Next Article in Journal
Characterization of Feline Basophils on the Sysmex XN-1000V and Evaluation of a New WDF Gating Profile
Previous Article in Journal
Fecal Carriage and Risk Factors Associated with Extended-Spectrum β-Lactamase-/AmpC-/Carbapenemase-Producing Escherichia coli in Dogs from Italy
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Using Social Network Analysis to Assess ‘Groupness’ in a Mixed-Species Zoo Exhibit of Tufted Capuchins (Sapajus apella) and Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri sciureus)

by
Sophia Daoudi-Simison
1,*,
Phyllis Lee
2 and
Hannah M. Buchanan-Smith
2
1
School of Psychology, Faculty of Medical Sciences, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne NE2 4DR, UK
2
Psychology, Faculty of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling FK9 4LA, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2024, 14(23), 3360; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14233360
Submission received: 8 October 2024 / Revised: 19 November 2024 / Accepted: 20 November 2024 / Published: 22 November 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Zoo Animals)

Simple Summary

This study investigated the associations between two different types of monkeys—tufted capuchins and squirrel monkeys—living together in captivity. Understanding how these mixed-species groups behave is crucial for informing effective husbandry practices that can ultimately enhance welfare practices. We employed social network analysis, a quantitative method for measuring relationships and behaviours within groups, to observe these monkeys at the Royal Zoological Society of Scotland’s Living Links to Human Evolution Research Centre, Edinburgh Zoo. Over 183 h, we monitored how frequently the monkeys came into proximity with one another. Our findings indicate that while the two species shared a space, they did not function as a cohesive social group, as both groups exhibited low rates of mixed-species associations. These results suggest that simply sharing an environment is insufficient for the animals to be considered a group. By better understanding the ecological pressures that favour mixed-species groups in the wild, we can apply this knowledge in captivity to encourage more naturalistic behaviours.

Abstract

Mixed-species groups have been recorded in various primates, including tufted capuchin and squirrel monkeys. Measures of their ‘groupness’ are typically based on factors such as group stability, social interactions, proximity, or behavioural coordination. Social network analysis has become a useful tool for quantifying relationships among group-living individuals. Here, we apply social network analysis to two captive mixed-species groups of tufted capuchins and squirrel monkeys housed at the Living Links to Human Evolution Research Centre, Royal Zoological Society of Scotland, Edinburgh Zoo, UK. We conducted 183 h of focal observations (three hours per individual, excluding co-observations) and calculated association rates using a simple index ratio. Permutation t-tests were used to assess differences in the overall mixed-species network and network metrics according to species. While the two species exhibited some level of association, they formed separate clusters in the mixed-species networks; however, the East group had more balanced group sizes and showed some signs of closer inter-specific social ties compared to the West group. Our data indicate that, in captivity at least, while these groups co-exist in a small, shared space, they do not form cohesive mixed-species groups. We suggest caution in the assumption of mixed-species groups based on shared space only.

1. Introduction

There are a number of taxa that have been reported to form mixed-species groups in the wild, particularly birds, fish and mammals [1]. The ‘dual benefit’ of grouping (i.e., reduced risk of predation and increased foraging success) has been provided as an explanation for this natural phenomenon [2]. However, it is often unclear how to determine what makes a group, especially when considering mixed-species. In the existing literature, there appears to be agreement on the following prerequisites for animal ‘grouping’ [3,4,5], which can be applied to both single-species and mixed-species groups, namely, there should be: (1) group stability, (2) an element of socialness [6] that is, interactions, (3) proximity and behavioural coordination in time and space, and (4) a minimum of two individuals. The first three require elaboration on how to define and measure whether these prerequisites are met. Group stability refers to the group maintaining a recognisable configuration over time, even as individual members join or leave. While the relative positions of individuals may shift, a degree of continuity among members is essential. The element of socialness may be directly observed by social attraction, where individuals seek out the presence of others in the group, which also serves to maintain the group. This association may be direct, such as physical contact or social behaviours, such as play and grooming, or indirect, such as through vocal communication, with physical proximity serving as a useful proxy for social engagement. Lastly, behavioural coordination in time and space implies that individuals are engaged in similar activities simultaneously and within a meaningful distance, allowing for both direct (e.g., physical) and indirect (e.g., vocal or visual) interactions. However, the ability to quantify social structure and the mechanisms and functions that underpin it has long been a challenge within the area of ethology, especially when considering the link between individual and group behaviour [7,8,9].
Patterns of association can arise due to individuals sharing identical or similar motivations and behaviours, such as the degree to which they are hungry, thirsty or how they respond to a potential threat [7,8,9,10]. Yet we know that social relationships and social structure are more complex than this, with individuals often engaging in non-random interactions and movements that require the development of behavioural strategies, resulting in the formation of a multi-layered and multi-behavioural structure [10,11]. Socially complex species such as non-human primates have a diverse repertoire of social behaviours and interactions that differ in type (e.g., cooperative, antagonistic, or sexual) as well as frequency and duration (e.g., years, days or minutes). Furthermore, the type of interactions that occur will likely depend on factors such as kinship, dominance, body size, sex, age, and general health (e.g., parasite load) of the participating individuals [7,8,10]. In the absence of kinship (e.g., unrelated conspecifics or heterospecifics), social behaviour can be explained by mutualism (i.e., shared benefits) and manipulation (i.e., benefits are disproportionate [12,13]). Therefore, to better understand social structure and the link between the behaviour of individuals within mixed-species groups, we need to analyse the relationships of all group members [7,11,14,15].
While this is not a new concept, within-group, between-individual links are often difficult to measure quantitatively. One approach is the use of social network analysis, based on mathematical graph theory [7,8,9], that examines social relationships (direct and indirect) via the construction of networks. Using social network analysis to gain insights into the position and role of the individual within a group enables building a more complete picture of the complex sociality and structure of group-living animals. In a captive setting, it has been used to examine the impact of environmental change, such as moving to a new enclosure [16], spatial association and social preferences [17,18], demographic changes (births, deaths, maturation [19]) and how manipulating group composition can improve social stability and reduce levels of intra-specific aggression [20,21].
Thus far, social network analysis has been applied to the study of mixed-species bird flocks [15,22,23,24], fish assemblages affected by non-native species invasions [25], and mixed-species groups of savannah herbivores [26]. Of the platyrrhine primates to form mixed-species groups, the most commonly reported are the tufted capuchins (Sapajus sp.) and squirrel (Saimiri sp.) monkeys [27,28,29,30], spending ~50% of their time in temporary but stable groupings [29]. Tufted capuchins (hereafter capuchins) are approximately three times the size (mean weight ~3 kg) of squirrel monkeys (mean weight ~0.74 kg), and as such, they are considered to be dominant over them [31]. Furthermore, capuchins are generally found to be tolerant towards subordinate others, including non-kin and other species [32,33], and compared to many cercopithecines such as rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) [34], and olive baboons (Papio anubis) [35], both capuchins and squirrel monkeys exhibit relatively low rates of aggression [30,36]. Perhaps it is for these reasons that this species combination has been successfully housed in captivity [37,38,39]. The two groups of Guianan tufted capuchins (Sapajus apella) and Guianan squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) housed at the Living Links Research Centre, Royal Zoological Society of Scotland at Edinburgh Zoo, UK (hereafter Living Links—see Figure 1) have been living together since 2008. While mixed-species exhibits often benefit from larger and more naturalistic enclosures, comparatively, the inhabitants are still forced into much closer proximity than they would be in the wild. Furthermore, with little to no risk of predation and food provisioning, more time can be allotted to intra- and perhaps inter-specific interactions. But without the ecological pressures that favour grouping in the wild, can merely living together in a shared space result in the formation of mixed-species groups?
Here, we report on the use of social network analysis as a method to quantify the ‘groupness’ of the two mixed-species groups at Living Links. Previous research has found that the monkeys display low rates of both inter-specific and intra-specific aggression, and affiliative behaviours such as play and grooming have rarely been observed [16,38,39], which is in keeping with observations in the wild [27,28,29,30]. Therefore, spatial proximity was considered as a proxy measure for network associations [40,41] within mixed-species networks, as individuals are typically unable to interact (e.g., cooperate) or react (e.g., flee from a predator) if they are not close enough (in proximity) to other individuals [8,42,43]. A review by Kasper and Voelkl [40] presented findings from a network analysis of 70 primate groups (30 different species; 36 captive; 28 wild), indicating that network density (Table 2) was generally high ( x - = 0.57, range = 0.49–0.93). Accordingly, we predict that overall mixed-species network densities will be high (≥0.5). However, based on previous studies examining space use [44] and behavioural synchrony [45] in the Living Links monkeys, we expect there to be few associations between species within the mixed-species networks. As dominant individuals typically occupy more central roles in their networks [46,47,48], we predict that capuchins, as the larger and more dominant species, will demonstrate higher centrality measures than squirrel monkeys. Their size and tolerance toward other species make capuchins key connectors within the network. While previous research has focused on space use and behavioural synchrony to assess groupness in the Living Links monkeys [44,45], the present study employs social network analysis as a novel tool for quantifying ‘groupness’ in mixed-species groups. Social network analysis offers a more nuanced understanding of the social dynamics between species, complementing previous methods [44,45]. Therefore, we aim to evaluate the effectiveness of social network analysis in measuring ‘groupness’ for the first time in this context.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Subjects

Two groups of Guianan brown tufted capuchins (Sapajus apella) and Guianan squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) housed in a mixed exhibit at Living Links (Figure 1) were studied. The West group consisted of 18 capuchins and 9 squirrel monkeys, and the East group consisted of 17 capuchins and 17 squirrel monkeys (Table 1). Both West and East sides include separate indoor enclosures for capuchins (7 m × 4.5 m × 6 m high) and squirrel monkeys (5.5 m × 4.5 m × 6 m high—with size restricted entrance/exit so capuchins cannot enter), as well as a shared outdoor enclosure of approximately 900 m2. Indoor temperatures were approximately 24 °C, maintained using radiant ceiling heaters and an air conditioning system, while outdoor temperature ranged between 12 °C and 25 °C during the study period. For daily husbandry routines, see [38].

2.2. Observational Sampling

We recorded 183 h of focal observations (three hours per individual, not including instances when individuals were observed in proximity during focal follows of other individuals) between April and August 2015. Monkeys were sampled evenly between 09:00 and 17:30, using instantaneous point sampling methods following Martin and Bateson [49]. The focal animal was selected at random, and the observer (SD-S) would count 5 s before beginning recording to ensure that there was no choice or visibility bias in the observed grouping(s). Each focal was recorded at 1 minute intervals for 10 minutes and sampled 18 times over the study period. In each point sample, all group members (capuchins and squirrel monkeys) within two body lengths (~40 cm, based on the size of the largest monkey in the group) from the focal monkey were noted [50,51]. This measure was applied to both species to standardise the measure for the purpose of mixed-species measures. If no monkey was within two body lengths, then the focal was described as solitary. Two body lengths were used as a cut-off point for social proximity as this was often the minimum distance that an individual allowed another individual to approach before they reacted (e.g., attend to or move away; pers. obs.). If a focal was out of sight for more than three minutes, then the focal was abandoned and re-sampled at another time. For mixed-species group analysis, where affiliative or aggressive interactions between species may be rare, building proximity networks is appropriate. In order for behaviour(s) and social information to transfer between individuals, they typically need to be in proximity, which is why proximity is a commonly used measure in primate social network analysis [16,40,52].

2.3. Data Analysis

All statistical tests were performed using the software package R (v.4.4.1) in the RStudio environment ([53,54]—see code and data https://osf.io/kax6s/?view_only=1b5e1bd3600345a69cc82d37ecfa9e23, accessed on 19 November 2024), with an α of 0.05 [55] for the overall network analysis. For the multiple testing of specific network metrics, we applied a Bonferroni correction, adjusting the significance level to 0.0125 (0.05/4). The p-values from the permutation tests were calculated as the proportion of cases in the distribution from the randomised datasets that were equal to or more extreme than the observed measure. We created networks using the asnipe package [56], and the package igraph [57] to calculate network metrics.
Association data were collected by repeatedly sampling the Living Links groups, recording who was in proximity (associating) with whom in each sampling period. We calculated association rates between dyads, using a simple ratio index (SRI) as described by Hoppitt and Farine [9]:
x / ( y a + y b + y a b + x ) ,   assuming   y N u l l = 0
where x is the number of sampling periods with a and b observed associated, y a is the number of sampling periods with just a identified, y b is the number of sampling periods with just b identified, y a b is the number of sampling periods with a and b identified but not associated and y N u l l the number of sampling periods with neither a nor b identified. Values closer to zero indicate low association rates, while values closer to one indicate high association rates (i.e., individuals were observed more frequently in proximity).
We used permutation tests, following the approach outlined by Farine [56], to assess the significance of observed associations within the mixed-species network, using 1000 iterations of the data. In addition, we conducted permutation t-tests, also based on 1000 iterations, to compare key network metrics—degree centrality, eigenvector centrality, betweenness centrality, and clustering coefficient (see also Table 2)—between capuchins and squirrel monkeys in both the West and East groups. Permutation tests are particularly valuable when working with smaller, unequally distributed samples, as they do not rely on the assumption of normality and are more robust than traditional non-parametric tests that are often based on ranked data. Finally, the difference from the original data is compared to the differences from the permuted data. If the original difference is greater or less than most of the differences from the random permutations, this suggests that the observed difference is unlikely to be due to random chance.
Specifically, a permutation t-test is a non-parametric method that compares two groups (such as species) to determine if there is a significant difference in a given variable. First, the difference in means for a given variable is calculated from the original data between the two species. Next, the original values for each individual are randomly shuffled to create new datasets where the assignment of species to data points is random for x number of iterations, and the difference between the two permuted groups is calculated for each shuffle. Finally, the difference from the original data is compared to the differences from the permuted data.
Permutation tests and network diagrams were visualised using the ggplot2 package [58] in RStudio. A node refers to an individual monkey and is displayed as a square (capuchin) or circle (squirrel monkey) in the network diagrams. An edge (i.e., a line) indicates the frequency that a node was in proximity with another node (Table 2).

3. Results

During focal observations, the West group squirrel monkeys were rarely observed in shared enclosures with the capuchins (outdoor enclosure = 18%, capuchin indoor enclosure = 1%). In contrast, members of both species in the East group were observed for over half of focal observations in shared enclosures (outdoors = 34%; capuchin indoor enclosure = 20%). The overall network density for the East group was calculated to be 0.733, indicating a highly interconnected network where most potential associations were observed. In comparison, the West group had a lower network density of 0.590, suggesting a less interconnected structure with fewer associations between individuals (Figure 2). Furthermore, the mean association rates between dyads from the overall mixed-species network data were 0.032 (SD ± 0.056) for the West group and 0.022 (SD ± 0.04) for the East group. When compared to 1000 randomly generated networks, the association rates in both the East and West groups were significantly lower (p < 0.0001), indicating that the observed associations occurred less frequently than would be expected by chance.
To further assess the efficiency of communication and potential interaction within the mixed-species networks, we calculated the average path length, which measures the average number of steps along the shortest paths for all possible pairs of nodes in the network. In the context of these mixed-species networks, a ‘step’ represents moving from one node to another through proximity-based connections, where each step reflects the spatial closeness or how many individuals (or spaces) one must ‘pass through’ within the network. The average path lengths for the East capuchins and squirrel monkeys were both 0.006 (SD ± capuchins = 0.0003, squirrel monkeys = 0.001). In contrast, the average path length for the West capuchins was 0.013 (SD ± 0.004), whereas that for squirrel monkeys was 0.0.016 (SD ± 0.008). These results indicate that the East group has a more efficient and cohesive mixed-species network compared to the West group, with more opportunities for communication and interaction between species, as reflected by their shorter average path lengths.
When examining the network metrics derived from the original data for capuchins and squirrel monkeys (Table 3), we performed a series of permutation-independent samples t-tests based on 1000 iterations (Figure 3 and Figure 4). For both the West (observed difference = 0.463) and East (observed difference = 0.387) groups, capuchins exhibited significantly higher eigenvector centrality compared to squirrel monkeys (p < 0.0001), indicating that the observed differences are unlikely to be due to chance. When examining clustering coefficients, we found no significant difference for species in the East group (observed difference = −0.001, p > 0.05). In contrast, in the West group, capuchins were significantly more tightly clustered in the mixed-species network compared to squirrel monkeys (observed difference = 0.115, p < 0.0001), indicating that this difference is highly unlikely to have occurred by random chance.
For betweenness centrality, the West capuchins exhibited significantly lower betweenness centrality compared to squirrel monkeys (observed difference = −11.364, p < 0.0001), indicating that within their mixed-species network, squirrel monkeys are likely to play a more bridging role. The opposite trend was found in the East group (observed difference = 4.855, p < 0.0001), suggesting that the capuchins may serve as an important bridge between species in the mixed network. Regarding degree centrality, the East capuchins exhibited significantly lower degree centrality (observed difference = −1.529, p < 0.0001) compared to squirrel monkeys, indicating that the East capuchins associate with fewer individuals than the squirrel monkeys in the mixed-species network. The opposite trend was found in the West group, with capuchins displaying significantly higher degree centrality (observed difference = 7.333) than squirrel monkeys (p < 0.0001).

4. Discussion

Network densities for both the West and East groups were high (≥0.5), which is consistent with previous research on primate social network analysis [40]. However, despite these high densities, there was a lack of mixed-species aggregation, with network diagrams showing distinctly separate clusters for both the West and East capuchins and squirrel monkeys (Figure 2). These findings are in keeping with previous studies investigating the groupness of the Living Links mixed-species groups using space [44] and behavioural synchrony [45], which found that the monkeys were using different areas of shared enclosures (squirrel monkeys on the periphery and capuchins more central zones) and were asynchronous in their behaviours. The strength of ties between species in the West group was likely weaker due to the low percentage of time that squirrel monkeys were observed in shared enclosures with the capuchins (outdoor: 18%, capuchin indoor: 1%). In contrast, members of both species in the East group were observed for over half of focal observations in shared enclosures (outdoors = 34%; capuchin indoor enclosure = 20%). Additionally, the geodesic distance was visibly larger in the West sociogram compared to the East (Figure 2). However, despite these increased opportunities for coming into proximity and the shorter geodesic distance between clusters in the East group, the sociograms still showed distinct species clusters, similar to those in the West (Figure 2), which could indicate that the smaller squirrel monkeys may be actively avoiding coming into proximity with the capuchins, reinforcing the notion that simply having shared space does not translate to forming a mixed-species group.
The analysis of average path length within the mixed-species networks highlighted differences in network efficiency between capuchins and squirrel monkeys between groups. In the East group, both capuchins and squirrel monkeys had similar and short average path lengths, indicating that overall, the mixed-species network is efficient (i.e., individuals can interact with one another with minimal distance and effort, promoting quicker and more effective exchanges of information, social interactions or simply coming into proximity with one another) and cohesive with ample opportunities for communication and association between species. In contrast, in the West group, capuchins had shorter average path lengths compared to squirrel monkeys, suggesting that within their mixed-species network, capuchins are more efficiently connected, potentially facilitating quicker and more direct associations (i.e., coming into proximity more frequently) within the group. These differences highlight the importance of between-species associations and their role in shaping network connectivity and social dynamics within mixed-species groups.
At the species level, eigenvector centrality was significantly higher in the capuchins compared to squirrel monkeys in both the West and East groups, suggesting that capuchins are more centrally positioned within the overall network. This finding supports our prediction and indicates that the capuchins may be more influential (i.e., they are well-connected with other highly connected individuals) in forming connections within the mixed-species network.
When examining the West group, capuchins exhibited a significantly higher clustering coefficient than squirrel monkeys within the mixed-species network. This difference suggests that capuchins form more tightly connected subgroups, while squirrel monkeys are less cohesive within their own social structure as well as the overall mixed-species network. The lack of a significant difference in clustering coefficients between the East capuchins and squirrel monkeys suggests that both species exhibit similar levels of local connectivity or cohesion within the mixed-species networks. This implies that, on average, each species forms similarly cohesive subgroups with comparable tendencies to form tightly connected clusters. However, when examining the sociogram (Figure 2), there are three distinct clusters (one for the squirrel monkeys and two for the capuchins); therefore, while clustering coefficients appear similar across species, they do not necessarily indicate integration across species within the network. Instead, they point to a situation where the two capuchin clusters and the single squirrel monkey cluster form cohesive but separate subgroups within the overall network. Thus, cohesion within species does not necessarily lead to cohesion between species. Notably, when further examining the East sociogram (Figure 2), one of the capuchin clusters is a smaller group (n = 5). These individuals are unrelated to the alpha female and have comparatively low eigenvector centrality scores (≤0.2) to the rest of the capuchins (>0.3), which indicates that they are low in the hierarchy [48].
Furthermore, they are on the periphery of the main capuchin cluster and also the squirrel monkey cluster. If we consider Hamilton’s selfish herd theory [59] and one of the reasons for forming mixed-species groups being reduced risk of predation [2], then it makes sense for lower-ranking capuchins who will also likely be positioned on the periphery of wild capuchin groups to be in proximity with squirrel monkeys when in a mixed-species group. Additionally, in relation to the detection of threats, capuchins have been found to benefit from the vigilance of squirrel monkeys [28]. Moreover, Hirsch’s [60] study on wild capuchins found that vigilance by subordinate individuals was attributed to predator detection as well as social monitoring of dominant conspecifics. This suggests that vigilance for social monitoring enables subordinate individuals to avoid agonistic interactions. Therefore, within a captive environment where there is minimal to no risk of predation, being in proximity with squirrel monkeys (who are generally more vigilant) could benefit lower-ranking capuchins by alerting them to potential threats such as aggression from more dominant capuchins.
The higher betweenness centrality for the East capuchins indicates their role as important bridges between species in the mixed-species network, despite having fewer direct connections compared to the squirrel monkeys that had significantly higher degree centrality. This intermediary position means that capuchins, while less directly connected, play a role in linking different parts of the social network. This difference highlights how degree centrality and betweenness centrality capture different roles within the mixed-species network. Higher degree centrality, as seen in the squirrel monkeys, reflects extensive social integration and frequent direct associations, whereas higher betweenness centrality, as seen in the capuchins, captures a role that bridges separate clusters and connects otherwise isolated parts of the network. However, we found the opposite in the West group, where the capuchins had significantly greater degree centrality, engaging with more individuals than squirrel monkeys. This may simply reflect the larger number of capuchins (n = 18) to squirrel monkeys (n = 9) in this group, although they had the same opportunities as the East group to enter shared enclosure spaces. During focal observations, however, squirrel monkeys were rarely observed in shared spaces or in proximity to capuchins. Moreover, although the West squirrel monkeys had lower degree centrality, they exhibited higher betweenness centrality, suggesting that they occupy the bridging role within this mixed-species network.
Overall, it is evident from the social network analyses that neither the West nor East groups behaved or interacted as a fully cohesive mixed-species group. However, the East group showed higher levels of association, greater network efficiency, and shorter geodesic distances. While they did not form a fully integrated network, these factors indicate that the East group was closer to functioning as a mixed-species group than the West group. In contrast, the West group exhibited more distinct species clusters and a larger geodesic distance, reflecting less proximity and integration between the species. Additionally, the low percentage of time the squirrel monkeys spent in shared enclosures with the capuchins further supports the idea of more limited association and weaker social ties within this group. This likely contributed to the lower cohesion observed in the West group’s mixed-species network. Nevertheless, these observations highlight that in the absence of ecological pressures faced in the wild (e.g., risk of predation, limited food availability), animals that would typically form mixed-species groups are unlikely to do so in a captive environment.
However, there could be other factors contributing to a lack of proximity between species. For instance, the group composition of the West monkeys (capuchins = 18, squirrel monkeys = 9) was markedly different to the East that had equal numbers (n = 17), and as squirrel monkeys are much smaller than the capuchins, the size difference plus this discrepancy in group size may have reduced the likelihood of the squirrel monkeys coming into proximity with the capuchins. Furthermore, the environment itself may have contributed to a lack of proximity between species. For instance, Daoudi et al. [44] observed that the Living Links monkeys used distinct areas of their outdoor enclosure reflecting natural ecological niches [27]. Since these data were collected, refurbishments to the outdoor enclosures have been implemented to promote greater space use by both species, and group sizes have changed. These changes may impact the mixed-species networks, highlighting the importance of long-term monitoring, as such changes can have a considerable impact on the social dynamics of the group. Therefore, continuous assessment can support both behavioural understanding and the management of zoo husbandry, guiding decisions for optimal enclosure design and group composition in mixed-species exhibits.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, social network analysis has proven to be a valuable tool for quantifying associations within mixed-species groups, providing important insights into how capuchins and squirrel monkeys associate in a captive setting. While both species largely formed separate clusters, indicating limited integration, the findings suggest that other factors beyond ecological pressures—such as group composition, enclosure design and individual species’ behavioural tendencies—might be influencing these social dynamics. Ultimately, our results highlight that in the absence of ecological pressures, such as predation risk and resource competition, which typically drive the formation of mixed-species groups in the wild [2], structural and social factors specific to captive environments become key. For example, imbalances in group size or species-specific preferences for habitat use may limit opportunities for aggregation and integration [44]. Furthermore, individual behaviour, rank and differences in social structures could also contribute to the persistence of distinct species clusters, even when shared space is available [45].
These findings suggest that while mixed-species groups in captivity may not form mixed-species groups as cohesive as expected, the use of social network analysis provides a nuanced understanding of these associations, allowing researchers to identify factors that either promote or hinder social cohesion. This approach offers valuable insights that could support zoo husbandry and management decisions regarding enclosure design and group composition. Future research should, therefore, continue to explore how these various factors influence mixed-species groups in captivity, potentially using more advanced network approaches such as multi-layer networks [19,61] to capture the complexity of social relationships and interactions within groups.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.D.-S. and H.M.B.-S.; methodology, S.D.-S. and H.M.B.-S.; software, S.D.-S.; formal analysis, S.D.-S.; writing—original draft preparation, S.D.-S.; writing—review and editing, S.D.-S., H.M.B.-S., P.L.; visualization, S.D.-S.; supervision, H.M.B.-S., P.L.; project administration, S.D.-S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Edinburgh Zoo’s Living Links Research Facility is core supported by the RZSS (Registered charity number: SC004064) through funding generated by its visitors, members and supporters, and by the University of St Andrews (Registered charity number: SC013532) who core supports the maintenance and management costs of the research facility.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The research conducted was observational and non-invasive and abided by the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour Ethical Guidelines [62] and the British Psychological Society’s guidelines for psychologists working with animals [63]. The research was also approved by the University of Stirling Psychology Ethics Committee, Newcastle University Animal Welfare Review Board, the Royal Zoological Society of Scotland, Living Links Management Board, and adhered to the legal requirements in the United Kingdom.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data supporting the results reported in this study, along with the relevant code, are publicly available and can be accessed https://osf.io/kax6s/?view_only=1b5e1bd3600345a69cc82d37ecfa9e23, accessed on 19 November 2024.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Royal Zoological Society of Scotland (RZSS) and the University of St Andrews for core financial support to the RZSS Edinburgh Zoo’s Living Links Research Facility where this project was carried out. We thank the RZSS keeping and veterinary staff for their care of animals and technical support throughout this project. We would also like to thank Dr Blake Morton for his input on the study design. Edinburgh Zoo’s Living Links Research Facility is core supported by the RZSS (Registered charity number: SC004064) through funding generated by its visitors, members and supporters and by the University of St Andrews (Registered charity number: SC013532) who core supports the maintenance and management costs of the research facility. We would also like to express our thanks and gratitude to Damien Farine for his valuable assistance and advice regarding the analysis.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest

References

  1. Carlson, N.V.; Freeberg, T.M.; Goodale, E.; Theo, A.H. Mixed-species groups and aggregations: Shaping ecological and behavioural patterns and processes. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 2023, 378, 20220093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Pulliam, H.R.; Pyke, G.H.; Caraco, T. The scanning behaviour of juncos: A game theoretical approach. J. Theor. Biol. 1982, 95, 89–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Wilson, E.O. Sociobiology: The New Synthesis; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar]
  4. Parrish, J.K.; Hamner, W.M. Animal Groups in Three Dimensions: How Species Aggregate; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  5. Ward, A.; Webster, M. Sociality: Behaviour of Group-Living Animals; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  6. Tokunaga, D.H.; Mitchell, G. Factors affecting sex differences in nonhuman primate grooming. J. Behav. Sci. 1977, 2, 259–269. [Google Scholar]
  7. Hinde, R.A. Interactions, relationships and social structure. Man 1976, 11, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Croft, D.P.; Madden, J.R.; Franks, D.W.; James, R. Hypothesis testing in animal social networks. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2011, 26, 502–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Hoppitt, W.J.E.; Farine, D.R. Association indices for quantifying social relationships: How to deal with missing observations of individuals or groups. Anim. Behav. 2018, 136, 227–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Croft, D.P.; Darden, S.K.; Wey, T.W. Current directions in animal social networks. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2016, 12, 52–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hemelrijk, C.K. Models of, and tests for, reciprocity, unidirectionality and other social interaction patterns at a group level. Anim. Behav. 1990, 39, 1013–1029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Whiten, A.; Byrne, R.W. Tactical deception in primates. Behav. Brain Sci. 1988, 11, 233–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Clutton-Brock, T. Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies. Nature 2009, 462, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Sueur, C.; Jacobs, A.; Amblard, F.; Petit, O.; King, A.J. How can social network analysis improve the study of primate behavior? Am. J. Primatol. 2011, 73, 703–719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Farine, D.R.; Garroway, C.J.; Sheldon, B.C. Social network analysis of mixed-species flocks: Exploring the structure and evolution of interspecific social behaviour. Anim. Behav. 2012, 84, 1271–1277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Dufour, V.; Sueur, C.; Whiten, A.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M. The impact of moving to a novel environment on social networks, activity and wellbeing in two new world primates. Am. J. Primatol. 2011, 73, 802–811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Clark, F.E. Space to choose: Network analysis of social preferences in a captive chimpanzee community, and implications for management. Am. J. Primatol. 2011, 73, 748–757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Koyama, N.F. Matrilineal cohesion and social networks in Macaca fuscata. Int. J. Primatol. 2003, 24, 797–811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Pereira, A.S.; Rebelo, I.D.; Cassanova, C.; Lee, P.C.; Louca, V. The multidimensionality of female mandrill sociality—A dynamic multiplex network. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0230942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Beisner, B.A.; Jin, J.; Fushing, H.; McCowan, B. Detection of social group instability among captive rhesus macaques using joint network modeling. Curr. Zool. 2015, 61, 70–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. McCowan, B.; Anderson, K.; Heagarty, A.; Cameron, A. Utility of social network analysis for primate behavioral management and well-being. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 109, 396–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Aplin, L.M.; Farine, D.R.; Morand-Ferron, J.; Sheldon, B.C. Social networks predict patch discovery in a wild population of songbirds. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2012, 279, 4199–4205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Farine, D.R.; Milburn, P.J. Social organisation of thornbill-dominated mixed-species flocks using social network analysis. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2013, 67, 321–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Marthy, W.; Farine, D.R. The potential impacts of the songbird trade on mixed-species flocking. Biol. Conserv. 2018, 222, 222–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Beyer, K.; Gozlan, R.E.; Copp, G.H. Social network properties within a fish assemblage invaded by non-native sunbleak Leucaspius delineatus. Ecol. Model. 2010, 221, 2118–2122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Meise, K.; Franks, D.W.; Bro-Jørgensen, J. Using social network analysis of mixed-species groups in African savannah herbivores to assess how community structure responds to environmental change. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2019, 374, 20190009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Fleagle, J.G.; Mittermeier, R.A.; Skopec, A.L. Differential habitat use by Cebus apella and Saimiri sciureus in central Surinam. Primates 1981, 22, 361–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Terborgh, J. Five New World Primates: A Study in Comparative Ecology; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  29. Podolsky, R.D. Effects of mixed-species association on resource use by Saimiri sciureus and Cebus apella. Am. J. Primatol. 1990, 21, 147–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Boinski, S. The social organizations of squirrel monkeys: Implications for ecological models of social evolution. Evol. Anthropol. 1999, 8, 101–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Rylands, A.N.; Mittermeier, R.A. Family Cebidae (squirrel monkeys and capuchins). In Handbook of the Mammals of the World, Volume 3, Primates; Mittermeier, R.A., Rylands, A.B., Wilson, D.E., Eds.; Lynx Edicions: Barcelona, Spain, 2013; pp. 348–413. [Google Scholar]
  32. Ferreira, R.G.; Izar, P.; Lee, P.C. Exchange, affiliation, and protective interventions in semifree-ranging brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Am. J. Primatol. 2006, 68, 765–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Schino, G.; Boggiani, L.; Mortelliti, A.; Pinzaglia, M.; Addessi, E. Testing the two sides of indirect reciprocity in tufted capuchin monkeys. Behav. Process. 2021, 182, 104290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. McCowan, B.; Beisner, B.A.; Capitanio, J.P.; Jackson, M.E.; Cameron, A.N.; Seil, S.; Fushing, H. Network stability is a balancing act of personality, power, and conflict dynamics in rhesus macaque societies. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e22350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. MacCormick, H.A.; MacNulty, D.R.; Bosacker, A.L.; Lehman, C.; Bailey, A.; Collins, D.A.; Packer, C. Male and female aggression: Lessons from sex, rank, age, and injury in olive baboons. Behav. Ecol. 2012, 23, 684–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Cooper, M.A.; Bernstein, I.S.; Fragaszy, D.M.; de Waal, F.B.M. Integration of new males into four social groups of tufted capuchins (Cebus apella). Int. J. Primatol. 2001, 22, 663–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Sodaro, V. Housing and exhibiting mixed species of Neotropical primates. In Callitrichid Husbandry Manual; Saunders, N., Sodaro, V., Eds.; Neotropical Primate Taxon Advisory Group: Mosman, Australia, 1999; pp. 7–34. [Google Scholar]
  38. Leonardi, R.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M.; Dufour, V.; MacDonald, C.; Whiten, A. Living together: Behavior and welfare in single and mixed species groups of capuchin (Cebus apella) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Am. J. Primatol. 2010, 72, 33–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Buchanan-Smith, H.M.; Griciute, J.; Daoudi, S.; Leonardi, R.; Whiten, A. Interspecific interactions and welfare implications in mixed species communities of capuchin (Sapajus apella) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) over 3 years. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 147, 324–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kasper, C.; Voelkl, B. A social network analysis of primate groups. Primates 2009, 50, 343–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Lehmann, J.; Ross, C. Baboon (Papio anubis) social complexity—A network approach. Am. J. Primatol. 2011, 73, 775–789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Silk, J.B.; Alberts, S.C.; Altmann, J. Patterns of coalition formation by adult female baboons in Amboseli, Kenya. Anim. Behav. 2004, 67, 573–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Zhang, P.; Li, B.G.; Qi, X.G.; MacIntosh, A.J.; Watanabe, K. A proximity-based social network of a group of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana). Int. J. Primatol. 2012, 33, 1081–1095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Daoudi, S.; Badihi, G.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M. Is mixed-species living cognitively enriching? Enclosure use and welfare in two captive groups of tufted capuchins (Sapajus apella) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2017, 4, 51–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Daoudi-Simison, S.; O’Sullivan, E.; Moat, G.; Lee, P.C.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M. Do mixed-species groups of capuchin (Sapajus apella) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) synchronize their behaviour? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2023, 378, 20220111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Kanngiesser, P.; Sueur, C.; Riedl, K.; Grossmann, J.; Call, J. Grooming network cohesion and the role of individuals in a captive chimpanzee group. Am. J. Primatol. 2011, 73, 758–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lusseau, D.; Newman, M.E. Identifying the role that animals play in their social networks. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2004, 271, S477–S481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Wooddell, L.J.; Kaburu, S.S.; Dettmer, A.M. Dominance rank predicts social network position across developmental stages in rhesus monkeys. Am. J. Primatol. 2020, 82, e23024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Martin, P.; Bateson, P.P.G. Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  50. Morton, F.B.; Weiss, A.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M.; Lee, P.C. Capuchin monkeys with similar personalities have higher-quality relationships independent of age, sex, kinship and rank. Anim. Behav. 2015, 105, 163–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Morton, F.B.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M.; Brosnan, S.F.; Thierry, B.; Paukner, A.; Essler, J.L.; Marcum, C.S.; Lee, P.C. Studying animal innovation at the individual level: A ratings-based assessment in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] sp.). J. Comp. Psychol. 2021, 135, 258–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. de Lima, V.C.C.; Ferreira, R.G. Social network changes during the development of immature capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.). Primates 2021, 62, 801–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  54. RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development for R; RStudio, Inc.: Boston, MA, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  55. Fisher, R.A. The arrangement of field experiments. In Breakthroughs in Statistics; Kotz, S., Johnson, N.L., Eds.; Springer Series in Statistics; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 82–91. [Google Scholar]
  56. Farine, D.R. Animal social network inference and permutations for ecologists in R using asnipe. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2013, 4, 1187–1194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Csárdi, G.; Nepusz, T.; Traag, V.; Horvát, S.; Zanini, F.; Noom, D.; Müller, K. igraph: Network Analysis and Visualization in R; R Package Version 2.1.1; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Wickham, H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2016; ISBN 978-3-319-24277-4. Available online: https://ggplot2.tidyverse.org (accessed on 11 November 2024).
  59. Hamilton, W.D. Geometry for the selfish herd. J. Theor. Biol. 1971, 31, 295–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Hirsch, B.T. Social monitoring and vigilance behavior in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2002, 52, 458–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Finn, K.R.; Silk, M.J.; Porter, M.A.; Pinter-Wollman, N. The use of multilayer network analysis in animal behaviour. Anim. Behav. 2019, 149, 7–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Buchanan, K.; de Perera, T.B.; Carere, C.; Carter, T.; Hailey, A.; Hubrecht, R.; Jennings, D.; Metcalfe, N.; Pitcher, T.; Peron, F.; et al. Guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioural research and teaching. Anim. Behav. 2012, 83, 301–309. [Google Scholar]
  63. Research Board of the British Psychological Society. Guidelines for Psychologists Working with Animals; The British Psychological Society: Leicester, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Schematic diagram (approximately to scale) of the Living Links West (WS = west squirrel monkey, WC = west capuchin) and East (ES = east squirrel monkey, EC = east capuchin) enclosures (adapted from [38]).
Figure 1. Schematic diagram (approximately to scale) of the Living Links West (WS = west squirrel monkey, WC = west capuchin) and East (ES = east squirrel monkey, EC = east capuchin) enclosures (adapted from [38]).
Animals 14 03360 g001
Figure 2. Mixed-species social networks for the (A) West and (B) East groups. Node shape is based on species: capuchins = square and squirrel monkeys = circle. Sex (capuchins: yellow = female; cyan = male; squirrel monkeys: red = female, blue = male). Node size is based on degree centrality, the strength of ties is based on the frequency of interactions between nodes and the distance between ties is based on the geodesic distance calculated as the sum of the weights of ties along the shortest path.
Figure 2. Mixed-species social networks for the (A) West and (B) East groups. Node shape is based on species: capuchins = square and squirrel monkeys = circle. Sex (capuchins: yellow = female; cyan = male; squirrel monkeys: red = female, blue = male). Node size is based on degree centrality, the strength of ties is based on the frequency of interactions between nodes and the distance between ties is based on the geodesic distance calculated as the sum of the weights of ties along the shortest path.
Animals 14 03360 g002
Figure 3. Permutation t-test distributions based on 1000 permutations of the network metrics (eigenvector centrality, clustering coefficient, betweenness centrality, and degree centrality) for the West capuchins and squirrel monkeys. Vertical dashed lines represent the observed difference.
Figure 3. Permutation t-test distributions based on 1000 permutations of the network metrics (eigenvector centrality, clustering coefficient, betweenness centrality, and degree centrality) for the West capuchins and squirrel monkeys. Vertical dashed lines represent the observed difference.
Animals 14 03360 g003
Figure 4. Permutation t-test distributions based on 1000 permutations of the network metrics (eigenvector centrality, clustering coefficient, betweenness centrality, and degree centrality) for the East capuchins and squirrel monkeys. Vertical dashed lines represent the observed difference.
Figure 4. Permutation t-test distributions based on 1000 permutations of the network metrics (eigenvector centrality, clustering coefficient, betweenness centrality, and degree centrality) for the East capuchins and squirrel monkeys. Vertical dashed lines represent the observed difference.
Animals 14 03360 g004
Table 1. Demographic information for the West and East capuchins (Sapajus apella) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) at Living Links.
Table 1. Demographic information for the West and East capuchins (Sapajus apella) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) at Living Links.
NameSpeciesSexAge Range (in Years) During StudyExhibit
DiegocapuchinM12–13West
LanacapuchinF19West
SantiagocapuchinF13West
SylvaniacapuchinF11–12West
TokacapuchinM10West
FigocapuchinM9West
PedracapuchinF7West
MekoecapuchinM7West
InticapuchinM5West
RufocapuchinM5West
XimocapuchinM5West
TorrescapuchinM4West
LunacapuchinF4West
AlbacapuchinF3West
Mr FudgecapuchinM1–2West
BearcapuchinM1–2West
HazelcapuchinF1West
PixiecapuchinF1West
Hugosquirrel monkeyM4–5West
Gerdasquirrel monkeyF15West
Jasminsquirrel monkeyF12West
Toomisquirrel monkeyF8West
Ditasquirrel monkeyF5West
Sanchasquirrel monkeyF5West
Orlasquirrel monkeyF3West
Giselesquirrel monkeyF2–3West
Lokisquirrel monkeyF1West
PopeyecapuchinM13–14East
AnitacapuchinF17East
JunoncapuchinF14–15East
KatocapuchinM9–10East
ManuelcapuchinM11East
PenelopecapuchinF9East
CarloscapuchinM8–9East
ChicocapuchinM6East
RosacapuchinF5East
ReubencapuchinM4–5East
SolcapuchinF4–5East
FlojocapuchinM3–4East
LindocapuchinF3–4East
WillowcapuchinF2East
NenacapuchinF2East
GustavocapuchinM1East
AgnescapuchinF1East
Boasquirrel monkeyM9East
Tatusquirrel monkeyF14East
Rocasquirrel monkeyF12East
Mayasquirrel monkeyF11East
Eliesquirrel monkeyF9East
Calisquirrel monkeyF9East
Picasquirrel monkeyF7East
Yendisquirrel monkeyF6East
Florasquirrel monkeyF6East
Sipisquirrel monkeyF5East
Lexisquirrel monkeyF4East
Dorasquirrel monkeyF4East
Amarillasquirrel monkeyF3East
Pelusasquirrel monkeyF3East
Gabrielasquirrel monkeyF3East
Valenciasquirrel monkeyF3East
Ciarasquirrel monkeyF3East
Table 2. Definitions of network metrics used in the analysis (adapted from [14]).
Table 2. Definitions of network metrics used in the analysis (adapted from [14]).
Network TermDescription
NodeRepresents an individual within a network diagram (also referred to as vertex)
TieRelationship between two nodes in a network (also referred to as edge or arc)
Network densityThe proportion of possible connections in a network that are actually present, measuring how interconnected individuals are within the network. A higher density means that more individuals are connected to each other, while a lower density indicates fewer connections.
Association IndicesDescription
Degree CentralityMeasure based on the number of ties a node has. A higher degree of centrality means that the individual interacts frequently or maintains more consistent associations with other individuals in the network.
Eigenvector CentralityMeasure of the connectivity of an individual within its network according to the number and strength of connections and considering the centrality of the individuals it is connected to. Individuals with high eigenvector centrality are well connected to other individuals who are also well connected.
Betweenness CentralityMeasure of the number of shortest paths that pass through the considered individual (with the shortest path being the shortest distance, i.e., number of edges, between two nodes). These individuals serve as a bridge between others; therefore, an individual with high betweenness centrality connects different parts of the network, facilitating interactions across groups or clusters.
Clustering CoefficientMeasures the amount that a node tends to cluster with other nodes and captures the level of cohesion of the network. A high clustering coefficient indicates that an individual is embedded within a cohesive community, where their associates frequently interact or are associated with one another.
Average Path LengthThe average number of steps (or connections) required to travel between all pairs of nodes (individuals) within the network. Shorter average path lengths indicate a more efficient and tightly connected network, while longer path lengths suggest less efficient connectivity.
Table 3. Network metrics based on the original data for capuchins (West = 18; East = 17) and squirrel monkeys (West = 9; East = 17), mean (±SD).
Table 3. Network metrics based on the original data for capuchins (West = 18; East = 17) and squirrel monkeys (West = 9; East = 17), mean (±SD).
Group and SpeciesDegree CentralityEigenvector CentralityBetweenness CentralityClustering Coefficient
West capuchin 17.78 (1.31)0.49 (0.33)18.08 (1.31)0.88 (0.09)
West squirrel monkey10.44 (2.40)0.02 (0.007)29.44 (24.50)0.77 (0.19)
East capuchin 23.41 (3.36)0.48(0.31)17.79 (9.34)0.79 (0.03)
East squirrel monkey 24.94 (4.59)0.09 (0.04)12.93 (13.05)0.79 (0.06)
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Daoudi-Simison, S.; Lee, P.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M. Using Social Network Analysis to Assess ‘Groupness’ in a Mixed-Species Zoo Exhibit of Tufted Capuchins (Sapajus apella) and Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Animals 2024, 14, 3360. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14233360

AMA Style

Daoudi-Simison S, Lee P, Buchanan-Smith HM. Using Social Network Analysis to Assess ‘Groupness’ in a Mixed-Species Zoo Exhibit of Tufted Capuchins (Sapajus apella) and Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Animals. 2024; 14(23):3360. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14233360

Chicago/Turabian Style

Daoudi-Simison, Sophia, Phyllis Lee, and Hannah M. Buchanan-Smith. 2024. "Using Social Network Analysis to Assess ‘Groupness’ in a Mixed-Species Zoo Exhibit of Tufted Capuchins (Sapajus apella) and Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri sciureus)" Animals 14, no. 23: 3360. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14233360

APA Style

Daoudi-Simison, S., Lee, P., & Buchanan-Smith, H. M. (2024). Using Social Network Analysis to Assess ‘Groupness’ in a Mixed-Species Zoo Exhibit of Tufted Capuchins (Sapajus apella) and Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Animals, 14(23), 3360. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14233360

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Article metric data becomes available approximately 24 hours after publication online.
Back to TopTop