforcement a lot of times you don’t. You have to worry about the people and what’s going on around you. Personal threats to the handler and the dog. Who’s the good guy, who’s the bad guy that I may have trouble with here in just a second? |
Appropriate acclimation | Quote 18. If you have a really good training program, a really good setup, gunfire doesn’t bother him, loud explosives don’t bother him, people don’t bother him. They stay focused. You’ve got to have that experience within your training environment, so it becomes normal. It doesn’t shock the dog. The dog just ignores everything. |
Criminal apprehension or assault training or utilization | Quote 19. Just getting your dog out of your car and going to the building, the majority of these dual purpose dogs think they’re looking for the man. Just because that’s the majority of their deployments. They’re going to alarm calls, they’re going to home invasions, burglary calls, or whatever. So, their natural instincts, until the handler can get it focused on detection, is looking for the human element, not the explosive element. |
Quote 20. If you do a call out at a house or a hit, the dog’s in bite mode which is very stimulating, and he’s looking to bite somebody. You have to say, “Let’s check these doors, let’s check these gates before we move in here, let’s clear this area so we are safe to move up.” Then you go in, and you arrest everybody, and you call them out or whatever you do, and then you say, “Okay, now we have to search for explosives again.” That switch is very difficult for the dog, to go from bite drive to explosive detection. |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Discrimination and generalization | Quote 21. The dog’s got to be discriminate that he’s only going to alert on certain targets, but then be broad enough that he’s only going to alert on the specific precursor odor that you train on. It comes down to differences in the targets that you’re training for and the ones you’re finding. So, the dog’s ability has to be broad enough that he’s going to hit on a wide variety of ammonium nitrates, for instance, but discriminate enough that he’s not going to hit on the stuff that you’re not interested in. |
Odor locations | Quote 22. You had to train these dogs that they could detect the odor in the ground. These dogs initially had problems with detection of the odor in the ground. And sometimes they would have problems with high odors, like if it was up in a tree. But, once you started training them and you started acclimating them and got them to understand, “Hey, there might be a very high odor, and I may not be able to reach it, but I’m going to get to a certain point and I’m going to respond.” With buried odor they would get extremely confused and then they would finally get it and they’d say, “Oh, okay, I got it. The odor’s in the ground and this is where it’s primarily coming from.” |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Farr, B.D.; Otto, C.M.; Szymczak, J.E. Expert Perspectives on the Performance of Explosive Detection Canines: Operational Requirements. Animals 2021, 11, 1976. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11071976
Farr BD, Otto CM, Szymczak JE. Expert Perspectives on the Performance of Explosive Detection Canines: Operational Requirements. Animals. 2021; 11(7):1976. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11071976
Chicago/Turabian StyleFarr, Brian D., Cynthia M. Otto, and Julia E. Szymczak. 2021. "Expert Perspectives on the Performance of Explosive Detection Canines: Operational Requirements" Animals 11, no. 7: 1976. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11071976
APA StyleFarr, B. D., Otto, C. M., & Szymczak, J. E. (2021). Expert Perspectives on the Performance of Explosive Detection Canines: Operational Requirements. Animals, 11(7), 1976. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11071976