Why Do People Choose a Particular Dog? A Mixed-Methods Analysis of Factors Owners Consider Important When Acquiring a Dog, on a Convenience Sample of Austrian Pet Dog Owners
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Subjects
2.2. Procedure
- Demographic attributes of the owner (e.g., age, sex, education, type of residence);
- Composition of the household (e.g., number of adults, children, other dogs);
- Demographic attributes of the dog (e.g., age, sex, breed, reproductive status, origin);
- Dog-keeping practices and conditions (e.g., where the dog stays, shared activities with the owner, training activities, competitions);
- Practical information relevant to behavioral studies (e.g., owner’s contact information, food and toy motivation, health and behavioral problems, dietary restrictions of the dog).
2.3. Statistical Analyses
2.3.1. Coding the Open-Text Responses
2.3.2. Consistency of the Responses
2.3.3. Owner Characteristics vs. Preferences in Dog Choice
3. Results
3.1. Subjects
3.2. Content Analysis
- Long response with multiple different codes: In many cases, owners provided extensive descriptions of their dog’s characteristics, as well as the reasons and circumstances behind their selection. For instance, the owner with ID 115 wrote this regarding their miniature spitz: “Standing ears, which reduce the likelihood of ear infections; long muzzle, which reduces the likelihood of respiratory problems; balanced build, which reduces the likelihood of damage to the postural apparatus; long double-layered coat, which makes the dog less sensitive to cold weather and wetness, in summer you can still shave him. So due to health reasons” (ID115, miniature spitz). In this response, we can identify four codes: looks (general), hair, size, and health.
- Long response with few different codes: In responses like these, owners elaborated on the adoption process, yet only a few reasons were distinguishable: “It was clear to us right from the start: if we take in a dog, it should be one that will be helped. So, we came across a couple who rescue dogs from various countries from killing stations and pass them on. That’s how we found our Benji. We saw him and just had to give him a nice home” (ID21, mixed breed). In this response, two codes can be identified: shelter/rescue, and choosing on a whim.
- Shorter response with multiple different codes: In responses like these, owners provided brief descriptions, often using just a word or two, to convey the desirable qualities of the dog. Despite the brevity, multiple reasons were distinguishable in the responses. For example: “active, friendly, size” (ID240, Hungarian vizsla). The codes found in this response are active/playful, friendly/family dog, and size.
- Shorter response with fewer different codes: In responses like these, owners provided brief descriptions, often with just a single word, to describe the adoption of the dog. Even in responses with multiple words, only one or two reasons were distinguishable: “Working dog” (ID229, border collie). In this response, only one code is found: work/sport skills.
3.2.1. Codes
- Preference, love, and fascination of the breed (breed-based choice, 29.43%): In nearly one-third of the responses, the owners indicated that they selected the breed and not the individual, either by stating that they liked the breed, or by mentioning the name of the breed in the response. Examples: “Because I like this breed” (ID281, American Staffordshire terrier); “The breed best met our requirements.” (ID73, French bulldog); “I fell in love with Border Collies in general on a winter holiday. When the opportunity arose to get a puppy, we decided on a female […]” (ID736, border collie); “A Golden Retriever has always been my dream […]” (ID824, golden retriever).
- The look of the animal (looks (general), 10.03%): In some instances, owners solely referenced the appearance or overall look of the dog, while in others, they emphasized the animal’s attractiveness. Owners typically mentioned the animal’s appearance (“appearance”), perhaps the body structure (“body structure”), or used terms denoting beauty (“beautiful”, “pretty”, “gorgeous”). Some owners appreciated their pets for resembling another breed but being more practical for their lifestyle. Examples: “In love with looks” (ID254, mixed breed); “[…] because he is such a beautiful dog.” (ID912, border collie); “looks exactly like a Doberman, only it’s smaller” (ID501, German pinscher).
- The coat of the animal (hair, 3.62%): Some owners specifically mentioned the color (“white”, “colorful coloring”, “funny pattern”, etc.), the length (“long haired”, “short haired”, “not too long or not too short”, etc.), or used words to describe the texture of the coat (“fluffy”, “bushy”). Others noted the lack of shedding as an important aspect (“does not shed hair”) and the ease of care for the type of hair (“easy to clean”, “easy to groom”). Examples: “Does not shed as much hair” (ID439, Tibetan terrier); “She caught our eye […] certainly because of her funny coloring.” (ID50, mixed breed); “I always wanted […] light-colored coat that wasn’t too long or too short” (ID749, mixed breed).
- The size of the animal (size, 10.77%): Owners often emphasized their preference for a dog of a specific size (“small”, “medium”, or “large”). Some considered the size of their home or the dog’s portability (“optimal apartment sized dog”, “easy to take anywhere”), while others considered future joint activities (“optimal size for rescue dog work”). Examples: “I wanted a working dog, medium-sized.” (ID265, border collie); “good size for a city apartment in Vienna” (ID418, standard schnauzer); “can be taken almost anywhere due to its size” (ID581, Chihuahua).
- Being a shelter dog, adopted or rescued, or a saved dog (shelter/rescue, 17.08%): Many owners expressed a specific desire to adopt or rescue a dog from a shelter. Some mentioned fostering the dog before adoption, while others sought dogs that had been in shelters for a long time. Some owners rescued their pets from the streets, emphasizing phrases like “found on the highway” or “found on the street”. It was important to some owners that the dog came from another country or from a killing station (phrase: rescued from killing stations). Examples: “I wanted to save her” (ID722, mixed breed); “we adopted her” (ID359, cocker spaniel); “I wanted to give a […] dog that had been in the shelter for a long time a place to live.” (ID415, mixed breed).
- Lineage, breeder, or the parentage of the animal (pedigree, 4.55%): Some owners emphasized the lineage or parentage of the dog (“pairing of parents”) or its good pedigree using the phrases “good bloodline”, “good parents of the dog”, and the “pedigree of the parents”. They considered factors such as the reputation of the breeder (“breeder friend of mine”, “known the breeder”), as well as being from a good background (“from a good place”, “trusted breeding”). Familiarity or positive past experiences with the breeder also influenced their choice (“from the same breeder”). Examples: “We know the mother and father of our bitch.” (ID836, Labrador retriever); “I bought her […] from the same breeder where we bought our Pumi bitch” (ID710, mudi).
- The health of the animal (health, 1.39%): Some owners prioritized finding a dog that was in good health and free of breed-specific diseases. They described specific health criteria (“too small for HD, too large for patellar luxation”), or simply emphasized the importance of the dog being “healthy”, “not sick”, “no health complaints”, or “free from hereditary diseases”. Examples: “With the hope of obtaining a healthy dog” (ID44, harzer fuchs); “breed with no typical breed complaints to speak of” (ID24, Parson Russell terrier).
- The age of the animal (age, 1.49%): Some owners mentioned the age category of the dog they were looking for, such as preferring a “young” or “old” dog. In some cases, age was mentioned as a reason on its own (“age”). Additionally, some owners considered the expected lifespan of the individual dog (“long-lived”). “He […] was the same age as Paul.” (ID15, Irish wolfhound); “Breed, age” (ID157, Labrador retriever).
- The sex of the animal (sex, 1.30%): Many owners emphasized their preference for a specific sex when selecting their dog (“a male dog”). Some casually mentioned the sex of the individual dog, while others considered the neutering status as an important factor (“intact”, “neutered”). Examples: “He was the only male in the litter.” (ID411, mixed breed); “we wanted a bitch from this litter” (ID914, border collie).
- Calmness (calm, 2.51%): Some owners sought dogs with calm temperaments, describing this behavior as “cozy nature” or “serenity”. Others mentioned qualities like being “restrained” and “not hectic”. Examples: “because this dog is a calm representative of her breed” (ID206, Australian shepherd); “After a Doberman, a calmer dog was sought […]”. (ID784, Labrador retriever).
- Friendliness, low aggression, being a family dog (friendly/family dog, 15.23%): Owners expressed a desire for a dog that would be friendly and well-behaved around people, including children and strangers. Terms like “friendly”, “dear”, “nice”, and “affectionate” were used. Alongside the term “family dog”, many emphasized kindness and sociability towards people (“people-oriented”, “friendliness towards people”, “good social behavior and people-friendly”). Many emphasized the importance of the dog being a good family companion, exhibiting kindness, sociability towards people, and being child-friendly (“loves children”, “very child-friendly”, “suitable for children”, “kind with children”) and patient (“patient”). Examples: “She bribed me with her friendliness towards people.” (ID269, mixed breed); “This breed is people-friendly and likes children.” (ID516, Bernese mountain dog); “[…] of all puppies, this dog was the best suited as a family dog.” (ID485, Appenzeller mountain dog).
- Docility, easily manageable character (docile/manageable, 5.48%): Owners valued dogs with an easy-to-handle temperament, often described as docile, good-natured, and adaptable. Terms like “docile”, “good-natured”, “easy tempered”, and “adaptable” were also used to describe the code. In addition to “manageable” or “easy to manage”, the terms “easy to handle”, “trouble-free”, “easy”, “good for beginners”, and “not difficult” were used to express manageability. Examples: “A mix of trusting and adaptable breeds” (ID764, mixed breed); “I wanted a dog that would fit into everyday life without any problems” (ID461, golden retriever).
- Working and/or sporting skills (work/sport skills, 21.82%): Owners sought dogs specifically for work or sports activities. Aside from generally mentioning “sporty” or “working dog”, the owners used a wide variety of terms to describe the characteristics of a dog that would be suitable for such activities in the future. They looked for dogs with characteristics suitable for various tasks, described as “carry out jobs”, “willing to work”, or “eagerness to work” to describe the dog’s suitability for work, or even described future activities like “guide dog training”. Those looking for a sporting dog often wrote that they were looking for a “dog for sports”, “suitable for sports”, or described the exact sporting activity as “agility”, “obedience”, “mantrailing”, etc. Examples: “I would like to work with him.” (ID229, border collie); “[…] deliberately chose a dog for sporting activities such as agility, dog racing” (ID647, mixed dog); “Dog sports partner” (ID304, border collie).
- Cleverness and trainability (smart/trainable, 13.93%): Owners desired dogs that were intelligent, easy to train, and obedient. Characteristics associated with this code included terms like “clever”, “intelligent”, “easy to train”, “eagerness to learn”, “training possibilities”, “motivated”, and “enjoys learning”. Examples: “Willing to work, will to please” (ID661, border collie); “He is very sensitive, but always good-natured, active and, above all, extremely eager to learn.” (ID302, bearded collie); “Because I wanted, above all, a smart, intelligent all-rounder […]” (ID38, Australian shepherd).
- Active, energetic, agile nature, being playful and boisterous (active/playful, 16.99%): Adjectives related to activity included “agile”, “athletic”, “fast”, and “lively”. The dog’s playful behavior was usually described by the words “playful” or “cheerful”. Examples: “I liked the dog’s energy […]” (ID437, mixed breed); “Very motivated, active.” (ID248, Australian shepherd); “I wanted a lively dog” (ID635, malinois).
- The character, temperament, and personality of the animal (character (global), 15.69%): Owners referenced the overall character, temperament, or personality of the dog using terms like “personality”, “nature”, or “temperament”, without specifying particular traits. Examples: “Liked his character” (ID417, mixed breed); “character, appearance” (ID554, mixed breed).
- Loyalty (loyalty, 1.95%): Some owners referred to the dependence or loyalty of the dog, mostly using the words “loyal” and “faithful”. Examples: “They are extremely loyal to their owners” (ID272, mixed breed); “Wanted a dog with a close bond […]” (ID282, Australian shepherd).
- Guarding skills (guarding, 0.56%): Owners mentioned characteristics related to the dog’s ability to guard the house or provide security. Terms like “guardian”, “vigilant”, or “watchdog” were used, possibly referring to the animal’s “protective instinct” and “sense of security”. Examples: “I wanted a big, intelligent, but not crazy guard dog” (ID888, Dutch shepherd); “[…] alert dog” (ID756, mixed breed).
- Being stubborn, headstrong, a challenge (stubborn, 3.81%): Responses indicating that the dog was stubborn, independent, or posed a challenge for the owner were included in this code. Terms like “stubborn”, “naughty”, “strong-willed”, “headstrong”, or “cheeky” were used to describe the dog’s obstinacy or strong will. Examples: “I find stubbornness very attractive.” (ID723, Parson Russell terrier); “The Puli is a very independent dog breed.” ID484, puli).
- Having direct experience with similar individuals or same breeds (background information, 7.89%): Owners sometimes mentioned that they relied on their previous experiences with similar breeds, breed types, or individuals when selecting their new dog. This included mentioning growing up with or owning another individual from the same breed/type, or knowing a similar dog (e.g., of the neighbor). Some owners mentioned that they knew the parents of this dog. Examples: “I grew up with a long-haired collie and then with border collies” (ID748, border collie); “My parents had such a dog” (ID542, Airedale terrier).
- Sentimental, reminiscent responses (sentimental reasons, 9.38%): Responses in this code indicate that the owner’s selection of the dog was influenced by positive childhood memories, favorite books or movies, or personal beliefs. Expressions such as “a wish since childhood”, “is my dog of fate”, and “long-awaited wish” were commonly used to convey this sentiment. Examples: “A childhood dream” (ID600, German shepherd); “[…] I now think that when I was looking for a dog, I remembered my childhood dog […]” (ID819, mixed breed).
- Unplanned, accident, falling in love (choosing on a whim, 23.96%): Responses in this code indicate that the owner’s selection of the dog was based on chance or an emotional connection. Expressions such as “coincidence”, “just so happened”, “by accident”, “dog’s choice”, “fell in love”, “liked it”, or “sympathetic” were commonly used to convey this sentiment. Examples: “Love at first sight” (ID844, mixed breed), “that was a coincidence” (ID402, mixed breed); “I saw her in pictures and immediately decided that she was a good fit for us.” (ID722, mixed breed); “born—seen—in love” (ID474, border collie).
- Mentioning that somebody else also had a say in the selection (chosen by other, 8.17%): Responses in this code indicate that the owner’s decision to select the dog was influenced by another person. This could be their child, partner, or another individual. Expressions such as “my son brought”, “wish of the daughter”, “a wish from my wife”, “my husband fell in love”, “my father chose him for me”, or “was my foster” were commonly used to convey this influence. Examples: “The decision was not made by me, but rather by my partner.” (ID680, mixed breed); “My previous one died unexpectedly, and my partner at the time immediately started looking for a new dog. We chose him together.” (ID51, mixed breed)
- Unique behaviors (unique behaviors, 4.18%): This code encompasses behavioral characteristics mentioned by only one or two respondents, which were not categorized separately due to their limited occurrence. Traits such as communication skills or being funny, happy, or sensitive fall under this category.
3.2.2. Themes
- Appearance (20.33%): included all three codes related to appearance, i.e., looks (general), hair, and size.
- Origin (21.54%): included the two codes concerning the dog’s origin, namely, shelter/rescue and pedigree.
- Demographics (3.99%): included the three codes (health, sex, age) related to the basic, demographic characteristics of the dog.
- Friendly/manageable (19.31%): we merged the three codes associated with the dog’s amicable, easy-to-handle disposition (calm, friendly/family dog, and docile/manageable) into this theme.
- Sport (36. 86%): we grouped three codes related to the dog’s suitability for sports or work into this theme [38]: work/sport skills, smart/trainable, and active/playful.
Codes and Themes | Description | Frequency | Inter-Coder Reliability | Test–Retest Reliability | Consistency Across Dogs | Included in the GLM Analyses? | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Appearance theme | One, if any, from looks (general), hair, or size was mentioned | 20.30% | - | 0.733 | 0.580 | Included in the analyses | |
Looks (general) | Any reference to the whole appearance of the dog without mentioning specifics (e.g., “looks” or “beauty”) | 10.03% | 0.928 | 0.151 | 0.500 | Low repeatability, analyzed only as part of the theme | |
Hair | Any reference to the fur or coat of the dog | 3.62% | 1.000 | Low frequency, analyzed only as part of the theme | |||
Size | Any reference to the size of the dog | 10.77% | 1.000 | 0.737 | 0.698 | Analyzed both as part of the theme and on its own | |
Origin theme | Either shelter/rescue or pedigree was mentioned | 21.54% | - | 0.600 | 0.274 | Included in the analyses | |
Shelter/rescue | Mentioning that they rescued the dog, adopted it out of pity, or adopted it from a shelter or rescue organization | 17.08% | 0.765 | 0.612 | 0.251 | Analyzed both as part of the theme and on its own | |
Pedigree | Mentioning the importance of the dog’s pedigree (or the lack of it), or knowing the breeder | 4.55% | 0.656 | Low frequency, analyzed only as part of the theme | |||
Demographics theme | One, if any, from health, age, or sex was mentioned | 3.99% | - | Excluded due to low frequency | |||
Health | Any reference to the health of the dog | 1.39% | 0.789 | Low frequency, analyzed only as part of the theme | |||
Age | Any reference to the age of the dog | 1.49% | 0.789 | Low frequency, analyzed only as part of the theme | |||
Sex | Mentioning specifically that they wanted a male or female dog | 1.30% | 0.795 | Low frequency, analyzed only as part of the theme | |||
Friendly/manageable theme | One, if any, from friendly/family dog, docile/manageable, or calm was mentioned | 19.31% | - | 0.625 | 0.360 | Included in the analyses | |
Calm | Any reference to the calmness of the dog | 2.51% | 1.000 | Low frequency, analyzed only as part of the theme | |||
Friendly/family dog | Any reference to the friendliness, lack of aggression, or family- or child-compatibility of the dog | 15.23% | 0.710 | 0.731 | 0.333 | Analyzed both as part of the theme and on its own | |
Docile/manageable | Any reference to the docility or easily manageable nature of the dog | 5.48% | 0.651 | 0.026 | 0.263 | Low repeatability, analyzed only as part of the theme | |
Sport theme | One, if any, from work/sport skills, smart/trainable, or active/playful was mentioned | 36.86% | - | 0.801 | 0.409 | Included in the analyses | |
Work/sport skills | Any reference to working or sporting skills of any type (e.g., agility, herding, hunting, therapy) | 21.82% | 0.727 | 0.612 | 0.478 | Analyzed both as part of the theme and on its own | |
Smart/trainable | Any reference to the smartness, trainability, or obedience of the dog | 13.93% | 0.926 | 0.470 | 0.483 | Analyzed both as part of the theme and on its own | |
Active/playful | Any reference to the activity level or playfulness of the dog | 16.99% | 0.759 | 0.394 | 0.453 | Low repeatability, analyzed only as part of the theme | |
Other behavioral codes | Miscellaneous behaviors that did not form a theme | 22.93% | |||||
Character (general) | Any reference to the whole character or personality of the dog (e.g., “nature”, or “temperament”), without mentioning specifics | 15.69% | 0.687 | 0.009 | 0.076 | Excluded due to low repeatability | |
Loyalty | Any reference to the clinginess, dependence, or loyalty of the dog | 1.95% | 1.000 | Excluded due to low frequency | |||
Guarding | Any reference to the dog’s house-guarding ability (also included if the dog provides safety) | 0.56% | 1.000 | Excluded due to low frequency | |||
Stubborn | Any reference to the personality of the dog (also included if the owner wanted a challenge with the choice) | 3.81% | 0.795 | Excluded due to low frequency | |||
Unique behaviors | A collective code including all and only those behavioral traits that were mentioned only once or twice in the dataset and, thus, have no code of their own | 4.18% | 0.729 | Excluded due to low frequency | |||
Owner-related codes | Miscellaneous owner-related codes that did not form a theme | 61.47% | |||||
Breed-based choice | Mentioning the breed by name or indicating that they chose the breed specifically | 29.43% | 0.735 | 0.498 | 0.524 | Included in the analyses | |
Background information | Any reference to previous experiences with the breed, the breed type, or the parent(s) of the dog | 7.89% | 0.852 | 0.696 | 0.425 | Included in the analyses | |
Choosing on a whim | Just fancying that particular dog (typically include expressions like “I fell in love”, “the dog chose me”, “just so happened”) | 23.96% | 0.628 | 0.695 | 0.367 | Included in the analyses | |
Sentimental reasons | Any mention of sentimental reasons (e.g., selecting the dog because of good childhood memories, favorite books or movies, etc.) | 9.38% | 0.936 | 0.340 | 0.498 | Excluded due to low repeatability | |
Chosen by other | Mentioning that another person took part in selecting the dog | 8.17% | 0.729 | 0.039 | 0.071 | Excluded due to low repeatability |
3.3. Consistency of the Responses
3.3.1. Test–Retest Reliability (Repeatability)
3.3.2. Consistency across Dogs
3.4. Owner Characteristics vs. Preferences in Dog Choice
4. Discussion
4.1. Codes and Themes
4.2. Consistency
4.3. Owner Characteristics vs. Preferences in Dog Choice
4.4. Limitations
4.5. Avenues for Future Studies
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Miklósi, Á.; Topál, J. What Does It Take to Become “Best Friends”? Evolutionary Changes in Canine Social Competence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013, 17, 287–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vieira de Castro, A.C.; Araújo, Â.; Fonseca, A.; Olsson, I.A.S. Improving Dog Training Methods: Efficacy and Efficiency of Reward and Mixed Training Methods. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0247321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McConnell, A.R.; Brown, C.M.; Shoda, T.M.; Stayton, L.E.; Martin, C.E. Friends with Benefits: On the Positive Consequences of Pet Ownership. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 101, 1239–1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bender, Y.; Bräuer, J.; Schweinberger, S.R. What Makes a Good Dog-Owner Team?—A Systematic Review about Compatibility in Personality and Attachment. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2023, 260, 105857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pinillos, R.G.; Appleby, M.C.; Manteca, X.; Scott-Park, F.; Smith, C.; Velarde, A. One Welfare—A Platform for Improving Human and Animal Welfare. Vet. Rec. 2016, 179, 412–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Holland, K.E. Acquiring a Pet Dog: A Review of Factors Affecting the Decision-Making of Prospective Dog Owners. Animals 2019, 9, 124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holland, K.E.; Mead, R.; Casey, R.A.; Upjohn, M.M.; Christley, R.M. Why Do People Want Dogs? A Mixed-Methods Study of Motivations for Dog Acquisition in the United Kingdom. Front. Vet. Sci. 2022, 9, 877950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holland, K.E.; Mead, R.; Casey, R.A.; Upjohn, M.M.; Christley, R.M. “Don’t Bring Me a Dog…I’ll Just Keep It”: Understanding Unplanned Dog Acquisitions Amongst a Sample of Dog Owners Attending Canine Health and Welfare Community Events in the United Kingdom. Animals 2021, 11, 605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bir, C.; Widmar, N.J.O.; Croney, C.C. Stated Preferences for Dog Characteristics and Sources of Acquisition. Animals 2017, 7, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woodhead, J.; Feng, L.; Howell, T.; Ruby, M.; Bennett, P. Perceptions of Dog Breeding Practices, Breeding Dog Welfare and Companion Dog Acquisition in a Self-Selected Sample of Australian Adults. Anim. Welf. 2018, 27, 357–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mead, R.; Holland, K.E.; Casey, R.A.; Upjohn, M.M.; Christley, R.M. “Do Your Homework as Your Heart Takes over When You Go Looking”: Factors Associated with Pre-Acquisition Information-Seeking among Prospective UK Dog Owners. Animals 2023, 13, 1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bognár, Z.; Kubinyi, E. The Brachycephalic Paradox: The Relationship between Attitudes, Demography, Personality, Health Awareness, and Dog-Human Eye Contact. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2023, 264, 105948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ghirlanda, S.; Acerbi, A.; Herzog, H.; Serpell, J.A. Fashion vs. Function in Cultural Evolution: The Case of Dog Breed Popularity. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e74770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Herzog, H. Forty-Two Thousand and One Dalmatians: Fads, Social Contagion, and Dog Breed Popularity. Soc. Anim. 2006, 14, 383–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Packer, R.M.A.; O’Neill, D.G.; Fletcher, F.; Farnworth, M.J. Come for the Looks, Stay for the Personality? A Mixed Methods Investigation of Reacquisition and Owner Recommendation of Bulldogs, French Bulldogs and Pugs. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0237276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, S.E.; Todd, P.M. Stated and Revealed Preferences in Companion Animal Choice. Behav. Res. 2019, 51, 1498–1509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Protopopova, A.; Wynne, C.D.L. Adopter-Dog Interactions at the Shelter: Behavioral and Contextual Predictors of Adoption. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 157, 109–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weiss, E.; Miller, K.; Mohan-Gibbons, H.; Vela, C. Why Did You Choose This Pet?: Adopters and Pet Selection Preferences in Five Animal Shelters in the United States. Animals 2012, 2, 144–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wells, D.; Hepper, P.G. The Behaviour of Dogs in a Rescue Shelter. Anim. Welf. 1992, 1, 171–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diverio, S.; Boccini, B.; Menchetti, L.; Bennett, P. The Italian Perception of the “Ideal Companion Dog”. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2016, 12, 27–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- King, T.; Marston, L.; Bennett, P. Describing the Ideal Australian Companion Dog. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 120, 84–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Popping, R. Analyzing Open-Ended Questions by Means of Text Analysis Procedures. Bull. Sociol. Methodol./Bull. De. Méthodologie Sociol. 2015, 128, 23–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rouder, J.; Saucier, O.; Kinder, R.; Jans, M. What to Do With All Those Open-Ended Responses? Data Visualization Techniques for Survey Researchers. Surv. Pract. 2021, 14, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Connor Desai, S.; Reimers, S. Comparing the Use of Open and Closed Questions for Web-Based Measures of the Continued-Influence Effect. Behav. Res. 2019, 51, 1426–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garland, R. The Mid-Point on a Rating Scale: Is It Desirable? Mark. Bull. 1991, 2, 66–70. [Google Scholar]
- Stemler, S.E.; Bebell, D.; Sonnabend, L.A. Using School Mission Statements for Reflection and Research. Educ. Adm. Q. 2011, 47, 383–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fratkin, J.L.; Sinn, D.L.; Patall, E.A.; Gosling, S.D. Personality Consistency in Dogs: A Meta-Analysis. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e54907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Payne, C.; Jaffe, K. Self Seeks like: Many Humans Choose Their Dog Pets Following Rules Used for Assortative Mating. J. Ethol. 2005, 23, 15–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turcsán, B.; Range, F.; Virányi, Z.; Miklósi, Á.; Kubinyi, E. Birds of a Feather Flock Together? Perceived Personality Matching in Owner-Dog Dyads. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 140, 154–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cobb, M.; Branson, N.; McGreevy, P.; Lill, A.; Bennett, P. The Advent of Canine Performance Science: Offering a Sustainable Future for Working Dogs. Behav. Process. 2014, 110, 96–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neuendorf, K.; Kumar, A. Content Analysis; SAGE: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2016; ISBN 978-1-118-29075-0. [Google Scholar]
- Stemler, S.E. Content analysis. In Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences: An Interdisciplinary, Searchable, and Linkable Resource; Scott, R., Kosslyn, S., Eds.; John Wiley and Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2015; pp. 1–14. ISBN 978-1-118-90077-2. [Google Scholar]
- Bammidi, D.P. Content Analysis: A Method in Social Science Research. In Research Methods for Social Work; Lal Das, D.K., Bhaskaran, V., Eds.; Rawat: New Delhi, India, 2008; pp. 173–193. [Google Scholar]
- Stemler, S. An Overview of Content Analysis. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 2001, 7, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Braun, V.; Clarke, V. Using Thematic Analysis in Psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 2006, 3, 77–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cunningham, M.; Wells, M. Qualitative Analysis of 6961 Free-Text Comments from the First National Cancer Patient Experience Survey in Scotland. BMJ Open 2017, 7, e015726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cicchetti, D.V.; Sparrow, S.A. Developing Criteria for Establishing Interrater Reliability of Specific Items: Applications to Assessment of Adaptive Behavior. Am. J. Ment. Defic. 1981, 86, 127–137. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Salonen, M.; Mikkola, S.; Niskanen, J.E.; Hakanen, E.; Sulkama, S.; Puurunen, J.; Lohi, H. Breed, Age, and Social Environment Are Associated with Personality Traits in Dogs. iScience 2023, 26, 106691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yonelinas, A.P. The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research. J. Mem. Lang. 2002, 46, 441–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Choi, B.C.K.; Pak, A.W.P. A Catalog of Biases in Questionnaires. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2004, 2, A13. [Google Scholar]
- Calvo, P.; Bowen, J.; Bulbena, A.; Tobeña, A.; Fatjó, J. Highly Educated Men Establish Strong Emotional Links with Their Dogs: A Study with Monash Dog Owner Relationship Scale (MDORS) in Committed Spanish Dog Owners. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0168748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dinwoodie, I.R.; Zottola, V.; Kubitz, K.; Dodman, N.H. Selection Factors Influencing Eventual Owner Satisfaction about Pet Dog Adoption. Animals 2022, 12, 2264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kretzler, B.; König, H.-H.; Hajek, A. Pet Ownership, Loneliness, and Social Isolation: A Systematic Review. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2022, 57, 1935–1957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Powell, L.; Chia, D.; McGreevy, P.; Podberscek, A.L.; Edwards, K.M.; Neilly, B.; Guastella, A.J.; Lee, V.; Stamatakis, E. Expectations for Dog Ownership: Perceived Physical, Mental and Psychosocial Health Consequences among Prospective Adopters. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0200276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- MacLean, E.L.; Snyder-Mackler, N.; vonHoldt, B.M.; Serpell, J.A. Highly Heritable and Functionally Relevant Breed Differences in Dog Behaviour. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2019, 286, 20190716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reem, N. Shelter-Housed versus Re-Homed Dogs: Adjustment, Behavior, and Adoption Outcomes. Biol. Futur. 2019, 70, 149–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tuber, D.S.; Miller, D.D.; Caris, K.A.; Halter, R.; Linden, F.; Hennessy, M.B. Dogs in Animal Shelters: Problems, Suggestions, and Needed Expertise. Psychol. Sci. 1999, 10, 379–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patronek, G.J.; Bradley, J.; Arps, E. Saving Normal: A New Look at Behavioral Incompatibilities and Dog Relinquishment to Shelters. J. Vet. Behav. 2022, 49, 36–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGuire, B. Characteristics and Adoption Success of Shelter Dogs Assessed as Resource Guarders. Animals 2019, 9, 982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arhant, C.; Beetz, A.M.; Troxler, J. Caregiver Reports of Interactions between Children up to 6 Years and Their Family Dog—Implications for Dog Bite Prevention. Front. Vet. Sci. 2017, 4, 130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McMillan, F.D. Behavioral and Psychological Outcomes for Dogs Sold as Puppies through Pet Stores and/or Born in Commercial Breeding Establishments: Current Knowledge and Putative Causes. J. Vet. Behav. 2017, 19, 14–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plana, N.M.; Kalmar, C.L.; Cheung, L.; Swanson, J.W.; Taylor, J.A. Pediatric Dog Bite Injuries: A 5-Year Nationwide Study and Implications of the COVID-19 Pandemic. J. Craniofac Surg. 2022, 33, 1436–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kobelt, A.; Hemsworth, P.; Barnett, J.; Coleman, G. A Survey of Dog Ownership in Suburban Australia—Conditions and Behaviour Problems. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2003, 82, 137–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, G.I.; Boyer, W.N. The Effects of Dog Obedience Training and Behavioural Counselling upon the Human-Canine Relationship. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1993, 37, 147–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Teng, K.T.; McGreevy, P.D.; Toribio, J.-A.L.M.L.; Dhand, N.K. Trends in Popularity of Some Morphological Traits of Purebred Dogs in Australia. Canine Genet. Epidemiol. 2016, 3, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Juarbe-Díaz, S.V. Social Dynamics and Behavior Problems in Multiple-Dog Households. Vet. Clin. N. Am. Small Anim. Pract. 1997, 27, 497–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wallis, L.J.; Iotchev, I.B.; Kubinyi, E. Correction: Assertive, Trainable and Older Dogs Are Perceived as More Dominant in Multi-Dog Households. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0231043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koski, L.; Bäcklund, P. On the Fringe: The Positions of Dogs in Finnish Dog Training Culture. Soc. Anim. 2015, 23, 24–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lazarowski, L.; Thompkins, A.; Krichbaum, S.; Waggoner, L.P.; Deshpande, G.; Katz, J.S. Comparing Pet and Detection Dogs (Canis familiaris) on Two Aspects of Social Cognition. Learn. Behav. 2020, 48, 432–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veitch, J.; Christian, H.; Carver, A.; Salmon, J. Physical Activity Benefits from Taking Your Dog to the Park. Landsc. Urban. Plan. 2019, 185, 173–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tesfom, G.; Birch, N.J. Does Definition of Self Predict Adopter Dog Breed Choice? Int. Rev. Public. Nonprofit Mark. 2013, 10, 103–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tami, G.; Gallagher, A. Description of the Behaviour of Domestic Dog (Canis familiaris) by Experienced and Inexperienced People. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 120, 159–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weir, S.; Kessler, S.E. The Making of a (Dog) Movie Star: The Effect of the Portrayal of Dogs in Movies on Breed Registrations in the United States. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0261916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Menchetti, L.; Calipari, S.; Guelfi, G.; Catanzaro, A.; Diverio, S. My Dog Is Not My Cat: Owner Perception of the Personalities of Dogs and Cats Living in the Same Household. Animals 2018, 8, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kluess, H.A.; Jones, R.L.; Lee-Fowler, T. Perceptions of Body Condition, Diet and Exercise by Sports Dog Owners and Pet Dog Owners. Animals 2021, 11, 1752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reese, L.; Skidmore, M.; Dyar, W.; Rosebrook, E. No Dog Left Behind: A Hedonic Pricing Model for Animal Shelters. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2016, 20, 52–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Characteristic | Category | Count | Percentage |
---|---|---|---|
Gender of the owner | Male | 126 | 11.70% |
Female | 951 | 88.30% | |
Age of the owner (year) | Mean ± SD | 36.59 ± 12.68 | |
Highest education level | Middle school | 461 | 42.88% |
Technical school | 305 | 28.37% | |
College, university | 309 | 28.74% | |
Type of residence | City (>50,000 people) | 671 | 62.30% |
Town (2500–50,000 people) | 277 | 25.72% | |
Village (0–2500 people) | 129 | 11.98% | |
Number of adults (>18 y) in the household | Single-person household | 320 | 29.71% |
Two or more | 757 | 70.29% | |
Number of children (<18 y) in the household | None | 865 | 80.32% |
One or more | 212 | 19.68% | |
Number of current dogs | Only one | 574 | 53.30% |
Two or more | 503 | 46.70% | |
Number of dogs when the current one arrived | Zero | 656 | 61.02% |
One or more | 419 | 38.98% | |
Number of dogs before the current one | None | 360 | 33.43% |
Had dog | 717 | 66.57% | |
Mixed or purebred | Mixed | 344 | 32.00% |
Purebred | 731 | 68.00% | |
Role of the dog in the family | Only for companionship | 484 | 45.02% |
Working or sporting too | 591 | 54.98% | |
Sex of the dog | Male | 482 | 44.84% |
Female | 593 | 55.16% | |
Reproductive status | Intact | 487 | 45.30% |
Neutered or spayed | 588 | 54.70% | |
Age of the dog (year) | Mean ± SD | 4.14 ± 3.39 | |
Origin of the dog | Breeder | 588 | 54.65% |
Private hands | 265 | 24.63% | |
Shelter or rescue dog | 223 | 20.72% | |
Age at acquisition | Under 1 year old | 933 | 86.79% |
Over 1 year old | 142 | 13.21% |
Dependent: Code or Theme | Explanatory: Demographic Traits | Wald χ2 | p Value | Exp(B) (95% CI) | Direction of Difference | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Appearance theme | Previous experience with dogs | 14.368 | <0.001 | 0.556 (0.410–0.753) | No dog before > had dog before | |
Size | Previous experience with dogs | 13.966 | <0.001 | 0.477 (0.323–0.703) | No dog before > had dog before | |
Origin theme | Child | 6.065 | 0.014 | 0.591 (0.389–0.898) | No child > child | |
N dogs when the dog arrived | 4.484 | 0.034 | 1.383 (1.024–1.867) | None < had dog | ||
Role of the dog | 7.042 | 0.008 | 0.669 (0.497–0.900) | Companion only > sport/work | ||
Owner age | 5.032 | 0.025 | 1.014 (1.002–1.026) | Younger > older | ||
Shelter/rescue | Child | 5.577 | 0.018 | 0.566 (0.353–0.908) | No child > child | |
Role of the dog | 15.175 | <0.001 | 0.524 (0.378–0.725) | Companion only > Sport/work | ||
Owner age | 5.812 | 0.016 | 1.017 (1.003–1.030) | Younger > older | ||
Friendly/ manageable theme | Child | 6.761 | 0.009 | 1.606 (1.124–2.294) | No child < have child | |
N dogs when the dog arrived | 9.442 | 0.002 | 0.598 (0.431–0.830) | None > had dog | ||
Friendly/family dog | Child | 10.678 | 0.001 | 1.917 (1. 297–2.831) | No child < child | |
N dogs when the dog arrived | 5.108 | 0.024 | 0.648 (0.445–0.944) | None > had dog | ||
Previous experience with dogs | 5.156 | 0.023 | 1.581 (1.065–2.348) | No dog before < had dog before | ||
Owner age | 5.956 | 0.015 | 1.018 (1.004–1.033) | Younger > older | ||
Sport theme | Previous experience with dogs | 5.819 | 0.016 | 1.418 (1.068–1.883) | No dog before < had dog before | |
Role of the dog | 110.933 | <0.001 | 4. 469 (3.382–5.905) | Companion only < sport/work | ||
Smart/trainable | Adults | 5.336 | 0.021 | 1.645 (1.078–2.511) | Single < more people | |
Child | 7.980 | 0.005 | 1.782 (1.193–2.660) | No child < child | ||
N dogs when the dog arrived | 5.645 | 0.018 | 0.634 (0.436–0.923) | None > had dog | ||
Role of the dog | 20.103 | <0.001 | 2.391 (1.633–3.500) | Companion only < sport/work | ||
Work/sports skills | Child | 4.437 | 0.035 | 1.497 (1.028–2.179) | No child < child | |
N dogs when the dog arrived | 14.789 | <0.001 | 1.841 (1.349–2.513) | None < had dog | ||
Role of the dog | 93.562 | <0.001 | 7.186 (4.819–10.715) | Companion only < sport/work | ||
Breed-based choice | Role of the dog | 12.403 | <0.001 | 1.623 (1.239–2.124) | Companion only < sport/work | |
Background information | Previous experience with dogs | 6.056 | 0.014 | 2.039 (1.156–3.597) | No dog before < had dog before | |
Owner age | 6.736 | 0.009 | 0.978 (0.962–0.995) | Younger < older | ||
Choosing on a whim | Adults | 7.657 | 0.006 | 0.657 (0.487–0.885) | Single > more people | |
N dogs when the dog arrived | 8.882 | 0.003 | 0.634 (0.470–0.856) | None > had dog |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Udvarhelyi-Tóth, K.M.; Iotchev, I.B.; Kubinyi, E.; Turcsán, B. Why Do People Choose a Particular Dog? A Mixed-Methods Analysis of Factors Owners Consider Important When Acquiring a Dog, on a Convenience Sample of Austrian Pet Dog Owners. Animals 2024, 14, 2634. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14182634
Udvarhelyi-Tóth KM, Iotchev IB, Kubinyi E, Turcsán B. Why Do People Choose a Particular Dog? A Mixed-Methods Analysis of Factors Owners Consider Important When Acquiring a Dog, on a Convenience Sample of Austrian Pet Dog Owners. Animals. 2024; 14(18):2634. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14182634
Chicago/Turabian StyleUdvarhelyi-Tóth, Kata Mária, Ivaylo B. Iotchev, Eniko Kubinyi, and Borbála Turcsán. 2024. "Why Do People Choose a Particular Dog? A Mixed-Methods Analysis of Factors Owners Consider Important When Acquiring a Dog, on a Convenience Sample of Austrian Pet Dog Owners" Animals 14, no. 18: 2634. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14182634
APA StyleUdvarhelyi-Tóth, K. M., Iotchev, I. B., Kubinyi, E., & Turcsán, B. (2024). Why Do People Choose a Particular Dog? A Mixed-Methods Analysis of Factors Owners Consider Important When Acquiring a Dog, on a Convenience Sample of Austrian Pet Dog Owners. Animals, 14(18), 2634. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14182634