Expert Perspectives on the Performance of Explosive Detection Canines: Performance Degrading Factors
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Study Design and Participants
2.2. Data Collection
2.3. Data Analysis
2.4. Security Review and Omitted Data
3. Results
3.1. Characteristics of Study Participants
3.2. The Context of EDC Utilization: Degrees of Control
3.3. EDC Team Factors—Handler Behavior
3.4. EDC Team Factors—Physical Limitations of the EDC
3.5. Physical Environmental Factors
3.5.1. Manmade Physical Environment Factors
Manmade Surfaces
Confined Spaces
Elevated Areas
Novel or Noxious Equipment
Light
Manmade Airflow
3.5.2. Natural Physical Environment Factors
Natural Surfaces
Subterranean Areas
Natural Airflow
3.6. Climate Environmental Factors
3.7. Operational Environmental Factors
3.8. Explosive Odor Environmental Factors
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Penn-Barwell, J.G.; Bennett, P.M.; Kay, A.; Sargeant, I.D. Severe Lower Extremity Combat Trauma (SeLECT) Study Group. Acute bilateral leg amputation following combat injury in UK servicemen. Injury 2014, 45, 1105–1110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barzilai, L.; Harats, M.; Wiser, I.; Weissman, O.; Domniz, N.; Glassberg, E.; Stavrou, D.; Zilinsky, I.; Winkler, E.; Hiak, J. Characteristics of Improvised Explosive Device Trauma Casualties in the Gaza Strip and Other Combat Regions: The Israeli Experience. Wounds 2015, 27, 209–214. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Ferreri, T.G.; Weir, A.J. EMS Improvised Explosive Devices and Terrorist Activity; StatPearls Publishing: Treasure Island, FL, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Harper, R.J.; Almirall, J.R.; Furton, K.G. Identification of dominant odor chemicals emanating from explosives for use in developing optimal training aid combinations and mimics for canine detection. Talanta 2005, 67, 313–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Furton, K.G.; Myers, L.J. The scientific foundation and efficacy of the use of canines as chemical detectors for explosives. Talanta 2001, 54, 487–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farr, B.F.; Otto, C.M.; Szymczak, J.E. Expert Perspectives on the Performance of Explosive Detection Canines: Operational Requirements. Animals 2021, 11, 1976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gazit, I.; Terkel, J. Explosives detection by sniffer dogs following strenuous physical activity. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2003, 149–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayes, J.E.; McGreevy, P.D.; Forbes, S.L.; Laing, G.; Stuetz, R.M. Critical review of dog detection and the influences of physiology, training, and analytical methodologies. Talanta 2018, 185, 499–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lazarowski, L.; Waggoner, L.P.; Krichbaum, S.; Singletary, M.; Haney, P.S.; Rogers, B.; Angle, C. Selecting Dogs for Explosives Detection: Behavioral Characteristics. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020, 7, 597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnen, D.; Heuwieser, W.; Fischer-Tenhagen, C. An approach to identify bias in scent detection dog testing. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swap, W.C.; Leonard, D.A.; Shields, M.; Abrams, L. Using Mentoring and Storytelling to Transfer Knowledge in the Workplace. J. Manag. Inf. Syst. 2011, 18, 137–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wasonga, T.A.; Murphy, J.F. Learning from tacit knowledge: The impact of the internship. Int. J. Educ. Manag. 2006, 20, 153–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Russano, M.B.; Narchet, F.M.; Kleinman, S.M.; Meissner, C.A. Structured Interviews of Experienced HUMINT Interrogators. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2014, 28, 847–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farrar, N.; Trorey, G. Maxims, tacit knowledge and learning: Developing expertise in dry stone walling. J. Vocat. Educ. Train. 2008, 60, 35–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Obama, B.B. Executive Order 13526: Classified National Security Information Memorandum; Office of the Press Secretary: White House. 2009. Available online: https://www.archives.gov/isoo/policy-documents/cnsi-eo.html?_ga=2.35794016.1133502615.1620783446-1620139500.1615685658 (accessed on 11 May 2021).
- Qualitative Data Analysis Software. Available online: https://www.qsrinternational.com/nvivo-qualitative-data-analysis-software/home (accessed on 4 March 2021).
- Deterding, N.M.; Waters, M.C. Flexible coding of in-depth interviews: A twenty-first-century approach. Sociol. Methods Res. 2021, 50, 708–739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mitchell, L.; Flin, R.; Yule, S.; Mitchell, J.; Coutts, K.; Youngson, G. Thinking ahead of the surgeon. An interview study to identify scrub nurses’ non-technical skills. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 2011, 48, 818–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Porritt, F.; Shapiro, M.; Waggoner, P.; Mitchell, E.; Thomson, T.; Nicklin, S.; Kacelnik, A. Performance decline by search dogs in repetitive tasks, and mitigation strategies. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 166, 112–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gazit, I.; Goldblatt, A.; Terkel, J. The role of context specificity in learning: The effects of training context on explosives detection in dogs. Anim. Cogn. 2005, 8, 143–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rutter, N.J.; Howell, T.J.; Stukas, A.A.; Pascoe, J.H.; Bennett, P.C. Diving in Nose First: The Influence of Unfamiliar Search Scale and Environmental Context on the Search Performance of Volunteer Conservation Detection Dog–Handler Teams. Animals 2021, 11, 1177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snigdha, S.; Christie, L.-A.; De Rivera, C.; Araujo, J.A.; Milgram, N.W.; Cotman, C.W. Age and distraction are determinants of performance on a novel visual search task in aged Beagle dogs. Age 2012, 34, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lopedote, M.; Valentini, S.; Musella, V.; Vilar, J.M.; Spinella, G. Changes in Pulse Rate, Respiratory Rate and Rectal Temperature in Working Dogs before and after Three Different Field Trials. Animals 2020, 10, 733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baker, J.; DeChant, M.; Jenkins, E.; Moore, G.; Kelsey, K.; Perry, E. Body Temperature Responses During Phases of Work in Human Remains Detection Dogs Undergoing a Simulated Deployment. Animals 2020, 10, 673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Osterkamp, T. Detector Dogs and Scent Movement: How Weather, Terrain, and Vegetation Influence Search Strategies; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Niedermeyer, G.M.; Hare, E.; Brunker, L.K.; Berk, R.A.; Kelsey, K.M.; Darling, T.A.; Nord, J.L.; Schmidt, K.K.; Otto, C.M. A Randomized Cross-Over Field Study of Pre-Hydration Strategies in Dogs Tracking in Hot Environments. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020, 7, 292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zanghi, B.M.; Robbins, P.J.; Ramos, M.T.; Otto, C.M. Working Dogs Drinking a Nutrient-Enriched Water Maintain Cooler Body Temperature and Improved Pulse Rate Recovery After Exercise. Front. Vet. Sci. 2018, 5, 202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diverio, S.; Menchetti, L.; Riggio, G.; Azzari, C.; Iaboni, M.; Zasso, R.; Di Mari, W.; Santoro, M.M. Dogs’ coping styles and dog-handler relationships influence avalanche search team performance. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 67–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haverbeke, A.; Laporte, B.; Depiereux, E.; Giffroy, J.-M.; Diederich, C. Training methods of military dog handlers and their effects on the team’s performances. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 110–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lasseter, A.E.; Jacobi, K.P.; Farley, R.; Hensel, L. Cadaver dog and handler team capabilities in the recovery of buried human remains in the southeastern United States. J. Forensic. Sci. 2003, 48, 617–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morisaki, A.; Takaoka, A.; Fujita, K. Are dogs sensitive to the emotional state of humans? J. Vet. Behav. 2009, 4, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lit, L.; Schweitzer, J.B.; Oberbauer, A.M. Handler beliefs affect scent detection dog outcomes. Anim. Cogn. 2011, 14, 387–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schwab, C.; Huber, L. Obey or not obey? Dogs (Canis familiaris) behave differently in response to attentional states of their owners. J. Comp. Psychol. 2006, 120, 169–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horn, L.; Huber, L.; Range, F. The importance of the secure base effect for domestic dogs-evidence from a manipulative problem-solving task. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e65296. [Google Scholar]
- Lazarowski, L.; Rogers, B.; Krichbaum, S.; Haney, P.; Smith, J.G.; Waggoner, P. Validation of a Behavior Test for Predicting Puppies’ Suitability as Detection Dogs. Animals 2021, 11, 993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Salassa, J.R.; Zapala, D.A. Love and fear of heights: The pathophysiology and psychology of height inbalance. Wilderness Env. Med. 2009, 20, 378–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baumann, T.K.; Burchiel, K.J.; Ingram, S.L.; Martenson, M.E. Responses of adult human dorsal root ganglion neurons in culture to capsaicin and low pH. Pain 1996, 31–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Highly Controlled Operations | Quote 1. Yeah, we have the luxury of, and I say it, we’re a little bit bully-ish about it if you will. If we’re going to sweep something, we clear it prior to starting the sweep. And that’s for a couple reasons, not the least of which is safety for all people involved, even the handler, random people walking by, employees, customers, whatever. We’re going to clear those locations, or those vehicles, or the area prior to the actual sweep occurring. And we’re able to accomplish that with posted [personnel], police officers, [specialized personnel], that sort of thing. |
Moderately Controlled Operations | Quote 2. Some of the environments, let’s say, for instance at (chain home improvement store). Not too long ago we had a bomb call there with one of my teams, and they had to search the whole parking lot which entailed probably 200–300 people, communicating, yelling, screaming around the area. On top of that they had cars with all forms of odors inside of them from narcotic odor to human odor to chemical odor, fertilizers. |
Low Controlled Operations | Quote 3. You’re generally moving faster. There’s a lot of yelling. Once the breach team has executed its mission, everything gets loud. And there could be gunfire. There could be flashbangs. There could be all of these things as the team moves through, and those are all stimuli on the dog. |
Criminal Apprehension or Assault Training or Utilization | Quote 4. Most of the law enforcement dogs have learned when they hear the siren that’s the, “Oh boy. I get to go get into some shit.” And it never fails, you’ll be trying to work, and you got four rookies that are still on their way there, so they come screaming up to the scene, with the siren still blasting, and at that point the dog’s ears perk up, he turns and looks at him and is like, “I want to go over that way, because obviously something good’s going on over there.” |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Handler Behavior | Quote 5. For me, the handler affects the dog more than anything. You know a lot of times what I see is that these departments are getting handlers that want a dog just because it sounds cool to have a dog. And the handler education is the biggest for me. The more knowledge a handler has outside of the leash, starts understanding animal behavior, starts understanding all the different systems of training, and not just saying “Okay, I’m only holding a leash on a bomb dog,” the more success you’ll have as a team. |
Quote 6. If the handler isn’t enjoying the work or isn’t rewarding enough or appropriately, or let’s flip that coin, is punishing too aggressively. All of those things impact the mental welfare of the dog. | |
Quote 7. Depending on the dog, the handler may be too involved. The dog may be fine, but the handler is too involved, depending on the history. If it’s a newer handler, newer dog, they haven’t learned each other, they haven’t associated whatever type of cues they have with each other, they don’t have a baseline, then that’s where things could get choppy. | |
Quote 8. I call it the Groundhog Day effect. Where you continually have to do pre-sweeps at the same location multiple times, sometimes two, three times a week, and things like that. After a while there’s a malaise that sets in. The handler does not interact with the dog as frequently as he normally does. Dogs succumb to this easily. I’ve literally had to search something and 12 h later have to do it again. Like we maybe have a college game on Saturday and then once it was over, we’d have to come in and do the same thing again in less than 12 h later for the next day, Sunday, when the NFL came in and took over for their game. And then the human factor kicks in. Well, I just did this and now I’m doing it again. I know there’s nothing here. So I’m just going to hit the high points so to speak. And then get done with that and then a half hour later get that call out for a bomb threat where they actually found a precursor or something or an item that could be utilized in the manufacture of something. Not so much odor, but maybe batteries and wires. And then change how the handler feels about it, and then all of the sudden they become overly involved in their dog’s search. | |
Quote 9. If the handler isn’t engaged and isn’t observant of the dog on where that dog is sniffing and if they’re actively sniffing, then that could ensure the dog’s either success or failure, depending on the particular situation. But that would be one factor. If the handler, not necessarily is lazy, just isn’t observant. So, I’ve talked about the lazy piece of it, but if they’re not observant, if they’re not experienced enough, don’t have that know-how to understand, to see their dog’s changed behavior. | |
Inappropriate Interaction with Handler | Quote 10. A lot of times for me what I see is dogs that are not successful lack the overall focus to task, focus to job, oftentimes lack of desire to search, or that that dog needs too much help from the handler. It’s not independent enough in its searching. That to me is a big sign if the handler always has to step in and help the dog. |
Quote 11. If he’s always fighting with the handler, you want him to go forward and he wants to go backward, you want him to go forward, and he goes left or right. That’s a dog that’s not working. It’s a dog that wants to do his own thing and it’s probably not something that we want in an explosives dog. |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Physical Limitations of the EDC | Quote 12. And then the dog has to be in good health. It’s got to be conditioned. If it’s not conditioned and in good health, you’ll get a product that will work okay for a short time, a very short time. Maybe 15 min, 20 min optimal. But after that, it will begin fading very quickly after that. |
Quote 13. Well case in point, I was watching this Lab working. I watched him go up high under a bunk bed to search, and I watched as the left rear foot rolled onto the back of the dog’s leg, and he stood on it while he was up and searching, which to me, was a telltale sign of a neurological issue, spondylosis or something like that. But then I noticed that he kind of rolled off of this search and every time he went high, he would go up and he’d immediately come back down but wouldn’t maintain it like a dog that was physically in good condition. Ultimately, we found out that’s what it was. There was actually a spondylosis issue. But the dog had a comfort issue that the handler wasn’t of that experience level that he knew that it was occurring. |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Manmade Surfaces | Quote 14. Anything that affects their footing. The sensation through the pads on their footing. Case in point is I’ve had to do some larger ships back in the day where I worked because we had a port. The grating on some of the decks and the steps provided a challenge because of the tactile feeling to the dog through their feet. There are some environments like that even the strongest dogs will sometimes shy away from. |
Specific Locations | Quote 13. Basically, anything and everything is in play for that dog to deploy in. So, it’s just like basically any type of environment, that dog should be able to function in. I live in the mountains of (U.S. state). I’m looking at the side of a mountain right now. How many, actually outside of probably the military in Afghanistan back in the day in the mountainous regions can work a steep grade? And how many dogs that can’t work a large grassy area without taking a piss on something? Because they have to be able to identify odor can come from any environment, not just particular ones like cars. Like they strictly do cars. Some of these private companies, all they do is cars. And then I will hear them say he just does cars; he can’t do rooms. Why not? See what I’m saying? It is broad. |
Confined Spaces | Quote 15. I would say one of the challenges is culverts, small types of tunnels. That is a challenge where it is one way down and it’s one way out. Did he go out, but you have to come backwards and there’s no room for turning around. Same thing with narrow passages in whatever building where it’s a long hallway, not much room. Same thing with aircraft, the restrooms in the aircraft, the cockpit. |
Quote 16. So again, if the training hasn’t happened or occurred for that dog to be in every single type of environment, lay down, and even though we typically teach the dog to sit for the response when they’re on odor or on source, if you teach the dog to lay down underneath an undercarriage and that’s their response because they just can’t sit, obviously, then the dog is going to fail if you don’t put them in those type of situations because the dog is not going to be comfortable in those type of scenarios. So, it’s all about getting the dog comfortable in certain environments. | |
Elevated Areas | Quote 17. Depending where you’re at, the elevation of something. That is a big factor. If you had to go search the top of something, the roof. Been there. That can affect the dog in the sense of what can be seen. In the sense of it’s glass, if you’re just high up on a high office and depending on the flooring, if it’s something that they could see down, let’s just say. This may be glass bottom like a patio or an offset of something where it’s enclosed with either glass or just your classic bars. The dog could see down and all of a sudden may show fear response. |
Novel or Noxious Equipment | Quote 18. It was the top floors of, for instance, stadiums or big HVAC units where there is a bunch of HVAC equipment with the … what is it … all the ventilation, the pipes and all that in small, confined areas, kind of dark… |
Quote 19. You come around there and unintentionally you catch the exhaust of a generator or anything like that, that dog is going to shy back from that heat as soon as it hits his olfactory. That’s just automatically. It’s like putting your hand on the stove right when it’s been shut off. You’re going to pull back immediately, and dogs will do the same thing too. | |
Light | Quote 20. Yeah, working in low light because if you only work in light conditions, that’s what the dog is accustomed to. And then you work them in a dark environment, sometimes you’ll have a bit of a search drop off. It’s the transition and not being acclimated to it properly and making it more a part of their search environment. |
Manmade Airflow | Quote 21. I would say in an environment where the air flow is uncontrollable. It could be a subway system. When a train pulls in, it pushes the air out. When a train leaves, it sucks the air with it. We’ve done a lot of stuff in mass transit, aviation, where the air flow in a terminal is just insanely brutal. Again, that’s a manmade structure, but because it’s climate controlled, a source might be in the right side of the building, but there’s no odor there because the odor’s pulling it to the left side of the building. You could be right on top of it and the air could be being sucked out. The bad guy could put his backpack near an intake of an air conditioning system. You put your nose right on it, no odors there because the air’s being sucked in through the ventilation system and it’s on the other side of the room. |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Natural Surfaces | Quote 22. I think one that’s really tough for us is the one overseas in the desert. It was hard on the dogs as far as even the sand was hard on the dogs’ feet because it would splay their tendons of their paw out when they’re walking, which is not what they’re used to, the soil here in the United States, and they’d come up lame within a couple of days. |
Subterranean Areas | Quote 23. Yeah, a novel environment might be the best way to capture it. I guess to give you an example is that we started trying to do some subterranean efforts and some of the dogs would struggle initially because they had never been in that type of environment. |
Natural Airflow | Quote 24. Again, the surfaces, vegetation is a huge thing. When you’re doing landmines, it’s extremely difficult if you’re working in very tall vegetation because if you cannot see your dog, you can’t send them out, but also, you’ve got dead zones within the tall vegetation. It changes the wind current. A lot of the natural stuff, vegetation, terrain, ditches, you have dead zones. That almost got me in [African country]. They have the ponds. The vehicles would drive through and create divots and if you didn’t have your dog investigate it properly, you could miss landmines in that. I’ve seen that happen twice in [African country]. Like a pothole is enough to … Yeah, because it’s actually a dead zone within that pothole. You’ve got to ensure the dog actually sticks his head down into the lowest part of the pothole. A handler has to identify certain targets within that scope. If you’re doing road clearances, you do a lot of air scenting, you send your dog out, and you want your dog utilizing the wind, but you got to also be aware that when you come onto divots and cracks on the roads and stuff, those are dead zones. A lot of times you can actually have a dog physically go over and check within that crater or whatever. |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Altered Handler Demeanor | Quote 25. The dogs don’t seem to do as bad as the people do, but I think the people affect the dogs. The dog’s hooked to the leash with the guy that’s basically not happy to be there, and that has an effect more so on the dog than the actual weather itself. I’ve been in situations where it was raining, and it was cold, and the dogs appeared to be cold as well, but a lot of times it’s that behavior that runs down the leash to the dog. |
Effect of Heat and Humidity | Quote 26. No matter how upbeat the handler is and how ready to go the dog is, if it’s 130 degrees or even if it’s 89 degrees with 100% humidity, you can only go so long. Endurance goes way down, duration of workable time the dog can actively search goes way down. That high respiration, that inhalation, to keep that up real high, they need to be in very good shape, and I think no matter how good of shape they’re in, it gets so hot that it just saps the energy out of them. You can keep the dog moving, but in some of them, he’s just moving, he’s not searching anymore. The useful operational time or the useful search time of the dog goes way down, they need more breaks. |
Requirement for Acclimation | Quote 27. You need to prepare the dogs for two to four weeks once you get the dogs into a hot environment, if you want to have them working optimally. And it doesn’t mean that you can’t work a dog right out of the box. You can take a dog from the United States that is on the East Coast and deploy that dog into Afghanistan or Iraq in 120-degree heat, you can put that dog immediately to work. But you’re not going to get optimal capability out of that dog if you don’t acclimate it first. |
Novel Climates | Quote 28. The thing is you don’t know. You could take a dog and put them in the snow for the first time ever and the dog is so keen on working that they just work through those environmentals and just do their job. And, then you have another dog that comes out there that’s never seen snow and they’re eating the snow. It’s just too distracting. So, putting them in those situations ahead of time prepares you for the day that operationally it’s pouring rain, and someone goes, “We need you to go out and search those cars,” and you go, “Well, we have never searched in the rain.” You get your dog out in the rain and go to search, they’re probably going to look at you like, “I don’t know what you want me to do because we don’t do this.” |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Repetition | Quote 29. The hardest part is keeping the dogs’ focus and motivation. It’s only going to be natural. “Dude, we’ve already run here before, there’s nothing here, there’s still nothing here.” If you repeatedly do the same thing and not present something new to the dog, a new environment to the dog even if you’re at the same place, you’re going to get less and less real focus to task unless you just have an absolute exceptional dog. |
Other Animals | Quote 30. Our biggest problem was not the capability to detect explosive ordinance or explosive materials, it was the environment that allowed stray dogs to enter into the dog’s working environment. So now you’ve got a stray dog, and the dog sees a play buddy that he’s never seen before and gone. That was the one thing that drew that dog away instantly. And if we had a male dog, and that was a female dog in heat, gone. We lost all capability. |
Personnel | Quote 31. External stimuli in the form of people is kind of a wildcard. When you’re around large crowds and specifically sporting events where there’s alcohol involved, people lose their good common sense. People can make eye contact with the dog and is that dog going to be aggressive, is it going to be skittish? Are they going to move in a manner that that dog feels threatened and distract the dog from the task at hand? Is the person handicapped and they wave their arms, or they walk differently than somebody else? The dog picks up on that anomaly. |
Food Sources | Quote 32. For a lot of dogs, if there are easily accessible sources of food, that’s a problem. That is an instinct that’s hard to completely train away. As long as they are appropriately conditioned, I’m not so worried about the moving trucks or the moving people and the sounds and sights of that environment. The instinct to hunt, to eat, and to reproduce, those will be a challenge. |
Sound | Quote 33. The raid environment is intense, noisy, often in the dark. So, there’s a lot of sensory things going on around the dog that can take the dog’s focus away from the mission at hand. Because they’re feeding off of us. We’re excited. We’re emotional. The dogs are reading that. |
Transportation | Quote 34. A lot of departments are using air transport for the canine team. You’re physically taking that dog from point A to point B, dropping it in an environment with the expectation that that dog is going to be able to perform at a proficient level for an operational search. In my experience, about 50% of them wouldn’t find anything. They were just so stressed. |
Criminal Apprehension or Assault Training or Utilization | Quote 35. If I’ve got a dual purpose dog out, I think the biggest thing to worry about is other people in the area, because since he has also learned it’s okay to bite people, and to watch bad guys, any time you put a strange person into his environment suddenly, he’s going to be overly suspicious and want to pay attention to that. |
Anticipation | Quote 36. I’ve been on raids before where there’s no indication that there would be an explosive in there, but you have the dog in the event you run into something. So sometimes, the dog may not leave the kennel in the back of the HMMWV. But the dog’s as exhausted as everybody else is, just because of all the stimuli. But if you had to take the dog out, everybody is excited, and the dog is reading that. And the dog, depending on how long that raid took and where you are in the phase of the raid, might already be tired just because of the stimuli. |
Interview Themes | Exemplar Quotations |
---|---|
Threshold | Quote 37. We do not train for operational relevance. We train for evaluation standards. If our canine community trained with operational hides, that produces a different scent picture, and it gives the dog a different threshold, we’d be much more effective at finding devices. We don’t know if we’re finding them, because we’re probably missing a lot. When’s the last time someone caught a bad guy with a quarter pound of black powder in a car door seam? Never. Now I get the need for search patterns, but why aren’t we training like that once these dogs graduate and they’re on the road for six months or a year? They should immediately go into threat devices. Put your training aids inside those pipe bombs, radios, human vests, laptops, put them in something because that’s what the bad guy’s going to do. |
Quote 38. They’re not going to put a block of C-4 in a vehicle door hinge or behind a headlight assembly. We continually do that, and it’s very concerning. It’s not just the dog that’s used to it. The handlers are used to seeing this massive change. As an evaluator, I’m 15 feet away, and I’m seeing dogs stop and change behavior real quick at source. Well, the handler doesn’t see that because they’re at four feet maybe six feet behind it. And a lot of these handlers are focusing on where they’re going next instead of watching their dog’s behaviors. Because we’re so in tune to evaluation standards. If we train to operational needs, we’ll blow evaluation standards away. But that’s not what we do. We’re backwards. | |
Concealment | Quote 39. I would say location is more difficult than amount. I think the dog would be able to find a small amount anywhere. I would rather there be a small amount than a bad guy trying to hide it efficiently. |
Vapor Pressure | Quote 40. Certain odors in explosive detection you look for have lower vapor pressures, so sometimes even in an ideal environment it may be difficult for them to pick it up or pinpoint right away. |
Set Time | Quote 41. The time the odor has been in place is important. Because even if it’s loosely packaged and easy to find, if they just dropped it wherever they dropped it, it’s not going to have had time to cook up and out. Originally very little if anything is coming out, but once it pushes up and out and gets up on top of the ground or out of the packaging, then the environment can get involved. So, the wind can push it around and make it more available to the dog. |
Nature of Explosive | Quote 42. You don’t know if it’s going to be a homemade explosive, TATP or HMTD, [omitted country] C4, [omitted region] TNT. You can sort of try to train for it, but when you’re going into a lot of this stuff, you don’t know. When we were in [Middle Eastern country] doing searches and a lot of these landmines have been there for 30 years, so they from [omitted]. We didn’t have [omitted] mines to train on a lot. When we went into [Caucasian country], again, they were [omitted region], but then could have been a different type. You try to prepare, but you never can for sure know what you come across. We train on the homemade explosives, but they’re constantly changing mixes based on what’s available, what chemicals they can get, or they’re maybe actively using intelligence trying to defeat the dog. |
Climate | Quote 43. If I always put out the training aids in nice, perfect climates and places, and the dogs will get used to that odor movement or finding the odor in a certain way. But, when it’s snowing, when it’s cold, when it’s raining, when it’s hot, when it’s humid, odor changes. In general, we see that it’s harder to find odor in cold temperatures than it is in warmer temperatures. But then if you throw high winds into the mix, it can create a lot of problems, and a miss. |
Quote 44. Environmental factors definitely affect odor, everything from temperature to humidity to wind to the terrain itself. If you’re in a tunnel or if you’re going along a mountainside, so hills, valleys, you name it. Is the dog going to be able to pick up the scent easily, or is it just going to go right over their head? Well, you’re going to have no luck based off of where the wind carries. If it’s heavily treed, will that end up blocking a lot versus an open field where the dog has the likelihood of picking up a scent? |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Farr, B.D.; Otto, C.M.; Szymczak, J.E. Expert Perspectives on the Performance of Explosive Detection Canines: Performance Degrading Factors. Animals 2021, 11, 1978. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11071978
Farr BD, Otto CM, Szymczak JE. Expert Perspectives on the Performance of Explosive Detection Canines: Performance Degrading Factors. Animals. 2021; 11(7):1978. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11071978
Chicago/Turabian StyleFarr, Brian D., Cynthia M. Otto, and Julia E. Szymczak. 2021. "Expert Perspectives on the Performance of Explosive Detection Canines: Performance Degrading Factors" Animals 11, no. 7: 1978. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11071978
APA StyleFarr, B. D., Otto, C. M., & Szymczak, J. E. (2021). Expert Perspectives on the Performance of Explosive Detection Canines: Performance Degrading Factors. Animals, 11(7), 1978. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11071978