Next Article in Journal
Perfective -le Use and Consciousness-Raising among Beginner-Level Chinese Learners
Previous Article in Journal
Information Structure and Word Order Preference in Child and Adult Speech of Mandarin Chinese
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Towards Modeling Second Dialect Speech Learning: The Production of Bogota [s] in Ciudad Bolivar by Speakers of Three Different Varieties of Colombian Spanish

1
Department of Languages & Cultures, Faculty of Arts & Humanities, Western University, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada
2
Department of French Studies, Faculty of Arts & Humanities, Western University, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Languages 2020, 5(2), 15; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5020015
Submission received: 14 February 2020 / Revised: 5 April 2020 / Accepted: 8 April 2020 / Published: 16 April 2020

Abstract

:
This study investigates the second dialect production of Bogota Spanish /s/ in coda position by speakers of three different varieties of Colombian Spanish, who have been in contact in Ciudad Bolivar, a community located in Bogota, Colombia. The study has three aims. First, it will examine the role of phonetic distance in the acquisition of /s/ production. Second, it will determine the linguistic factors that constrain the realization of /s/ sound by the speakers of the three varieties studied. Third, it will look into the role of extralinguistic factors in the production of /s/. A total of 2322 tokens extracted from sociolinguistic interviews with 50 participants were acoustically analyzed in PRAAT. Statistical analyses were conducted using GoldVarb. The results showed the highest rate of [s] was produced by the speakers of the Eastern Andean variety, followed by the Western Andean, and then by the Coastal variety, suggesting that first dialect phonological processes may affect the acquisition of second dialect sounds. Consistent with previous studies that have examined /s/ variation and change, the linguistic factors position in the word, following segment, and syllable stress were also predictors of /s/ in second dialect production. The extralinguistic factors of age of arrival, age, and gender also had a significant effect on /s/ production in this study. Implications are discussed for models of second dialect speech learning.

1. Introduction

Second Dialect (D2) speech increasingly arouses interest due to the mechanisms that are involved in this process (e.g., Siegel 2010; Babel 2011; Nielsen 2011). One of the most frequent questions asked is whether acquiring a D2 variety is equivalent to the process of acquiring second language speech (L2). Moreover, researchers are interested in whether the linguistic and/or extralinguistic factors that constrain language variation and change also affect D2 acquisition. Although a number of theoretical models of L2 acquisition have been developed for L2 speech learning (e.g., Brown 1998; Flege 1995; Best and Tyler 2007; Colantoni and Steele 2008), we currently do not have a model of D2 speech learning.
The first objective of this study is to investigate whether proximity/similarity between a first dialect (D1) sound and a D2 sound would make learning a D2 sound easier or more difficult. There is currently no consensus on this in the literature (e.g., Siegel 2010; Escure 1997). In particular, we will be investigating whether the Speech Learning Model (SLM) Flege (1995) applies to the acquisition of a phonological variant of a second dialect (D2) in the same way that it applies to the acquisition of L2 sounds. The SLM puts forth that the degree of difficulty of acquisition of an L2 segment depends on the degree of acoustic distance between the first language (L1) sounds and L2 sounds. That is, the larger the acoustic distance between the L1 and L2 sounds, the lower the chances of ‘equivalence classification’ and the easier it would be to acquire the L2 sound. In other words, this model predicts that ‘new’ sounds can be acquired and ‘old’ sounds are not problematic but that ‘similar’ sounds are difficult. We will examine the production of the voiceless predorso-alveolar sibilant [s] that is typical of Bogota speech in Colombia by speakers of three D1 varieties of Colombian Spanish (Eastern Andean, Coastal, and Western Andean) who had migrated to, and then lived in, Ciudad Bolivar for five years. Ciudad Bolivar is a neighbourhood where migrants from various parts of Colombia live together, and it is located in South West Bogota. Whereas /s/ production in coda position is the same in both the Eastern Andean variety of Colombian Spanish and Bogota Spanish, it is different in the Costal and Western Varieties. In the Coastal variety, /s/ tends to be aspirated (e.g., <buenos> [‘bwe.noh] ‘good’) or deleted (e.g., <todos> [‘to.ðo] ‘all’), and in the Western varieties it is typically produced as a voiceless apical sibilant [ s ] (e.g., <dos > [do s ] ‘two’). Therefore [s] would be an ‘old’ sound for the speakers of the Eastern Andean variety, a ‘new’ sound for the speakers of the Coastal variety, and a ‘similar’ sound for the speakers of the Western Andean variety. These differences allow us to test the role of acoustic distance between the D1 and D2 sounds in a dialect contact situation. Because the SLM does not outline how acoustic distance is measured, in this paper, we have relied on our intuition to predict which sounds would be similar.
The second objective of this study is to investigate whether linguistic and extralinguistic factors condition the acquisition of [s] in Ciudad Bolivar. It is well-known that both linguistic and extralinguistic factors constrain language variation (Labov 1972; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; De Weinberg 1973; Cedergren 1978; Poplack 1980; Mazzaro 2003, 2011; Tararova 2012). A body of literature on D2 acquisition has also provided evidence for the role of both linguistic and extralinguistic factors in D2 speech learning. Some of these factors include age of arrival (e.g., Kerswill and Williams 1994; Rys 2007; Siegel 2010; Tagliamonte and Molfenter 2007; Trudgill 1986; Bortoni-Ricardo 1985; Chambers 1992; Munro et al. 1999; Nycz 2015), age (e.g., Munro et al. 1999; Nycz 2015), gender (e.g., Holmquist 2011; Foreman 2003; Rys 2007; Ruiz-Peña et al. 2015), and education (e.g., Tagliamonte and Molfenter 2007; Chambers 1992).
A number of studies, such as Harris (1983), Penny (2000), and Lipski (1999), among others, have also provided evidence for the role of linguistic factors in the phonological process of aspiration and deletion of /s/ in Spanish. Some of these factors include position in the word, previous and following segment, and stress (see the diachronic weakening pathway for /s/ proposed by Lipski (1999) in File-Muriel and Orozco (2012, p. 92). That extralinguistic factors would also constrain variation in Colombian Spanish has been proposed by File-Muriel and Orozco (2012). However, we are not aware of any previous studies that have examined the role of linguistic and extralinguistic factors on the D2 speech learning of /s/. Therefore, the study of these factors merits investigation.

1.1. Literature Review

1.1.1. /s/ Variation in Spanish

Several authors consider /s/ to be the sound that determines dialect division in Spanish (e.g., Zamora and Guitart 1982; Lipski 1996; Montes 2000). Others have also used /s/ as a criterion in the identification of varieties to identify dialect divisions between the main standard varieties of Spanish (peninsular and American variety), or to identify dialect divisions between countries (e.g., Ureña 1921; Honsa 1975; Flórez 1963, cited by Lipski 1996, p. 21). Crucially, different varieties of Spanish exhibit a different degree of /s/ weakening. /s/ weakening refers to a phonological process, where /s/ is aspirated and produced as [h] (e.g., <costa> [‘koh.ta] ‘coastal’), or deleted (e.g., <niños> [‘ni.ɲo] ‘children’) (e.g., Tejada Giráldez 2012; Alba 1990; Samper Padilla and Hernández Cabrera 1995; Brown 2012; Brown and Torres Cacoullos 2002; Terrell 1986; File-Muriel 2010; Poplack 1980; Brown 2009a, 2009b; Orozco 2018; Becerra 1985; Lafford 1986; Rodríguez Cadena 2004).

1.1.2. /s/ Variation in Colombian Spanish

Colombian Spanish is made up of a set of regional varieties that are different in terms of pronunciation, intonation, and vocabulary. Officially, Colombia has two macro-dialect regions, the Coastal and the Andean varieties. Each one has separate sub-dialects. The coastal variety is divided into the Pacific and Atlantic varieties and the Andean variety is divided into the Eastern and the Western Andean varieties. The Bogota variety belongs to the Eastern Andean region but is considered the standard Colombian variety (Montes 1984). One of the most notable differences between the regions is /s/ realization in coda position.
In Colombian Spanish, like the other varieties of Latin American Spanish, the distinction between alveolar /s/ and the interdental /θ/ is neutralized to [s]. In most of the Colombian territory, /s/ in coda position is produced as a voiceless predorso-alveolar sibilant [s]. However, in the Western varieties, especially in Antioquia region, the /s/ sound is produced as a voiceless apical sibilant [ s ] (e.g., <dos> [do s ] ‘two’) and in the Coastal variety, it is aspirated (e.g., <costa> [‘koh.ta] ‘coastal’) or deleted (e.g., <niños> [‘ni.ɲo] ‘children’ (Izquierdo and Utrilla 2002, p. 68; NGLEFF 2011, p. 191). Montes (2000), proposed that the /s/ sound in Colombia constitutes a binary variation where /s/ is retained in the Eastern and Western Andean regions, but aspirated and deleted in the Coastal regions. According to Orozco (2018, p. 6), while in the Eastern and Western Andean varieties the consistent articulation of /s/ as a sibilant constitutes a prestige marker, /s/ deletion in the Coastal region enjoys covert prestige. Moreover, it is essential to mention that in Colombia, there are marked attitudes towards the different varieties spoken in the country. Duarte (2005), in his study on the attitudes of Bogota residents towards the dialects of Colombian Spanish, examined the attitudes of 52 Bogota speakers—men, and women between 38 and 60 years old, of low and high socioeconomic status—had towards their own variety and the other varieties of Colombian Spanish. His results showed that the participants attributed qualifiers to the different varieties. For example, the Bogota variety received qualifications such as “neutral,” “suitable for diplomatic and international relations,” “easy to understand,” and “cultured.” On the other hand, the coastal variety received qualifications such as “relaxed,” “frank,” “uneducated,” and “difficult to understand.” The Paisa variety was considered “very pleasant,” and “synonymous with vigor.” Similarly, other studies (e.g., Caicedo 1992; Chamorro and López 2020) have reported results with the same trends regarding attitudes to the different Colombian dialectal varieties. In Colombia, the way of speaking constitutes an accentuated mark of cultural identity.
The following sections will provide an overview of the classification of Colombian dialect regions regarding /s/ realization.

Bogota Variety

Bogota Spanish is spoken in the capital district and is considered the standard variety of Colombia (Flórez 1973; Montes 1984; NGLEFF 2011). It is characterized by the fact that its speakers maintain the /s/ sound and articulate it as a voiceless predorso-alveolar sibilant [s], in word-medial and word-final positions. Such [s] realization in these phonetic contexts also constitutes a prestige marker (Flórez 1963; Lipski 1996; Montes 1984; Quesada Pacheco 2011). However, in a syllable onset at the beginning of the word (e.g., <señora> [se.’ɲo.ɾa] ‘lady’) and in intervocalic position (e.g., <nosotros> [no.’so.tros] ‘us’) /s/ is aspirated and sometimes deleted (e.g., <señora> [he.’ɲo.ɾa] ‘lady’ <nosotros> [no.’o.tɾos] ‘us’). Aspiration and deletion occur more frequently in spontaneous speech (Brown and Brown 2012; Flórez 1973; Montes 1984).

Coastal Variety

The coastal variety refers to the variety that is spoken in the Caribbean and Pacific area of Colombia. The coastal variety is characterized by the fact that aspiration and deletion tend to occur in a post-vocalic coda position (e.g., <las casas> [lah ‘ka. sah] ‘the houses’ or <las casas> [la ‘ka. sa] ‘the houses’) (Montes 1984; Lafford 1986; Canfield 1988).

Eastern Andean Variety

The Eastern Andean variety is spoken in the provinces of the central area of the country, specifically in the provinces of Cundinamarca, Boyaca, Santander, Tolima, Huila. Normally, /s/ is maintained in coda position (e.g., <bosque> [‘bos.ke] ‘forest’ and <manos> [ma.nos] ‘hands’) but it is aspirated (e.g., <nosotros> [no.’ho.tros] ‘us’) or deleted (e.g., <señora> [e.’ɲo.ɾa] ‘lady’) in onset position (Brown and Brown 2012; Flórez 1973; Montes 1984), a pronunciation which is “unique in the Spanish-speaking world” according to Lipski (1984, p. 209).

Western Andean Variety

The Western Andean variety, also known as the “paisa” variety, is spoken in the Central Western area of Colombia in the provinces of Antioquia, Caldas, and Cauca. /s/ is realized as a voiceless apical alveolar sibilant [ s ] in coda position, e.g., <bosque> [‘bo s .ke] ‘forest’, <manos> [ma.no s ] ‘hands’. (Montes 1984; Canfield 1988). The Western Andean variety is recognized as the second most prestigious variety in Colombia after the Bogota variety, according to Londoño, Estupiñán, and Idárraga (Londoño et al. 2012) and Bernal, Munévar, and Barajas (Bernal et al. 2014).

1.1.3. The Effect of Linguistic and Extra-Linguistic Factors on /s/ Variation and D2 Acquisition

The effects of several linguistic factors have been studied with respect to /s/ variation such as position in the word (e.g., Becerra 1985; Lafford 1986; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; Terrell 1986; Poplack 1980; Tejada Giráldez 2012; Alba 1990; Samper Padilla and Hernández Cabrera 1995; Brown 2009a, 2009b), following segment (e.g., Becerra 1985; Lafford 1986; Rodríguez Cadena 2004), number of syllables (Tejada Giráldez 2012; Alba 1990; Samper Padilla and Hernández Cabrera 1995), and stress (Becerra 1985; Lafford 1986; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; Orozco 2018). Although most studies have shown that these factors constrain /s/ variation, there has not always been consensus. For example, studies on the realization of /s/ in coda position in Cartagena Spanish (e.g., Becerra 1985; Lafford 1986), Barranquilla Spanish (e.g., Rodríguez Cadena 2004), and Colombian Caribbean Spanish (e.g., Orozco 2018) have reported a higher rate of /s/ aspiration in word-medial position than in word-final position, and a higher rate of /s/ deletion in word-final position than in word-medial position. However, Samper Padilla and Hernández Cabrera (1995) and Terrell (1981) have found a higher rate of /s/ deletion in word-medial coda position than in word-final position in the Spanish of las Palmas de Gran Canaria, Puerto Rico, and the Dominican Republic.
With respect to the preceding and following segment for /s/ realization, Terrell (1981) found that Dominican Republic Spanish speakers reduce /s/ when it is preceded by a voiced consonant. Likewise, Brown and Torres Cacoullos (2002), and Rodríguez Cadena (2004), in their study of Cali and Barranquilla Spanish, found that /s/ is retained when it is preceded by a vowel and aspirated when preceded by consonant. Regarding the effect of stress, Alba (1982), Poplack (1980) and Rodríguez Cadena (2004) found that stressed syllables favor /s/ retention, while unstressed ones favor weakening in Dominican Republic and Puerto Rican varieties. Regarding the effect of the number of syllables, Alba (1990) concluded that in Dominican Santiago Spanish, /s/ is deleted in long words more than in short ones. Samper Padilla and Hernández Cabrera (1995) in their study of Las Palmas de Gran Canaria formal Spanish, concluded that /s/ is retained as a sibilant or aspirated variant in monosyllabic words, while it is deleted in those with more than two syllables. Likewise, Tejada Giráldez (2012), in her study of Granada Spanish, found that in word-final position, /s/ is maintained in monosyllabic words, and that there was a high tendency for deletion in polysyllabic ones.
Regarding extralinguistic factors affecting /s/ variation, previous studies have reported that speakers with a working-class background favor /s/ weakening, while those from middle-class background favor its retention (e.g., Diaz-Campos 2014; López Morales 1989; Becerra 1985; Lafford 1986). Another factor that plays a role is gender; women have been reported to have a lower rate of weakening of /s/ and to favor the retention of this sound when the weakening is stigmatized (e.g., Rodríguez Cadena 2004; De Weinberg 1973). However, men favor /s/ weakening. Age has also been shown to be a factor that conditions /s/ variation. Cedergren (1978), Poplack (1980), and Terrell (1981) concluded that older participants exhibit a lower rate of /s/ weakening than younger participants. Level of education has also been shown to be a significant factor; Rodríguez Cadena (2004) and Terrell (1981) found that in Cartagena and Santo Domingo Spanish, level of education significantly affected /s/ variation, as a higher of level of education favored /s/ retention.
Likewise, Mazzaro (2003) reported in his study on /s/ deletion in Corrientes Argentinian Spanish that speakers of lower socioeconomic classes favored the use of the stigmatized variant (deletion) while those of higher classes favored the more prestigious variant, [s], in the Spanish dialect of Corrientes Argentina. Moreover, Alba (1982) found that the most relevant factor in /s/ variation in Dominican Spanish was level of education. In effect, the rate of /s/ deletion rises significantly as the level of schooling decreases. Alba (1982) also found that women tended to favor more /s/ realization, while men tended to favor /s/ deletion.
Extralinguistic factors have also been analyzed in variationist and D2 studies. Esparza and Mazzaro (2018) in their study of /s/ variation and speech accommodation of Veracruzanos newcomers in Ciudad Juárez, found that the linguistic factor position in the syllable and extralinguistic factors such as gender and education affected the speech of Veracruzanos in their D2, while the length of residence was not significant.
Speakers with higher levels of education tend to maintain the standard variants of their language (e.g., Labov 1972; Tagliamonte and Molfenter 2007; Chambers 1992). As for the effect of socio-economic background, speakers with higher socioeconomic status favor standard variants, while speakers with lower status favor non-standard ones (e.g., Labov 1972, 2000). With respect to the effect of gender, women tend to adhere to the standard and more prestigious variants of their language (e.g., Labov 1972). In terms of acquisition of D2, women have also favored the acquisition of standard variants of their D2 (e.g., Holmquist 2011; Foreman 2003; Rys 2007; Ruiz-Peña et al. 2015).
Age of acquisition (AoA) has received a good deal of attention, especially in D2 acquisition studies (e.g., Bortoni-Ricardo 1985; Tagliamonte and Molfenter 2007; Trudgill 1981) and L2 studies (e.g., Berthele 2002; Bortoni-Ricardo 1985; Chambers 1992; Foreman 2003; Ivars 1994; Kerswill and Williams 1994; Payne 1976; Rys 2007). It has been shown that younger age is linked to greater acquisition of D2 and L2 features, but several ages have been proposed as the cut-off point for attaining native-like patterns in D2: 5 years (Tagliamonte and Molfenter 2007), 8 years (Payne 1976) and 13 years (Chambers 1992). In their study on D1 Canadian English-speakers acquiring D2 British English, Tagliamonte and Molfenter (2007) found that three children with an AoA of 2 to 4 achieved a D2 native-like level of proficiency and that two of them reached a 95% rate of use of the D2 phonetic variants. Likewise, Trudgill (1981), in his study of two British English D1 children learning the Australian D2 variety found that the participants (a boy and a girl) who had moved from southern England to Australia at the age of seven were able to produce the majority of the 15 phonetic and phonological variables analyzed, and both were perceived as native Australian speakers. Bortoni-Ricardo (1985), in her study of D1 rural Caipira dialect speakers of Brazilian Portuguese acquiring the D2 standard urban dialect, found that participants who had migrated from a rural area (D1) to and urban one (D2) under the age of thirteen made use of the D2 variants in 79% of occurrences, while those over 18 years only made 44% of usage of D2 variants. Chambers (1992) refers to a pioneering study on the acquisition of D2 conducted by Sibata (1958) in which about 500 interviews conducted in 1949 are analyzed for children sent from Tokyo and Yokohama to the Shirakawa region, to escape the bombings during the Second World War. Children who arrived before age 7 appear to have acquired the Shirakawa dialect, while those who arrived at age 14 or older kept their D1 dialect completely. However, this does not mean that adult participants cannot achieve a successful acquisition of an D2. Munro, Derwing and Flege (Munro et al. 1999) studied the perception of the difference in pronunciation of the diphthong /aj/ in adults with D1 Canadian English living in Alabama, where a monophthongal pronunciation of this vowel is common. The authors concluded that Canadian adults in a large percentage acquired this variant of the Alabama variety.
Considering the findings summarized above, we chose age 7 as the cut-off age for AoA in this study, grouping speakers into two categories: those who came to Ciudad Bolivar before the age of 7, and those who came after. According to Chambers (1992, p. 689), “a person 7 or under will acquire native-like usage of D2, a person over 14 almost will not make it and between those ages people will vary”. In addition, we made this distinction because in Colombia, although children attend pre-school education, they are formally exposed to the standard dialect in the school setting at 7 years.
Tagliamonte and Molfenter (2007) conducted a six-year longitudinal study on t-voicing in Canadian English D1 children living in the D2 area of York, England. The results showed variability in the degree of acquisition of the D2 variants, but the authors emphasized the difficulty of discerning whether the variability in degree of acquisition was due to social causes (interaction with the D2 community) or factors related to the child’s development (age and language faculties).
In the current study, we examine the effect of linguistic and extralinguistic factors that have been studied in relation to /s/ variation and in relation to D2 acquisition. It is plausible that the factors previously studied in variationist and D2 studies would also play a role in the D2 production of /s/ in Ciudad Bolivar in Bogota. Whereas the description of linguistic factors (e.g., position in the word, the following segment, number of syllables, and stress) is straightforward, this is not the case for all of the extralinguistic factors. For example, the socio-economic status levels used in the current study correspond to those of the classification that the city of Bogota has used in all 1922 neighborhoods. In this city, six levels of socio-economic stratification are identified (1 very low, 2 low, 3 medium-low, 4 medium, 5 medium-high, and 6 high), based on the type of housing in which the citizens reside. Residents classified as levels 1, 2, and 3 are those with the lowest income, and they benefit from subsidies and pay less for public services. Residents in level 4 are those with a modest income and do not benefit from subsidies but are not required to pay a premium for public services. Residents in levels 5 and 6 are those with higher incomes, and do not benefit from subsidies and must pay a premium for public services (DANE 2018). The participants in this study self-identified as belonging to levels 1 and 2, which correspond to very low and low socio-economic backgrounds.
The other extralinguistic factors that we will study here are gender, level of education, and age. While in each society the effects of these factors exhibit in a particular way, it is beyond the scope of this paper to comment in detail on the particularities of these social groups within Colombia compare them to other societies.

1.2. Research Questions

The research questions in this study are as follows:
  • Does the degree of similarity between a D2 sound and its nearest D1 sound determine the rate of production of the D2 sound (i.e., [s]) by the speakers of the three different D1 varieties of Colombia (Eastern Andean, Coastal, and Western Andean varieties), who have moved to Ciudad Bolivar and lived in contact with each other and with speakers of Bogota Spanish in Ciudad Bolivar?
  • Is there an effect of linguistic factors?
  • Is there an effect of extralinguistic factors?

1.3. Hypotheses

It is well known in the L2 literature that equivalence classification is an important mechanism that can explain L2 speech learning. However, we do not know whether this mechanism also plays a role in D2 speech learning. Therefore, in the current study, we will test both the predictions of the SLM and the effect of linguistic and extralinguistic factors that are typically studied in a variationist study. The hypotheses in this study are as follows:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Since phonetic/acoustic distance modulates the degree of equivalence classification between an L1 sound and an L2 sound in L2 speech learning (Flege 1995), we predict that the degree of acoustic distance between the D2 sound and its nearest D1 sound will also play a role and determine whether equivalence classification will take place or not. In particular, we predict that (i) The voiceless predorso-alveolar sibilant [s] in coda position will be an ‘old’ sound for Eastern Andean region speakers because they previously produced it in their D1 variety and therefore it will not be problematic for this group of speakers; (ii) The voiceless predorso-alveolar sibilant [s] will be a ‘new’ or ‘old’ sound for Coastal speakers because they tend to aspirate or delete it in their D1, so it will not be problematic for this group of speakers either; (iii) The voiceless predorso-alveolar [s] will be a ‘similar’ sound for the Western Andean region speakers because they realize it as a voiceless apical alveolar sibilant [ s ]. Therefore, [s] will be a difficult sound for the latter group of speakers. In all, the voiceless predorso-alveolar sibilant [s] will be produced at a higher rate by speakers of the Eastern Andean and Coastal varieties, followed by speakers of the Western Andean region variety).
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
[s] production in coda position by the speakers from the Eastern Andean, Coastal, and Western Andean zones will follow the same patterns observed in previous studies on /s/ in Spanish. In particular, the following is predicted:
(i) 
There will be an effect of word position. Specifically, /s/ will be produced at a higher rate in the word-medial than in the word-final coda position (Becerra 1985; Lafford 1986; Rodríguez Cadena 2004);
(ii) 
There will be an effect of the following segment. In particular, /s/ will be produced at a higher rate in word-final coda position before a vowel than before a consonant (Cedergren 1978; Lipski 1984; File-Muriel 2007; Terrell 1981; Brown and Torres Cacoullos 2002; Rodríguez Cadena 2004);
(iii) 
There will be an effect of number of syllables. Namely, /s/ will be produced at a higher rate in monosyllabic words than in disyllabic, trisyllabic, and polysyllabic words (Tejada Giráldez 2012; Alba 1990; Samper Padilla and Hernández Cabrera 1995);
(iv) 
There will be an effect of stress. That is, /s/ will be produced at a higher rate in stressed syllables than in unstressed syllables (Brown 2004; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; File-Muriel 2007; Brown and Torres Cacoullos 2002; Orozco 2018).
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
Extralinguistic factors will constrain [s] production. Specifically, the following results are predicted:
(i) 
There will be an effect of age of arrival. Namely, participants who moved from the Eastern Andean, Coastal, and Western Andean zones to Ciudad Bolivar at an earlier age will favor [s] production (Kerswill and Williams 1994; Rys 2007; Siegel 2010; Tagliamonte and Molfenter 2007; Trudgill 1981; Bortoni-Ricardo 1985; Chambers 1992; Munro et al. 1999; Nycz 2015);
(ii) 
There will be an effect of education. Specifically, all participants who have moved from the Eastern Andean, Coastal, and Western Andean zones to Ciudad Bolivar and have a higher level of education will favor [s] production. (Labov 1972; Terrell 1981; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; Tagliamonte and Molfenter 2007; Chambers 1992);
(iii) 
There will be an effect of socioeconomic background. In particular, participants who moved from the Eastern Andean, Coastal, and Western Andean zones to Ciudad Bolivar and have the lowest socioeconomic status will exhibit a lower rate of [s] production (Labov 1972, 2000; Becerra 1985; Lafford 1986);
(iv) 
There will be an effect of age. Specifically, participants who moved from the Eastern Andean, Coastal, and Western Andean zones to Ciudad Bolivar and are younger will favor [s] production. (Lenneberg 1967; López Morales 1983; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; Terrell 1981; Ramírez and Almira 2011; Cedergren 1978);
(v) 
There will be an effect of gender. Namely, female participants who moved from the Eastern Andean, Coastal, and Western Andean zones to Ciudad Bolivar will favor [s] production (Labov 1972; Holmquist 2011; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; De Weinberg 1973; Cedergren 1978; Poplack 1980; Foreman 2003; Rys 2007; Ruiz-Peña et al. 2015).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants consisted of 17 native speakers of the Eastern Andean variety of Colombian Spanish, 20 native speakers of the Coastal varieties of Colombian Spanish, and 13 native speakers of Western Andean Colombian Spanish who had lived in Ciudad Bolivar in Bogota Colombia for more than five years. Altogether, there were 31 females and 19 males aged between 15 and 68 years (mean age 36 years). Their level of education ranged from primary school to college. They had self-identified as members of the lower middle class (socioeconomic status 1 and 2).

2.2. Data

The corpus analyzed in this study was taken from the Atlas Sociolingüístico y Etnográfico de Colombia en Espacios de restablecimiento Poblacional (ASLEC-EURP) project, owned by the Caro and Cuervo Institute of Bogota Colombia. This corpus was gathered according to the technical and ethical protocols required in the collection of data for sociolinguistic studies and was carried out by a team of professional researchers with experience in studies on language and linguistics. All participants gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Grupo de Investigación Lingüística del Instituto Caro y Cuervo code COL0093818.
The ASLEC EURP corpus contains approximately 30 hours of recordings of speech of men and women of different ages, belonging to the Eastern Andean (Cundiboyacense), Atlantic and Pacific (Costeño), Western Andean (Antioqueño) and Llanero dialect varieties. This database also provides socio-demographic data such as age, gender, region of origin, length of residence in Ciudad Bolivar, occupation, address, socioeconomic status, and marital status.

2.3. Data Analysis

Three minutes of recording were selected from participants’ conversations, starting ten minutes into the interview conducted by the Caro y Cuervo Institute researchers. After the speech sample was selected, the orthographic transcripts were extracted to indicate each /s/ realization produced by the speakers. Between 80 to 130 tokens were produced per individual. A total of 2322 tokens with /s/ in coda position were analyzed both auditorily and acoustically in Praat (Boersma and Weenink 2016) by one of the authors, who is a female native speaker of Colombian Spanish and has training in acoustic analysis. The analysis resulted in 1548 (66.7%) voiceless predorso-alveolar sibilants [s], 426 (18.3%) aspirations [h], 276 (11.9%) deletions [∅], 27 (1.2%) voiceless apico-alveolar sibilants [ s ] and 45 (1.9%) voiced predorso-alveolar sibilants [z].
In order to analyze the linguistic factors that conditioned the realization of /s/ in the speech of this group of participants, the linguistic variables corresponding to each of the occurrences of /s/ were identified. First, position in the word was looked at to see if [s] occurred in (a) word-medial coda position (e.g., <fresco> [‘fɾɛs.ko] ‘fresh’) or (b) word-final coda position (e.g., <lejos> [‘le.xos] ‘far’).
Similarly, the following segments were classified into the following categories: (a) voiced consonant (e.g., <nos vinimos> [‘noz.βi.’ni.mos] ‘we came’), (b) voiceless consonant (e.g., <constructor> [kons.tɾuk.’toɾ] ‘builder’), (c) high vowel (e.g., <mis hijos> [mis ‘i.xos] ‘my children’), (d) low vowel (e.g., <mis amigos> [mis a.’mi.ɣos] ‘my friends”), and (e) pause, (e.g., entonces (pause) mis abuelos vivían en Troncoso [en.ton.ses’ mis.a.βwe.’li.toz β.i’βi.an en troŋ.’ko.so] ‘then ... (pause) my grandparents lived there in Troncoso’).
As for the number of syllables in the word, this was categorized into the following sub-categories: (a) monosyllabic (e.g., <los> [‘los] ‘the’), (b) disyllabic (e.g., <manos> [‘ma.nos] ‘hands’), (c) trisyllabic (e.g., <misterio> [mis.‘te. rjo] ‘mystery’), and (d) polysyllabic (e.g., <descansando> [dɛs.kan.’san.do] ‘taking a break’).
Finally, stress was divided into (a) stressed (e.g., <papás> [pa.’pas] ‘dads’), (b) unstressed (e.g., <niños> [‘ni.ños] ‘children’). This information, in addition to the variant of /s/ pronounced was coded in a Praat tier.
To determine the effect of extralinguistic factors, tokens of /s/ were coded for participant information. The first factor considered was the participants’ age at the time they arrived in Ciudad Bolivar. Two categories were identified: (a) older than seven years old and (b) younger than seven years old. The second factor analyzed was the level of education, with three categories: (a) primary, (b) secondary, and (c) technical. The third factor was age at the time of the interview: (a) between 15 and 26, (b) between 27 and 45, and (c) between 46 and 68. The fourth factor was gender: (a) female and (b) male. The fifth factor analyzed was socioeconomic status: (a) status 1 and (b) status 2, as described above.
Statistical analysis of variable realization of [s] in coda position was conducted using Goldvarb 3.0b9 for Mac (Sankoff et al. 2005). The logistic regression on the “binomial up & down” function (Rand and Sankoff 1990, pp. 24–27) of this multivariate analysis software for language variation research allowed us to determine which linguistic and extralinguistic factors had a significant conditioning effect on the variable.

3. Results

A total of 2322 tokens with /s/ in the coda position were analyzed by a native speaker of Colombian Spanish. Tokens are classified according to their realization in Table 1 and Table 2.
Table 1 shows that in word final position there were five different realizations of /s/, namely, a voiceless predorso-alveolar sibilant [s] (60.5%), aspirated [h] (19.8%), deleted [∅] (15.8%), voiced predorso-alveolar sibilant [z] (2.3%), and voiceless apico-alveolar sibilant [ s ] (1.6%). In word medial position, /s/ was realized as a voiceless predorso-alveolar sibilant [s] (82.1%), aspirated /h/ (14.6%), deleted [∅] (2.1%), voiced predorso-alveolar sibilant [z] (1.1%), and voiceless apico-alveolar [ s ] (0.2%).
Figure 1 and Figure 2 show that the percentage of production of the different variants of /s/ by the speakers of the three different D1 varieties are different between word-medial and word-final position. In all, there was a word-medial vs. word-final asymmetry, where the former was characterized by a higher rate of [s] production but the latter by more variation including a higher rate of aspiration and deletion. Moreover, the speakers of the East Andean variety exhibited a higher rate of [s] production, followed by the speakers of the West Andean variety, and then by the speakers of the Coastal variety. The rate of production of the different variants of /s/ in the East and West Andean varieties were similar.
Appendix A shows the rest of the results for percentages of production by position for the effect of each of the linguistic and extralinguistic factors examined in this (see Table A1, Table A2, Table A3, Table A4, Table A5, Table A6, Table A7, Table A8, Table A9, Table A10, Table A11, Table A12, Table A13 and Table A14).

Binomial Up & Down Analyses

This section presents the results of the GoldVarb logistic regression analysis which determined the statistical significance of the independent variables (factor groups) in predicting the dependent variable (realization of /s/). A factor weight greater than 0.5 indicates that the factor favors the application value (in this study [s], [h], [∅], [ s ], and [z]), while a factor weight less than 0.5 indicates that the factor disfavors it. The range of the factor group is calculated by subtracting the maximum factor weight of the factor group from the minimum factor weight.
Table 2 demonstrates that that [s] production was significantly favored by the following 7 linguistic factors: following segment as pause (0.769), voiceless consonant (0.675), high vowels (0.646), trisyllabic words (0.636), polysyllabic words (0.560), by stressed syllables (0.610), and word-medial position (0.620). It is disfavored by low vowels (0.466), voiced consonants (0.148), monosyllabic words (0.365), unstressed syllables (0.422), and word-final position (0.451). Regarding the region of origin, [s] production was favored by speakers from the Eastern Andean region (0.646) and the Western Andean region (0.614) and disfavored by the speakers of the Coastal region (0.299). In addition, [s] production was favored by the group of younger speakers (15–25) (0.623) and disfavored by the oldest ones (46–68) (0.402). Moreover, the extralinguistic factor of age of arrival was significant because the group of speakers who came to the D2 area when they were less than 7 years old favored [s] production (0.651), while those older than 7 disfavored it (0.474). Finally, females (0.544) favored [s] production while males (0.428) disfavored it. Previous segment, socioeconomic status, and level of education did not show any significant effect on [s] production.
The results analyzed so far show evidence that the rate of [s] production in the dialectal contact situation varies among the three groups examined. However, to obtain a more holistic image of what is happening in this dialect contact situation, the linguistic and extralinguistic factors analyzed above were also analyzed for the other realizations evinced in the data, namely aspirated [h], deletion [∅], voiceless apico-alveolar sibilant [ s ], and voiced predorso-alveolar sibilant [z].
Table 2 also reveals that the aspirated [h] variant was favored by the following linguistic factors: a voiced consonant (0.796), monosyllabic words (0.614), and unstressed syllables (0.559), while it is disfavored by voiceless consonants (0.487), low vowels (0.451), high vowels (0.353), and pauses (0.130). It is also favored by speakers of the Coastal variety (0.659), the older (46–68) population (0.559), and the middle-aged group (27–45) (0.529). Factors such as education, age of arrival, gender, socio-economic status, and word position did not show any significant effect on the production of aspirated realization [h].
Table 2 shows that deletion was favored by the linguistic factors of final position (0.607) following segment voiced consonant (0.781), low vowels (0.575), and unstressed syllables (0.540), while it was disfavored by pauses (0.444), voiceless consonants (0.342), and high vowels (0.139). It was also favored by the speakers of the Coastal variety (0.634), those who arrived after the age of 7 (0.531), and those with the lowest level of education, namely primary school (0.610). It was disfavored by the speakers of the Eastern Andean variety (0.455) Western Andean variety (0.351), those who had arrived below the age of 7 (0.324), and technical (0.474) and secondary school (0.446) level of education. Factors such as syllable stress, age, gender, socio-economic status, and word size did not show any significant effect on /s/ deletion.
Table 2 also demonstrates that voiceless apical [ s ] was favored by linguistic factors such as word final position (0.660) and disfavored by the medial word position (0.159). Regarding the extralinguistic factors, it was favored by speakers with technical level of education (0.820) and for those with primary school (0.729) and disfavored by speakers with secondary school level of education. Two groups of speakers favored this realization, speakers of the Western Andean variety (0.776) and the speakers of the Coastal variety (0.559). The group of speakers from the Eastern Andean region disfavored realization (0.224). Likewise, older speakers (46–68) favored it (0.771), while the those aged between 26–45 disfavored it (0.277). The rest of the linguistic and extralinguistic factors examined did not show any significant effect on the use of this variant.
Finally, Table 2 shows that variant [z] was favored by linguistic factors as following segment high vowels (0.882) and low vowels (0.814) and is disfavored by voiced consonants (0.478) and voiceless (0.441) consonants and by a following pause (0.360). Two groups of speakers favored /z/, the speakers of the Western Andean variety (0.751) and the speakers of the Coastal variety (0.528), while the speakers of the Eastern Andean disfavored it (0.300). /z/ was also favored by male speakers (0.800) and disfavored by female speakers (0.302). The group of middle-aged speakers (26–45) favored it, while the older group (46–68) (0.488) and the younger group (15–25) (0.343) disfavored it. The rest of linguistic and extralinguistic factors studied did not show any significant effect on the use of this variant.

4. Discussion

Three questions were examined in this article. The first was whether the degree of similarity between a D2 sound and its nearest D1 sound determines the rate of production of the D2 sound by the speakers of the three different D1 varieties of Colombia (Eastern Andean, Coastal, and Western Andean varieties), who had moved to Ciudad Bolivar and lived in contact with each other and with speakers of Bogota Spanish in Ciudad Bolivar. Taking into account the differences between D1 and D2 varieties regarding /s/ realization (Montes 1984; Flórez 1973; NGLEFF 2011) and based on the SLM (Flege 1995), H1 predicted that for the speakers of the Eastern Andean variety, the [s] sound would be an old sound, and therefore, it would not be problematic; for the speakers of the Coastal variety, it was predicted that [s] would either be a new sound or an old sound because these speakers tend to aspirate or delete /s/ in their D1. For the Western Andean region speakers, it was predicted that [s] would be a similar sound because they typically realize /s/ as voiceless apico-alveolar sibilant in their D1. This was based on the assumption that acoustic distance between /s/ and [ s ] was small and therefore, that the speakers of the West Andean variety would map [s] onto [ s ] and not acquire it. In all, it was predicted that the voiceless predorso-alveolar sibilant [s] would be favored by the speakers of the Eastern Andean variety, followed by the speakers of the Coastal variety and then by the speakers of the Western Andean variety.
The results partially supported our first hypothesis, since speakers from the Eastern Andean region favored voiceless predorso-alveolar sibilant [s] over other variants, as can be seen in Table 2. This is consistent with Montes (1984) and NGLEFF (2011), who report that the speakers of the Eastern Andean variety who maintain [s] in coda position favored the production of [s] (0.646), while the speakers of the Coastal variety (0.299) slightly favored [s] production when compared to the speakers of the Western Andean variety (0.614). This said, the speakers of this variety also exhibited some influence from other varieties of Spanish, namely the Coastal and Western Andean varieties, because they produced different rates of different variants such as deleted /s/ (10.1), [h] (12.9), [ s ] (0.4), and [z] (0.8). However, being a speaker of the Eastern Andean variety was not a significant predictor of these other variants [h] (0.371), deletion (0.455), [ s ] (0.224) and [z] (0.300), respectively).
The influence observed from the Western Andean varieties might be in part due to the fact that in Colombia, the speech of speakers from the Western Andean region has gained high prestige (Londoño et al. 2012; Bernal et al. 2014). This prestige may be due to the fact that a genre of television series known as “narconovelas” has gained popularity in the country. These series mostly revolve around the “paisa” culture and are viewed on platforms such as Netflix and HBO.
Based on the SLM (1995) we had predicted that the speakers of the Coastal variety would acquire [s] production in D2 speech learning because for them [s] would be a new or old sound since they tend to aspirate or delete it in their D1. However, the results show that the speakers of the Coastal variety had a lower rate of production of this sound in comparison with the speakers of the other two varieties. That the /s/ realization of these speakers was characterized by aspiration and deletion is consistent with the description of /s/ realization by the speakers of the Eastern Andean variety by Montes (1984) and NGLEFF (2011). Despite the fact that the Coastal variety in Colombia is stigmatized because of aspiration and deletion (Orozco 2018; Flórez 1963; Lipski 1996; Montes 1984; Quesada Pacheco 2011), the speakers of this variety do not exhibit a high rate of [s] production like the speakers of the Andean varieties. This could be due to the fact that regardless of the stigmatization of their speech within the country, the Coastal variety enjoys covert prestige within the speech community. Orozco (2004) argues that Coastal speakers disdain the Bogota variety and the results obtained in this study can be interpreted as reflecting Coastal speakers’ rejection of its predominant [s] variant. This suggests that attitudes need to be considered in an explanation of the interaction between different uses of the language and the construction of social identities in intercultural relations.
A different way to explain the results of the Coastal speakers could be that because the SLM makes predictions based on the assumption that a sound in the L2 may only be linked to another sound in the L1. That is, it does not consider phonological processes and allophonic variation. In this case, the speakers continue to aspirate and delete /s/ in their D2, suggesting that D1 phonological processes are transferred in to the D2 and/or that a D2 sound may be linked to more than one sound in the D1 in the same position. Therefore, in the future, the possibility of transfer of D1 phonological processes and/or the effect of allophonic variation needs to be considered in modeling D2 speech acquisition.
H1 also stated that speakers from the Western Andean region would not favor the production of the [s] variant because of the similarity between [s] and [ s ], the most frequent realization of the variable in their D1. The results (see Table 2 revealed that production of [ s ] is higher for the speakers of the Western Andean variety (0.776) than for the speakers of the Coastal (0.599) and Eastern Andean (0.224) varieties, which are consistent with the dialectal classification of the regions by Montes (1984) and NGLEFF (2011, p. 199). However, as shown in Table 3, the Western Andean region speakers also favored the production of [s] (0.614), which was not predicted by our first hypothesis. Their percentage realization of the various variants was as follows: [s] (72.6%), deletion (6.4%), [h] (15.5%), [ s ] (1.2), and [z] (4.3). Favoring [s] production by this group of speakers could be because the acoustic difference between [ s ] and [s] may not actually be as small as it had been predicted, and therefore, the speakers may indeed be able to perceive the difference between the D1 and the D2 and therefore learn to produce the [s]. Future studies need to determine the degree of similarity between [s] and [ s ] by conducting a perception test and further investigate the role of similarity between a D1 and D2 sounds in D2 speech learning. Foreman (2003) studied the acquisition of D2 Australian English vowels by speakers of North American English and found that the vowels that were phonetically closer to those of their variety were more readily acquired than the feature of non-rhoticity (see Chambers 1992).
It is also worth mentioning that similarly to the speakers of the East Andean variety, in this dialectal contact situation, the Western Andean group of speakers was a significant predictor of [z] (0.751) realization but not [h] (0.435) and deletion (0.351). Therefore, it appears that they may have been somewhat influenced by contact with the speakers of the Coastal variety, although not significantly. Another possibility is that some variation in /s/ realization may have preexisted in the speech of the Western Andean variety, although the literature does not support this idea. This warrants a more exhaustive study of /s/ variation in the varieties studied in this study.
The second question was related to which linguistic factors constrain /s/ realization in coda position in the group of speakers from the three D1 varieties. Hypothesis 2 predicted that /s/ realization would pattern in a way consistent with what had previously been reported regarding /s/ weakening in Spanish. The results indeed indicated a positional asymmetry, where [s] occurred more frequently in word-medial position than in word-final position, which is consistent with the results of previous studies regarding /s/ realization in Cartagena and Barranquilla (Becerra 1985; Lafford 1986; Rodríguez Cadena 2004). These results also support Lindblom’s theory of sound change and variation, which asserts that the word-final position is more vulnerable to change because it is not information bearing/salient and is therefore prone to hypo-articulation. Positional asymmetries have previously been reported in L2 speech learning studies (see Colantoni and Steele 2006; Rafat 2011). The role of universal phonetic factors has also been examined in D2 and L2 speech learning (Varona et al. 2018) and heritage speech as well (Rafat and Stevenson 2018).
The results also revealed that [s] is retained when the following segment is a voiceless consonant, a high vowel and when it occurs in a stressed syllable (see Table 2), but /s/ weakening occurs in the opposite contexts such as a voiced consonant, a low vowel, and an unstressed syllable (see Table 2). In addition, aspirations [h] and deletions are more frequent in final-word position when the following segment is a voiced consonant, a low vowel, and when it occurs in an unstressed syllable. These results are consistent with the results of previous studies such as those on Cartagena Spanish (Becerra 1985; Rodríguez Cadena 2004), Santo Domingo Spanish (Terrell 1981), Puerto Rican Spanish (Alba 1982; Poplack 1980), Cali Spanish (Brown and Torres Cacoullos 2002; Brown and Brown 2012; File-Muriel and Brown 2010).
Finally, while the results of this study showed that /s/ is maintained in polysyllabic words and weakened in monosyllabic ones, Alba (1990); Samper Padilla and Hernández Cabrera (1995) in their studies on Dominican Santiago and Las Palmas de Gran Canaria Spanish found the opposite. The effect of number of syllables will have to be further investigated in future studies.
We also investigated the effect of extralinguistic factors to see whether they would modulate the rate of [s] production by the three groups of D1 speakers. It was predicted that extralinguistic factors would affect the degree of acquisition of [s] by the speakers from the three D1 varieties studied. Specifically, it was predicted that the [s] variant would be favored by (a) participants who had arrived in Ciudad Bolivar in Bogota before the age of 7 (Kerswill and Williams 1994; Rys 2007; Siegel 2010; Tagliamonte and Molfenter 2007; Trudgill 1986; Bortoni-Ricardo 1985; Chambers 1992; Munro et al. 1999), (b) by those with the highest level of education (Labov 1972; Terrell 1981; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; Tagliamonte and Molfenter 2007; Chambers 1992), (c) by those with the highest socioeconomic status (Labov 1972, 2000; Diaz-Campos 2014; López Morales 1989; Becerra 1985; Lafford 1986), (d) by the youngest group (Lenneberg 1967; López Morales 1989; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; Terrell 1981; Ramírez and Almira 2011; Cedergren 1978; Villena Ponsoda 2012), and (e) by the female participants (Labov 1972; Holmquist 2011; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; De Weinberg 1973; Cedergren 1978; Poplack 1980; Foreman 2003; Rys 2007; Ruiz-Peña et al. 2015).
Our hypothesis regarding the age of arrival was supported for the speakers of the Coastal and Western Andean varieties, where the /s/ realization is different from /s/ that occurs in zone D2 (see Table 3). These results are consistent with D2 acquisition studies (Kerswill and Williams 1994; Rys 2007; Siegel 2010; Tagliamonte and Molfenter 2007; Trudgill 1986; Bortoni-Ricardo 1985; Chambers 1992; Munro et al. 1999; Nycz 2015) where they reported that age of arrival in zone D2 was a significant factor in the acquisition of phonological features of a D2 dialect. This factor has also been extensively analyzed in studies of L2 acquisition, where it has been also shown to significantly affect L2 speech learning (Abrahamsson 2012; Baker 2010; Johnson and Newport 1989; Major 2014). Age of arrival was not a significant factor for the speakers of the Eastern Andean region, possibly because [s] is an “old” sound in this variety.
Regarding the effect of the level of education, the results of the present study did not support our prediction that those with a higher level of education would favor [s] production. We propose this is due to the fact that the speakers had a relatively homogenous level of education, according to participant self-reports. In retrospect, it might have been a better idea to have classified the speakers based on the number of the years of education completed rather than the degree of education (primary school, secondary school, technical). However, it is worth mentioning that although we have not looked at interaction/cross-tabulations in this study, we noted that the speakers of the Coastal and Western Andean varieties with higher levels of education (secondary and technical) favored the production of the [s] variant, both in word-medial and word-final position. It is important to remember that [s], in addition to being the most frequent sound in zone D2, is also the most prestigious variant since it is the Colombian Spanish standard variant. These results are consistent with previous studies in which has also been shown that the level of education was a significant factor when measuring the preference of speakers for certain variants, especially those attributed to the standard language or those with high social prestige (Labov 1972; Terrell 1981; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; Tagliamonte and Molfenter 2007; Chambers 1992). As for the speakers of the Eastern Andean region, those with a higher level of education had a higher rate of [s] in the word-medial position only. This is consistent with the overall results of the study in which the production of [s] is favored more in word-medial position than in word-final position. Future studies should consider investigating interactions between region of origin, level of education, and position in the word.
With respect to the age of speakers, the results supported the hypothesis, since the younger group of participants favor [s] production. The results are consistent with previous studies of both L2 acquisition (Lenneberg 1967) and variationist studies (Labov 1994; López Morales 1989; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; Terrell 1981; Ramírez and Almira 2011; Cedergren 1978; Villena Ponsoda 2012), in which age has been observed as a marker in the apparent-time linguistic change hypothesis, where according to Labov (1972), linguistic change is frequently the result of innovations in younger speakers.
With respect to gender, the results support our prediction, since female speakers of the Coastal and Eastern Andean varieties favor [s] production in coda position, both in word-medial and word-final position. These results are consistent with previous studies of both variationist (Labov 1972; Rodríguez Cadena 2004; De Weinberg 1973; Cedergren 1978; Poplack 1980) and D2 acquisition studies (Foreman 2003; Rys 2007; Holmquist 2011; Ruiz-Peña et al. 2015), in which women have also been found to favor the production of standard variants. However, the results of this study showed that not all the observed groups had the same results, because unlike the other two groups, in the Western Andean variety, it was the male speakers who favored [s] production, compared to the female participants of the same variety, who conserved their D1 variant. One possible explanation for this phenomenon could be that women are aware of the prestige that the Western Andean variety enjoys, both locally in Ciudad Bolivar and nationally in Colombia, and therefore are conserving their variety. The difference observed in the Western Andean group with respect to the behavior of women could also be due to the fact that “gender does not have a uniform effect on the linguistic behavior of the community in general,” and “gender differences in socio-linguistic behavior materialize differently in different contexts.” (Orozco 2018, p. 130 citing Eckert 1989, p. 247; James 1996, p. 119).
Finally, regarding the socio-economic status factor, the results of the present study did not support our prediction that those with a higher socio-economic background would favor [s] production. We propose this is due to the fact that the speakers came from a relatively homogenous socioeconomic group, according to participant self-reports. All participants had self-identified as belonging to level status 1 or 2.

Implications for Models of D2 Speech Learning

Although the field of D2 speech learning is growing (Esparza and Mazzaro 2018; Foreman 2003; Rys 2007; Holmquist 2011; Ruiz-Peña et al. 2015; Siegel 2010), currently we do not have a model of D2 speech learning. Some of the previous studies as well as the current study have relied on the SLM; however, as we have seen here, D2 speech learning is a complex phenomenon in which several linguistic and extra-linguistic factors may play a role. Based on the data in this study it is not immediately clear whether the SLM can explain D2 speech learning. This is partly because we do not know whether the assumption that the acoustic distance between [ s ] and [s] is small. The effect of the degree of acoustic distance between a D2 and its nearest D1 sound on equivalence classification in D2 speech learning certainly merits further investigation. Moreover, given that there was evidence to suggest that a variationist approach can explain some of the D2 acquisition patterns, we recommend that future models consider both linguistic and extralinguistic factors. Moreover, because our findings are consistent with those reported in variationist studies regarding factors affecting language variation and change, we also suggest that universal phonetic principles such as positional salience that determine sound change be incorporated into future models. Moreover, it is crucial that the role of D1 phonological processes be considered in modeling D2 speech learning.

5. Conclusions

The present study makes a novel contribution to our understanding of D2 speech learning of Spanish by showing various linguistic and extralinguistic factors that may constrain the production of [s] in a contact situation. The results for the rate of production of the D2 variant [s] in coda position differs between the speakers of the Eastern Andean, Coastal, and Western Andean varieties that have been in contact in Ciudad Bolivar in Bogota, Colombia. Specifically, it was found that the speakers of the Eastern Andean variety favor the production of [s] the most, followed by the speakers of the Western Andean variety and then the speakers of the Coastal variety. The latter group tends to exhibit aspiration and deletion. Aspiration and deletion were also evident in the productions of the speakers of the Eastern and Western Andean varieties at a lower rate and mainly in the word-final coda position, suggesting that speakers exhibited some degree of influence from different varieties in contact.
We tested the SLM, but the prediction of this model did not always hold for the contact situation we studied. We have proposed that future perception tests and future models of D2 speech learning consider the effect of D1 phonological processes and/or variants as well as linguistic and extra-linguistic factors that were tested in this study.
Finally, we have drawn parallels with previous sociolinguistic studies and specifically variationist studies on /s/ as well as L2 acquisition studies. Doing so, we have made a new contribution to the field of D2 speech learning, by showing that some of the factors that may influence language variation and change as well as L2 acquisition also affect D2 speech learning.
In addition, although the main goal of the study was not to investigate sound change in a dialect contact situation, the results of this research suggest that dialectal contact is a factor that promotes linguistic change in Bogota Spanish as stated (Montes 1984). These results also suggest that /s/ variation is a complex phenomenon since both linguistic and extralinguistic factors are involved.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.G., Y.R. and J.T.; methodology, C.G., J.T.; formal analysis, C.G. and Y.R.; data curation, C.G.; writing—original draft preparation, C.G.; writing—review and editing, C.G., Y.R. and J.T.; visualization, C.G., Y.R. and J.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare there are no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. The effect of the following segment: percentages of production by position.
Table A1. The effect of the following segment: percentages of production by position.
Word MedialWord Final
[s][ø][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%
Vc85.41.212.60.20.7757.115.61.40.9
Vdc39.213.741.205.93227.838.90.50.7
Lv100000064.814.2131.86.1
Hv100000077.32.39.13.48
PNANANANANA82.510.53.51.81.8
EsNANANANANA82.4111.94.60
Vc = voiceless consonant, Vdc = voiced consonant, Lv = low vowel, Hv = high vowel, P = pause, Es = end of sentence, NA = not applicable.
Table A2. The effect of the previous segment: percentages of production by position.
Table A2. The effect of the previous segment: percentages of production by position.
Word MedialWord Final
[s][ø][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%
Lv.841140160162022
Hv.697201365131804
Vc.8501500NANANANANA
Lv = low vowel, Hv = high vowel, Vc = voiceless consonant, NA = not applicable.
Table A3. The effect of the stress syllable: percentages of production by position.
Table A3. The effect of the stress syllable: percentages of production by position.
Word MedialWord Final
[s][ø][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%
Stressed831.3150.20.96014221.32.9
Unstressed8141401.56118181.81.8
Table A4. The effect of number of syllables: percentage of production by position.
Table A4. The effect of number of syllables: percentage of production by position.
Word MedialWord Final
[s][ø][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%
10000058.713.9231.43
279.72.816.40.30.859.716.7202.11.5
390.11.47.101.46416.915.71.22.3
4731.124.701.165.221.410.71.80.9
1 = one syllable, 2 = two syllables, 3 = three syllables, 4 = four syllables.
Table A5. The effect of the level of education: percentages of production by region and word medial position.
Table A5. The effect of the level of education: percentages of production by region and word medial position.
Primary SchoolSecondary SchoolTechnical
[s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z] [s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
EA80.22.37.81.28.6931.33.901.795.600.603.8
C74.91.615.74.73.188.418.60282.70.512.713
WA79.2005.715.184.90.83.71.98.8NANANANANA
EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Table A6. The effect of the level of education: percentages of production by region and word final position.
Table A6. The effect of the level of education: percentages of production by region and word final position.
Primary SchoolSecondary SchoolTechnical
[s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
EA7411.512.80.90.967.214.317.800.868.112.619.300
C40.738.1182.60.552.91530.30.41.551.918.819.96.13.3
WA63.21.520.67.47.46510190.65.4NANANANANA
EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Table A7. The effect of gender: percentages of production by region and word-medial position.
Table A7. The effect of gender: percentages of production by region and word-medial position.
FemaleMale
[s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%
EA921.75.30.20.5820.93.50.913
C901.58.10.20.5740.4173.55.6
WA940.82.62.30680.64.32.425
EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Table A8. The effect of the gender: percentages of production by region and word-final position.
Table A8. The effect of the gender: percentages of production by region and word-final position.
FemaleMale
[s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%
EA6912180.50.27314120. 01.5
C5421241.30.54226244.73.5
WA725.2200.91.35413183. 012
EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Table A9. The effect of age of arrival: percentages of production by region and word-medial position.
Table A9. The effect of age of arrival: percentages of production by region and word-medial position.
Under 7 YearsOlder Than 7 Years
[s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%
EA95.802.801.4903.150.61.2
C92.307.700652.930.801.2
WA100000090.71.37.300.7
EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Table A10. The effect of age of arrival: percentages of production by region and word-final position.
Table A10. The effect of age of arrival: percentages of production by region and word-final position.
Under 7 YearsOlder Than 7 Years
[s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%
EA68.21417.80070.412.815.60.40.8
C754.420.60045.725.124.331.9
WA78.6017.93.6063.69.219.31.66.2
EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Table A11. The effect of socio-economic status: percentages of production by region and word-medial position.
Table A11. The effect of socio-economic status: percentages of production by region and word-medial position.
Level 1Level 2
[s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%
EA8815.50.64.9902.6205.3
C821121.72.7841111.42.4
WA840.97.11.65.9881.47.00.83.2
EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Table A12. The effect of socio-economic status: percentages of production by region and word- final position.
Table A12. The effect of socio-economic status: percentages of production by region and word- final position.
Level 1Level 2
[s][∅]/h/[ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%
EA7114150.40.6689.92100.7
C4626232.62.65319252.70.4
WA629.5201.76.6791.91.71.90.0
EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Table A13. The effect of age of participant: percentages of production by region and word medial position.
Table A13. The effect of age of participant: percentages of production by region and word medial position.
15–2526–4546–68
[s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
EA97110187.92.55.40.43.886.20.75.30.77.2
C91.51.26.40.30.679.813.4015.874.90.416.34.44
WA93.50.93.70.90.976.11.517.205.283.202.57.66.7
EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Table A14. The effect of age of participant: percentages of production by region and word final position.
Table A14. The effect of age of participant: percentages of production by region and word final position.
15–2526–4546–68
[s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z][s][∅][h][ s ][z]
%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%
EA79.66.114.300.71.59.917.800.865.11815.50.70.7
C56.517.624.310.662.98.620.30.57.635.430.125.46.72.4
WA71.812.911.803.553.323.819.70.3.362.45.123.15.14.3
EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.

References

  1. Abrahamsson, Niclas. 2012. Age of onset and nativelike L2 ultimate attainment of morphosyntactic and phonetic intuition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 34: 187–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Alba, Orlando. 1982. Función del acento en el proceso de elisión de la /s/ en la República Dominicana. In El español del Caribe: Ponencias del VI simposio de dialectología. Edited by Orlando Alba. Santiago: Universidad Católica Madrey Maestra, pp. 17–26. [Google Scholar]
  3. Alba, Orlando. 1990. Variación fonética y diversidad social en el español dominicano de Santiago. Santiago: Pontificia Universidad Católica Madre y Maestra, pp. 217–18. [Google Scholar]
  4. Brown, Earl. 2009a. The relative importance of lexical frequency in syllable and word final /s/reduction in Cali, Colombia. In Selected Proceedings of the 11th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Edited by Joseph Collentine, Mary Ellen García, Barbara Lafford and Francisco Marcos Marín. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 165–78. [Google Scholar]
  5. Brown, Earl. 2009b. A Usage-Based Account of Syllable- and Word-Final /s/ Reduction in Four Dialects of Spanish. Munich: Lincom. [Google Scholar]
  6. Babel, Molly. 2011. Evidence for phonetic and social selectivity in spontaneous phonetic imitation. Journal of Phonetics 40: 177–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Baker, Wendy. 2010. Effects of age and experience on the production of English word-final stops by Korean speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13: 263–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Becerra, Servio. 1985. Fonología de las consonantes implosivas en el español urbano de Cartagena de Indias (Colombia). Bogotá: Instituto Caro y Cuervo. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bernal, Julio, Alejandro Munévar, and Catalina Barajas. 2014. Actitudes lingüísticas en Colombia. Bergen Language and Linguistics Studies 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Berthele, Raphael. 2002. Learning a second dialect: A model of idiolectal dissonance. Multilingua 21: 327–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Best, Catherine T., and Michael D. Tyler. 2007. Nonnative and Second-Language Speech Perception. Language Experience in Second Language Speech Learning Language Learning & Language Teaching, 13–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Boersma, Paul, and David Weenink. 2016. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer (Version 6.0. 23) [Computer Software]. Amsterdam: Authors. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bortoni-Ricardo, Stella Maris. 1985. The Urbanization of Rural Dialect Speakers. A Sociolinguistic Study in Brazil. Cambridge Studies in Linguistics. London: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  14. Brown, Cynthia. 1998. The role of the L1 grammar in the L2 acquisition of segmental structure. Second Language Research 14: 136–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Brown, Earl Kent, and Esther Brown. 2012. Syllable-final and syllable-initial/s/reduction in Cali, Colombia: One variable or two. In Colombian Varieties of Spanish. Frankfurt am Main and Alemania: Iberoamericana Vervuert Publishing Corp, pp. 89–106. [Google Scholar]
  16. Brown, Earl Kent, and Rena Torres Cacoullos. 2002. Que le vamoh aher?: Taking the syllable out of Spanish /s/ reduction. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 8: 17–31. [Google Scholar]
  17. Brown, Esther L. 2004. The Reduction of Syllable Initial /s/ in the Spanish of New Mexico and Southern Colorado: A Usage-Based Approach. Ph.D. thesis, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA. [Google Scholar]
  18. Caicedo, Humberto. 1992. Dialecto y sociedad en Buenaventura. Cali: Revista Lenguaje, pp. 19–20, 55–67. [Google Scholar]
  19. Canfield, Delos Lincoln. 1988. El español de América: Fonética. Barcelona: Editorial Crítica. [Google Scholar]
  20. Cedergren, Henrietta Jonas. 1978. En torno a la variación de la S final de sílaba en Panamá: Análisis cuantitativo. Corrientes actuales en la dialectología del Caribe hispánico. Río Piedras: Universidad de Puerto Rico, pp. 80–103. [Google Scholar]
  21. Chambers, Jack. 1992. Dialect acquisition. Language 68: 673–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Colantoni, Laura, and Jeffrey Steele. 2006. Native-like attainment in the L2 acquisition of Spanish stop-liquid clusters. In Selected Proceedings of the 7th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages. Somerville: Cascadilla. [Google Scholar]
  23. Colantoni, Laura, and Jeffrey Steele. 2008. Integrating Articulatory Constraints into Models of Second Language Phonological Acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics 29: 489–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Chamorro, Mónica, and Karen López. 2020. Aspectos afectivos de las actitudes lingüísticas de estudiantes universitarios. Cuadernos de Lingüística Hispánica 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Departamento Administrativo Nacional de Estadística (DANE). 2018. Censo Nacional de Población y Vivienda 2018. Available online: https://www.dane.gov.co/index.php/estadisticas-por-tema/demografia-y-poblacion/censo-nacional-de-poblacion-y-vivenda-2018 (accessed on 20 February 2020).
  26. De Weinberg, María Beatriz Fontanella. 1973. Comportamiento ante “s” de hablantes femeninos y masculinos del español bonaerense. Romance Philology 27: 50–58. [Google Scholar]
  27. Díaz-Campos, Manuel. 2014. Introducción a la sociolingüística hispánica. Hoboken: Wiley Blackwell & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  28. Duarte, Gloria. 2005. Actitudes idiomáticas de los bogotanos frente a los dialectos del español de Colombia. Edited by En Mariano Lozano. Homenaje a José Joaquín Montes Giraldo: Estudios de dialectología, lexicografía, lingüística general, etnolingüística e historia cultura. Bogotá: Instituto Caro y Cuervo. [Google Scholar]
  29. Eckert, Penelope. 1989. The whole woman: Sex and gender differences in variation. Language Variation and Change 1: 245–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Escure, Genevieve. 1997. Creole and Dialect Continua: Standard Acquisition Processes in Belize and China (PRC). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing, vol. 18. [Google Scholar]
  31. Esparza, Sandra, and Natalia Mazzaro. 2018. Variantes de /s/ y ajustes del habla en los asentamientos antiguos y nuevos de veracruzanos en Ciudad Juárez. Revista Iberoamericana de Lingüística 13: 121–50. [Google Scholar]
  32. File-Muriel, Richard. 2007. The Role of Lexical Frequency and Phonetic Context in the Weakening of Syllable-Final Lexical /s/ in the Spanish of Barranquilla, Colombia. Ph.D. dissertation, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA. [Google Scholar]
  33. File-Muriel, Richard. 2010. Lexical frequency as a scalar variable in explaining variation. The Canadian Journal of Linguistics 55: 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. File-Muriel, Richard, and Earl Kent Brown. 2010. The gradient nature of s-lenition in Caleño Spanish. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 16: 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. File-Muriel, Richard, and Rafael Orozco, eds. 2012. Colombian Varieties of Spanish. Madrid, Frankfurt and Iberoamericana: Vervuert. [Google Scholar]
  36. Flege, James Emil. 1995. Second-language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In Speech perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research. Edited by Winifred Strange. Timonium: York, pp. 233–73. [Google Scholar]
  37. Flórez, Luis. 1963. El español hablado en Colombia y su atlas lingüístico. Thesaurus 1: 268–356. [Google Scholar]
  38. Flórez, Luis. 1973. Las “apuntaciones críticas” de Cuervo y el español Bogotano cien años después. Bogotá: Editorial Instituto Caro y Cuervo. [Google Scholar]
  39. Foreman, Annick. 2003. Pretending to Be Someone You’re Not: A Study of Second Dialect Acquisition in Australia. Ph.D. dissertation, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia. [Google Scholar]
  40. Harris, James. 1983. Syllable Structure and Stress in Spanish: A Nonlinear Analysis. Cambridge: Linguistic Inquiry Monographs, pp. 1–158. [Google Scholar]
  41. Holmquist, Jonathan. 2011. Gender and Variation: Word final /s/ in Men’s and Women’s Speech in Puerto Rico’s Western Highlands. In The Handbook of Hispanic Sociolinguistics. Edited by Manuel Diaz-Campos. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 230–43. [Google Scholar]
  42. Honsa, Vladimir. 1975. Clasificación de los dialectos españoles de América y la estructura de los dialectos de Colombia. In Actas del III Congreso de Asociación Lingüística y Filología de la América Latina. México: Asociación de Lingüística y Enseñanza de Idiomas. [Google Scholar]
  43. Ivars, Ann-Marie. 1994. Bidialectalism and identity. In The Sociolinguistics of Urbanization: The Case of the Nordic Countries. Edited by Bengt Nordberg. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, pp. 203–22. [Google Scholar]
  44. Izquierdo, Milagros Aleza, and José María Enguita Utrilla. 2002. El Español de América: Aproximación Sincrónica. Valencia: Tirant lo Blanch. [Google Scholar]
  45. James, Deborah. 1996. Women, men and prestige speech forms: A critical review. In Rethinking Language and Gender Research: Theory and Practice. Edited by Victoria L. Bergvall, Janet M. Bing and Alice F. Freed. London and New York: Longman. [Google Scholar]
  46. Johnson, Jacqueline, and Elisa L. Newport. 1989. Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive psychology 21: 60–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Kerswill, Paul E., and Ann Williams. 1994. A New Dialect in a New City: Childrens’ and Adults’ Speech in Milton Keynes. Milton Keynes: Economic and Social Research Council. [Google Scholar]
  48. Labov, William. 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. [Google Scholar]
  49. Labov, William. 1994. Principles of Language Change: Internal Factors. Madrid: Gredos. [Google Scholar]
  50. Labov, William. 2000. Principles of Language Change. Vol II. Social Factors. Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  51. Lafford, Barbara A. 1986. Valor diagnóstico-social del uso de ciertas variantes de /s/ en el español de Cartagena, Colombia. In Estudios sobre la fonología del español del Caribe. Edited by Rafael Núñez-Cedeño, Iraset Páez and Jorge Guitart. Caracas: La Casa de Bello, pp. 53–75. [Google Scholar]
  52. Lenneberg, Eric. 1967. Biological Foundations of Language / [by] Eric H. Lenneberg. With Appendices by Noam Chomsky and Otto Marx. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  53. Lipski, John. 1984. On the weakening of /s/ in Latin American Spanish. Germany. Zeitschrift für Dialektologie und Linguistik 51: 31–43. [Google Scholar]
  54. Lipski, John. 1996. Introducción a la descripción de los dialectos del español de América. In En: El Español de América. Madrid: Cátedra. [Google Scholar]
  55. Lipski, John. 1999. The many faces of Spanish /s/-weakening: (Re)Alignment and ambisyllabicity. In Advances in Hispanic Linguistics. Edited by Javier Gutierrez-Rexach and Fernando Martinez-Gil. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, pp. 198–213. [Google Scholar]
  56. Londoño, Rafael Areiza, Mireya Cisneros Estupiñán, and Luis Enrique Tabares Idárraga. 2012. Sociolingüística: Enfoques Pragmático y Variacionista. Bogotá: ECOE ediciones. [Google Scholar]
  57. López Morales, Luis. 1983. La estratificación Social del Español de Puerto Rico. México: UNAM, pp. 74–75. [Google Scholar]
  58. López Morales, Luis. 1989. Sociolingüística. Madrid: Editorial Gredos. [Google Scholar]
  59. Major, Charisse Alaine. 2014. The Effect of Age on Second Language Acquisition in Older Adults. Master’s thesis, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USA. Available online: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd/3973 (accessed on 8 April 2020).
  60. Mazzaro, Natalia. 2003. Speaking Spanish with Style: A Sociolinguistic Analysis of /s/ in Corrientes, Argentina. Master’s dissertation, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
  61. Mazzaro, Natalia. 2011. Experimental Approaches to Sound Variation: A Sociophonetic Study of Labial and Velar Fricatives and Approximants in Argentine Spanish. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
  62. Montes, José Joaquín. 1984. Algunos casos de /s/ sonora en Colombia y sus implicaciones dialectales, en Homenaje a Luis Flórez. Bogotá: Instituto Caro y Cuervo. [Google Scholar]
  63. Montes, José Joaquín. 2000. Otros estudios sobre el español de Colombia. Bogotá: Instituto Caro y Cuervo. [Google Scholar]
  64. Munro, Murray James, Tracey M. Derwing, and James Emil Flege. 1999. Canadians in Alabama: A perceptual study of dialect acquisition in adults. Journal of Phonetics 27: 385–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. NGLEFF. 2011. Real Academia Española &Asociación de Academias de la Lengua Española. Nueva Gramática de la Lengua Española: Fonética y Fonología (2011). Madrid: Espasa Libros. [Google Scholar]
  66. Nielsen, Kuniko. 2011. Specificity and abstractness of VOT imitation. Journal of Phonetics 39: 132–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Nycz, Jennifer. 2015. Second Dialect Acquisition: A Sociophonetic Perspective. Language and Linguistics Compass 9: 469–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Orozco, Rafael. 2004. A sociolinguistic study of Colombian Spanish in Colombia and New York City. Ph.D. thesis, New York University, New York City, NY, USA. [Google Scholar]
  69. Orozco, Rafael. 2018. Spanish in Colombia and New York City: Language Contact Meets Dialectal Convergence. Amnsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, vol. 46. [Google Scholar]
  70. Pacheco, Allen Quesada. 2011. Issues on raising a bilingual child in Costa Rica: A myth or a reality? Revista Electrónica “Actualidades Investigativas en Educación” 11: 1–21. [Google Scholar]
  71. Payne, Arvilla Chapin. 1976. The Acquisition of the Phonological System of a Second Dialect. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA. [Google Scholar]
  72. Penny, Ralph. 2000. Variation and Change in Spanish. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  73. Poplack, Shana. 1980. The notion of the plural in Puerto Rican Spanish: Competing constraints on (s) deletion. In Locating Language in Time and Space. Edited by William Labov. New York: Academic Press, pp. 56–67. [Google Scholar]
  74. Rafat, Yasaman. 2011. Orthography-Induced Transfer in the Production of Novice Adult English-Speaking Learners of Spanish. Ph.D. thesis, Toronto University, Toronto, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
  75. Rafat, Yasaman, and Ryan A. Stevenson. 2018. Auditory-orthographic integration at the onset of L2 speech acquisition. Language and Speech 62: 427–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Rand, David, and David Sankoff. 1990. GoldVarb Version 2: A Variable Rule Application for the Macintosh¬ Tm. Montréal: Université de Montréa. [Google Scholar]
  77. Ramírez, Alexander, and Alina Almira. 2011. Descripción fonética del español culto caleño. Acercamiento exploratorio no exhaustivo al comportamiento de /s/ en los diferentes contextos en juntura de palabra. Master’s thesis, Universidad del Valle, Cali, Colombia. [Google Scholar]
  78. Rodríguez Cadena, Yolanda. 2004. Variación y cambio lingüístico en el Caribe colombiano: Estudio sobre la ciudad de Barranquilla. Historia y Cultura 1: 103–20. [Google Scholar]
  79. Ruiz-Peña, Esperanza, Diego Sevilla, and Yasaman Rafat. 2015. Second dialect imitation: The production of Ecuadorian Spanish assibilated rhotics by Andalusian speakers of Spanish. Paper presented at Monolingual and Bilingual Speech, Chania, Greece, 7–10 September 2015; p. 288. [Google Scholar]
  80. Rys, Kathy. 2007. Dialect as Second Language: Linguistic and Non-Linguistic Factors in Secondary Dialect Acquisition by Children and Adolescents. Ph.D. thesis, Ghent University, Gent, Belgium. [Google Scholar]
  81. Samper Padilla, José Antonio, and Clara Eugenia Hernández Cabrera. 1995. La variación de-/s/ en el español culto de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria: Condicionantes lingüísticos. Philologica Canariensia 2: 391–408. [Google Scholar]
  82. Sankoff, David, Sali Tagliamonte, and Eric Smith. 2005. Goldvarb X: A Variable Rule Application for Macintosh and Windows. Toronto: Department of Linguistics, University of Toronto. [Google Scholar]
  83. Siegel, Jeff. 2010. Second Dialect Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  84. Sibata, Takesi. 1958. Conditions controlling standardization. Excerpt from Nihon no hogen [The dialects of Japan]. Tokyo: Iwanami Shoten, Translated by Motoei Sa-waki. 1990, MS. [Google Scholar]
  85. Tagliamonte, Sali, and Sonja Molfenter. 2007. How’d You Get That Accent?: Acquiring a Second Dialect of the Same Language. Language in Society 36: 649–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Tararova, Olga. 2012. A Study of In-Group And Out-Group Attitudes in An Italo-Mexican Community, Chipilo. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
  87. Tejada Giráldez, María. 2012. Los factores lingüísticos de la /-s/ implosiva en el nivel de estudios altos de Granada. de Valencia: Normas: Revista de Estudios Lingüísticos Hispánicos 2: 185–217. [Google Scholar]
  88. Terrell, Tracy. 1981. Diachronic reconstruction by dialect comparison of variable constraints: S-aspiration and deletion in Spanish. In Variation Omnibus. Edited by Henrietta Cedergren and David Sankoff. Edmonton: Linguistic Research Inc., pp. 115–24. [Google Scholar]
  89. Terrell, Tracy. 1986. La desaparición de/s/ posnuclear a nivel léxico en el habla dominicana. In Estudios sobre la fonología del español del Caribe. Edited by Rafael Núñez-Cedeño, Iraset Páez Urdaneta and Jorge Guitart. Caracas: La casa de Bello, pp. 117–34. [Google Scholar]
  90. Trudgill, Peter. 1981. Linguistic accommodation: Sociolinguistic observations on a sociopsychological theory. Parasession on Language and Behavior, Chicago Linguistic Society 17: 218–37. [Google Scholar]
  91. Trudgill, Peter. 1986. Dialects in Contact. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  92. Ureña, Pedro Henríquez. 1921. Observaciones sobre el español en América. Granada. Revista de Filología Española 8: 357–90. [Google Scholar]
  93. Varona, Daniel, Esperanza Ruiz-Pena, Elkin Darío Sierra, Ryan Stevenson, and Yasaman Rafat. 2018. Second dialect and second language imitation of geminates by Colombian Spanish speakers. Paper presented at the International Symposium on Monolingual and Bilingual Speech, Chania, Greece; December, pp. 99–106. Available online: http://www.ismbs.eu/publications-2018/ (accessed on 8 April 2020).
  94. Villena Ponsoda, Juan Andrés. 2012. Patrones sociológicos del español de Andalucía. In Estudios Sobre el Español de Málaga: Pronunciación, Vocabulario y Sintaxis. Edited by Antonio Manuel Ávila Muñoz. Málaga: Sarriá. Wiley Blackwell & Sons, Inc., pp. 27–66. [Google Scholar]
  95. Zamora, Juan, and Jorge Guitart. 1982. Dialectología hispanoamericana. Salamanca: Ediciones Almar. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Percentages of /s/ production in word-medial position by region in contact in Ciudad Bolivar. Note: EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Figure 1. Percentages of /s/ production in word-medial position by region in contact in Ciudad Bolivar. Note: EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Languages 05 00015 g001
Figure 2. Percentages of /s/ production in word-final position by region in contact in Ciudad Bolivar. Note: EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Figure 2. Percentages of /s/ production in word-final position by region in contact in Ciudad Bolivar. Note: EA = Eastern Andean, C = Coastal, WA = Western Andean.
Languages 05 00015 g002
Table 1. /s/ realization in coda position by Colombian speakers in Ciudad Bolivar, Bogotá.
Table 1. /s/ realization in coda position by Colombian speakers in Ciudad Bolivar, Bogotá.
PositionType of ProductionTotal%
[s][∅][h][ s ][z]
FinalN10042623292638165971.4
%60.515.819.81.62.3
MedialN54414971766328.6
%82.12.114.60.21.1
TOTALN154827642627452322
%66.711.918.31.21.9
Table 2. Summary of linguistic and extra-linguistic factors favoring the 5 variants of /s/.
Table 2. Summary of linguistic and extra-linguistic factors favoring the 5 variants of /s/.
Linguistic and Extralinguistic FactorsVariants
[s][h][∅][ s ][z]
Following segmentWeightWeightWeightWeightWeight
Pause0.7690.1300.444[ ]0.441
Vc0.6750.4870.342[ ]0.360
Hv0.6410.3530.139[ ]0.882
Lv0.4660.4510.575[ ]0.814
Vdc0.1480.7960.781[ ]0.478
Range626764 52
Region of origin
Eastern Andean0.6460.3710.4550.2240.300
Western Andean0.6140.4350.3510.7760.751
Coastal0.2990.6590.6340.5990.528
Range3529285545
Number of syllables
Trisyllabic0.6360.360[ ][ ][ ]
Polysyllabic0.5600.462[ ][ ][ ]
Disyllabic0.5130.503[ ][ ][ ]
Monosyllabic0.3650.614[ ][ ][ ]
Range2725
Age
15–250.6230.399[ ]0.4170.343
26–450.4930.529[ ]0.2770.650
46–68 0.4020.559[ ]0.7710.488
Range2216 5031
Syllable Stress
Stressed0.6100.4170.540[ ][ ]
Unstressed0.4220.5590.443[ ][ ]
Range19149
Age of Arrival
Under 70.651[ ]0.324[ ][ ]
Older than 70.474[ ]0.531[ ][ ]
Range17 21
Word position
Medial0.620[ ]0.2510.159[ ]
Final0.451[ ]0.6070.660[ ]
Range0.169[ ]3650[ ]
Gender
Female 0.544[ ][ ][ ]0.302
Male 0.428[ ][ ][ ]0.800
Range12 50
Socioeconomic status
Level 1 [ ][ ][ ][ ][ ]
Level 2[ ][ ][ ][ ][ ]
Education
Secondary school[ ][ ]0.4460.247[ ]
Primary school[ ][ ]0.6100.729[ ]
Technical[ ][ ]0.4740.820[ ]
Range 1657
Vc = voiceless consonant, Lv = low vowel, Hv = high vowel, Vdc = voiced consonant [ ] = the analysis did not reveal significant effects among these factors and [s].
Table 3. Summary of the region factor weights for occurrence of /s/ variants, taken from the Up & Down analysis Table 2.
Table 3. Summary of the region factor weights for occurrence of /s/ variants, taken from the Up & Down analysis Table 2.
Region of Origin[s][h][∅][ s ] [z]
Eastern Andean0.6460.3710.4550.2240.300
Coastal0.2990.6590.6340.5990.528
Western Andean0.6140.4350.3510.7760.751

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gómez, C.; Tennant, J.; Rafat, Y. Towards Modeling Second Dialect Speech Learning: The Production of Bogota [s] in Ciudad Bolivar by Speakers of Three Different Varieties of Colombian Spanish. Languages 2020, 5, 15. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5020015

AMA Style

Gómez C, Tennant J, Rafat Y. Towards Modeling Second Dialect Speech Learning: The Production of Bogota [s] in Ciudad Bolivar by Speakers of Three Different Varieties of Colombian Spanish. Languages. 2020; 5(2):15. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5020015

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gómez, Cenaida, Jeff Tennant, and Yasaman Rafat. 2020. "Towards Modeling Second Dialect Speech Learning: The Production of Bogota [s] in Ciudad Bolivar by Speakers of Three Different Varieties of Colombian Spanish" Languages 5, no. 2: 15. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5020015

APA Style

Gómez, C., Tennant, J., & Rafat, Y. (2020). Towards Modeling Second Dialect Speech Learning: The Production of Bogota [s] in Ciudad Bolivar by Speakers of Three Different Varieties of Colombian Spanish. Languages, 5(2), 15. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5020015

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop