Can the Ability to Play Steady Beats Be Indicative of Cognitive Aging? Using a Beat Processing Device
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- Does the type of rhythm idioms significantly affect the reproduction accuracy of older adults?
- Are the participants’ age or current involvement in musical activities associated with their rhythm reproduction accuracy?
- Can rhythm performance accuracy, along with age, explain the general cognitive level as indicated by MMSE-K?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Rhythm Idioms for the Rhythm Reproduction Task
2.3. Measurement Tool: Beat Processing Device
2.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Rhythm Reproduction Accuracy Across Different Rhythm Idioms
3.2. Rhythm Reproduction Accuracy Depending on the Age Sub-Group and Current Engagement in Music
3.3. Rhythm Performance as a Predictor of Cognitive Ability
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Li, H.C.; Wang, H.H.; Chou, F.H.; Chen, K. The effect of music therapy on cognitive functioning among older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 2015, 16, 71–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maclean, L.M.; Brown, L.J.; Astell, A.J. The effect of rhythmic musical training on healthy older adults’ gait and cognitive function. Gerontologist 2014, 54, 624–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grahn, J.A.; Brett, M. Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007, 19, 893–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Iannarilli, F.; Vannozzi, G.; Iosa, M.; Pesce, C.; Capranica, L. Effects of task complexity on rhythmic reproduction performance in adults. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2013, 32, 203–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, S.J.; Yoo, G.E. Instrument playing as a cognitive intervention task for older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moussard, A.; Bermudez, P.; Alain, C.; Tays, W.; Morerno, S. Life-long music practice and executive control in older adults: An event-related potential stay. Brain Res. 2016, 1642, 146–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biasutti, M.; Mangiacotti, A. Assessing a cognitive music training for older participants: A randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2017, 33, 271–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turgeon, M.; Lustig, C.; Meck, W.H. Cognitive aging and time perception: Roles of Bayesian optimization and degeneracy. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016, 8, 102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chong, H.J.; Lee, E.J. Reproducing rhythmic idioms: A comparison between healthy older adults and older adults with mild cognitive impairment. J. Music Hum. Behav. 2019, 16, 73–88. [Google Scholar]
- Duerksen, G.; Chong, H.J. Preliminary study on developing protocol for Music therapy Assessment for Cognitive and Emotional-behavioral Domain using Rhythm (MACED-Rhythm). J. Music Hum. Behav. 2013, 10, 67–83. [Google Scholar]
- Baudouin, A.; Vanneste, S.; Isingrini, M. Age-related cognitive slowing: The role of spontaneous tempo and processing speed. Exp. Aging Res. 2004, 30, 225–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vanneste, V.; Pouthas, J.H.; Wearden, S. Temporal control of rhythmic performance: A comparison between young and old adults. Exp. Aging Res. 2001, 27, 83–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Turgeon, M.; Wing, A.M.; Taylor, L.W. Timing and aging: Slowing of fastest regular tapping rate with preserved timing error detection and correction. Psychol. Aging 2011, 26, 150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ragot, R.; Ferrandez, A.M.; Pouthas, V. Time, music, and aging. Psychomusicol. J. Res. Music Cogn. 2002, 18, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thaut, M.H.; McIntosh, G.C.; Hoemberg, V. Neurobiological foundations of neurologic music therapy: Rhythmic entrainment and the motor system. Front. Psychol. 2015, 5, 1185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trost, W.; Frühholz, S.; Schön, D.; Labbé, C.; Pichon, S.; Grandjean, D.; Vuilleumier, P. Getting the beat: Entrainment of brain activity by musical rhythm and pleasantness. NeuroImage 2014, 103, 55–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallego Hiroyasu, E.M.; Yotsumoto, Y. Disentangling the effects of modality, interval length and task difficulty on the accuracy and precision of older adults in a rhythmic reproduction task. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0248295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iannarilli, F.; Vannozzi, G.; Minganti, C.; Capranica, L.; Pesce, C. Rhythmic ability decline in aging individuals: The role of movement task complexity. Biomed. Hum. Kinet. 2022, 14, 41–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bangert, A.S.; Balota, D.A. Keep up the pace: Declines in simple repetitive timing differentiate healthy aging from the earliest stages of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2012, 18, 1052–1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turgeon, M.; Wing, A.M. Late onset of age-related difference in unpaced tapping with no age-related difference in phase-shift error detection and correction. Psychol. Aging 2012, 27, 1152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, J.H.; Kwon, Y.C. Standardization of Korean version of the Min-Mental State Examination (MMSE-K) for use in the elderly Part II: Diagnostic validity. J. Korean Med. Assoc. 1989, 28, 508–513. [Google Scholar]
- Persichini, C.; Capranica, L. Reproduction of rhythmic patterns in young individuals: Toward the construction of a computerized rhythmic test. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 2004, 35, 246–265. [Google Scholar]
- Capizzi, M.; Sanabria, D.; Correa, Á. Dissociating controlled from automatic processing in temporal preparation. Cognition. 2012, 123, 293–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piras, F.; Coull, J.T. Implicit, predictive timing draws upon the same scalar representation of time as explicit timing. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e18203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daikoku, T.; Takahashi, Y.; Tarumoto, N.; Yasuda, H. Motor reproduction of time interval depends on internal temporal cues in the brain: Sensorimotor imagery in rhythm. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Iannarilli, F.; Pesce, C.; Persichini, C.; Capranica, L. Age-related changes of rhythmic ability in musically trained and untrained individuals. Sport Sci. Health 2013, 9, 43–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Che, Y.; Jicol, C.; Ashwin, C.; Petrini, K. An RCT study showing few weeks of music lessons enhance audio-visual temporal processing. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 20087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitch, W.T.; Rosenfeld, A.J. Perception and production of syncopated rhythms. Music Percept. 2007, 25, 43–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Helmbold, N.; Troche, S.; Rammsayer, T. Processing of temporal and nontemporal information as predictors of psychometric intelligence: A structural-equation modeling approach. J. Pers. 2007, 75, 985–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variable | Total Group (N = 79) | Age Sub-Group | Current Music Engagement | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ages 60 to 74 (n = 44) | Ages 75 to 90 (n = 35) | Yes (n = 29) | No (n = 50) | ||
Gender, Female, n (%) | 59 (74.7%) | 36 (81.8%) | 23 (63.9%) | 25 (86.2%) | 34 (68.0%) |
Age in years, M (SD) | 73.8 (6.4) | 69.1 (3.9) | 79.8 (3.0) | 70.8 (6.9) | 75.6 (5.4) |
MMSE-K, M (SD) | 27.2 (1.8) | 27.7 (1.7) | 26.7 (1.7) | 27.6 (1.8) | 27.0 (1.7) |
Rhythm Idiom | Inter-Onset-Intervals (ms) | Ratio of Each Beat Interval | Components of Musical Notation | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Rhy1 | 1000, 1000 | 1:1 | Basic steady notes (pulsating beats) | |
Rhy2 | 500, 500, 500, 500 | 1:1:1:1 | Subdivided notes (eighth notes) | |
Rhy3 | 500, 500, 1000 | 1:1:2 | Subdivided notes followed by longer note (basic interval) | |
Rhy4 | 1000, 500, 500 | 2:1:1 | Basic note followed by subdivided notes | |
Rhy5 | 500, 1500 | 1:3 | Subdivided shorter note (eighth note) followed by longer duration (extended beat) | |
Rhy6 | 1500, 500 | 3:1 | Dotted quarter note (extended beat) followed by a single short note (eighth note) |
Rhythm Idiom | Total Group (N = 79) | Age Sub-Group | Current Engagement in Music | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Young-Old (Ages 60–74; n = 44) | Older-Old (Ages 75–90; n = 35) | Yes (n = 29) | No (n = 50) | |||
M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | ||
Rhy1 | 0.11 (0.17) | 0.08 (0.10) | 0.15 (0.23) | 0.06 (0.09) | 0.14 (0.20) | |
Rhy3 | 0.17 (0.20) | 0.16 (0.18) | 0.19 (0.22) | 0.13 (0.12) | 0.20 (0.23) | |
Rhy2 | 0.25 (0.26) | 0.23 (0.26) | 0.28 (0.27) | 0.17 (0.19) | 0.30 (0.29) | |
Rhy5 | 0.28 (0.23) | 0.26 (0.19) | 0.32 (0.26) | 0.24 (0.22) | 0.31 (0.23) | |
Rhy4 | 0.34 (0.24) | 0.34 (0.21) | 0.33 (0.26) | 0.31 (0.18) | 0.35 (0.26) | |
Rhy6 | 0.47 (0.29) | 0.49 (0.28) | 0.45 (0.30) | 0.52 (0.32) | 0.44 (0.28) |
Group 1 | Group 2 | Total Group | Age Sub-Group Analysis | Current Music Engagement Subgroup Analysis | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MD | p | MD | p | MD | P | ||
Rhy1 | Rhy2 | −0.142 | 0.001 ** | –0.140 | 0.002 ** | –0.136 | 0.004 ** |
Rhy3 | –0.063 | 0.371 | –0.060 | 0.489 | –0.064 | 0.422 | |
Rhy4 | –0.225 | <0.001 *** | –0.220 | <0.001 *** | –0.230 | <0.001 *** | |
Rhy5 | –0.173 | <0.001 *** | –0.172 | <0.001 *** | –0.174 | <0.001 *** | |
Rhy6 | –0.361 | <0.001 *** | –0.355 | <0.001 *** | –0.383 | <0.001 *** | |
Rhy2 | Rhy3 | 0.079 | 0.452 | 0.080 | 0.448 | 0.071 | 0.860 |
Rhy4 | –0.083 | 0.585 | –0.080 | 0.727 | –0.095 | 0.362 | |
Rhy5 | –0.031 | 1.000 | –0.032 | 1.000 | –0.038 | 1.000 | |
Rhy6 | –0.220 | <0.001 *** | –0.215 | <0.001 *** | –0.247 | <0.001 *** | |
Rhy3 | Rhy4 | –0.162 | <0.001 *** | –0.160 | <0.001 *** | –0.166 | <0.001 *** |
Rhy5 | –0.110 | <0.001 *** | –0.112 | <0.001 *** | –0.109 | 0.001 ** | |
Rhy6 | –0.298 | <0.001 *** | –0.294 | <0.001 *** | –0.318 | <0.001 *** | |
Rhy4 | Rhy5 | 0.052 | 1.000 | 0.048 | 1.000 | 0.057 | 1.000 |
Rhy6 | –0.137 | 0.007 ** | –0.134 | 0.010 * | –0.152 | 0.003 ** | |
Rhy5 | Rhy6 | –0.189 | <0.001 *** | –0.183 | 0.001 ** | –0.209 | <0.001 *** |
R Index, r (p) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Rhy1 | Rhy2 | Rhy3 | Rhy4 | Rhy5 | Rhy6 | |
MMSE-K score | −0.258 | −0.017 | −0.011 | 0.013 | 0.000 | −0.162 |
(0.022 *) | (0.879) | (0.920) | (0.907) | (0.999) | (0.154) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Chong, H.J.; Choi, J.H.; Yoo, G.E. Can the Ability to Play Steady Beats Be Indicative of Cognitive Aging? Using a Beat Processing Device. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111113
Chong HJ, Choi JH, Yoo GE. Can the Ability to Play Steady Beats Be Indicative of Cognitive Aging? Using a Beat Processing Device. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(11):1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111113
Chicago/Turabian StyleChong, Hyun Ju, Jin Hee Choi, and Ga Eul Yoo. 2024. "Can the Ability to Play Steady Beats Be Indicative of Cognitive Aging? Using a Beat Processing Device" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 11: 1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111113
APA StyleChong, H. J., Choi, J. H., & Yoo, G. E. (2024). Can the Ability to Play Steady Beats Be Indicative of Cognitive Aging? Using a Beat Processing Device. Behavioral Sciences, 14(11), 1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14111113