(A)typical Clauses across Languages

A special issue of Languages (ISSN 2226-471X).

Deadline for manuscript submissions: closed (1 September 2024) | Viewed by 2359

Special Issue Editors


E-Mail Website
Guest Editor
Department of Finnish, Finno-Ugrian and Scandinavian Studies, University of Helsinki, 00100 Helsinki, Finland
Interests: usage-based grammar; interactional linguistics; Finnish

E-Mail Website
Guest Editor
Faculty of Arts, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB T6G 2R3, Canada
Interests: usage-based linguistics; language documentation; Japanese; Miyako

Special Issue Information

Dear Colleagues,

In both scholarly papers and textbooks, linguists have presented the typical clause as consisting of an active, transitive verb and its overt arguments. For example, consider the following, constructed clause given in a section on structural analysis in a popular introductory linguistics textbook (Yule, 2006: 80; see also, e.g., Comrie, 1989, and Payne, 1997):

Her father brought a shotgun to the wedding

The conception of the clause in the dominant grammatical tradition, passed on in linguistic training and reflected in our terminology, is based on a rather limited set of data. Such a conception of the clause originated from the study of constructed ‘sentences’ and is heavily influenced by written language, primarily from the Indo-European family (for discussion, see Linell, 2005; Ono, Laury and Suzuki, 2021).

However, it has been shown that, in conversation, the most basic use type of human languages (Schegloff, 1996; Fillmore, 1974), transitive clauses such as the one above are actually not that common. Thompson and Hopper (2001) showed that, in English conversation, two-participant clauses account for only 27% of all clauses in the conversational data they studied, with one-participant clauses accounting for 73%. Similar results have been obtained from several other languages (for Finnish, see Helasvuo, 2001; for Javanese, see Ewing, 2005; for Japanese, see Ono, Sadler and Daiju, 2020; and for Russian, see Turk, 2000). Furthermore, in many languages, overt mentions of participants who are involved in the action or state expressed in the clause are rare (for Japanese, see, e.g., Ono and Thompson, 1997; for Indonesian, see Ewing, 2019). Thus, it appears that the clause types that are often thought to be the most common are actually atypical, even in the most commonly studied languages from the Indo-European language family.

In our Special Issue, we propose to examine the syntactic formats of common and uncommon clause types and their functions in everyday conversation in a wide range of geographically, typologically, and genetically unrelated languages. We will explore the analytical and theoretical relevance of the concept of the ‘clause’ for our understanding of human language in general. We ask questions such as (1) how the concept of the ‘clause’ should be defined and whether the celebrated status given to it is deserving, given its manifestation in actual data across languages (Ono et al., 2019); (2) how ‘verbless’ clause types should be understood (Haspelmath, 2022); and (3) whether the concept of the ‘predicate’ might be more useful in crosslinguistic study than the concept of the ‘clause’ (Thompson, 2019). We encourage the participation of scholars who are interested in the status of the clause in everyday conversation, regardless of theoretical orientations and language specialties.

Tentative Completion Schedule
Abstract Submission Deadline: 5 January 2024
Notification of Abstract Acceptance: 1 February 2024
Full Manuscript Deadline: 1 September 2024

We request that, prior to submitting a manuscript, interested authors initially submit a proposed title and an abstract of 400–600 words summarizing their intended contribution. Please send it to the Languages Editorial Office ([email protected]). Abstracts will be reviewed by the guest editors for the purposes of ensuring a proper fit within the scope of this Special Issue. Full manuscripts will undergo a double-blind peer review.

References

Comrie, Bernard 1989. Language Universals and Linguistic Typology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Ewing, Michael 2005. Grammar and Inference in Conversation: Identifying Clause. Amsterdaqm: Benjamins.

Ewing, Michael 2019. The Predicate as a locus of grammar and interaction in colloquial Indonesian. Studies in Language 43 (2).

Fillmore, Charles J. 1974. Pragmatics and the description of discourse. In Siegfried J. Schmidt (ed), Pragmatics II, 83–104. Munich: Fink.

Haspelmath, Martin 2022. Nonverbal clause constructions. Ms. https://www.academia.edu/71539642/Nonverbal_clause_constructions.

Helasvuo, Marja-Liisa 2001. Syntax in the Making. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

Laury, Ritva, Tsuyoshi Ono & Ryoko Suzuki 2019. Questioning the Clause as a Crosslinguistic Unit in Grammar and Interaction. Studies in Language 43 (2) 364-401.

Linell. Per 2005. The Written Language Bias in Linguistics. London: Routledge.

Ono, Tsuyoshi and Sandra A. Thompson 1997. Deconstructing “Zero Anaphora” in Japanese. BLS 23, 481-491.

Ono, Tsuyoshi, Misumi Sadler and Saori Daiju 2020. Nihongo no nichijookaiwa ni okeru 'kihonteki' tadooshisetsu ['Canonical' transitive clauses in Japanese everyday conversation]. Otani, N, & Nakayama, T (Eds.). Ninchi Gengokaku to Danwa Kinoo Genogaku no Setten [Interface between Cognitive Linguistics and Discourse Functional Linguistics], 165-182. Tokyo: Hituzi.

Payne, Thomas 1997. Describing Morphosyntax. A Guide for Field Linguists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Schegloff, Emanuel A. 1996. Turn organization: one direction for inquiry into grammar and interaction. In Elinor Ochs, Emanuel Schegloff, and Sandra A. Thompson (eds.), Interaction and Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Thompson, Sandra A. 2019. Understanding ‘clause’ as an emergent ‘unit’ in everyday conversation. Studies in Language 43 (2) 254–280.

Thompson, Sandra and Paul Hopper 2001. Transitivity, clause structure, and argument structure: Evidence from Conversation. In Joan Bybee & Paul Hopper (eds.) Frequency and the Emergence of Linguistic Structure. 27-60. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

Turk, Monica 2000. Word Order in Russian Conversation: A Quantitative Study. MA Thesis, UC Santa Barbara.

Yule, George 2006. The Study of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Prof. Dr. Ritva Laury
Prof. Dr. Tsuyoshi Ono
Guest Editors

Manuscript Submission Information

Manuscripts should be submitted online at www.mdpi.com by registering and logging in to this website. Once you are registered, click here to go to the submission form. Manuscripts can be submitted until the deadline. All submissions that pass pre-check are peer-reviewed. Accepted papers will be published continuously in the journal (as soon as accepted) and will be listed together on the special issue website. Research articles, review articles as well as short communications are invited. For planned papers, a title and short abstract (about 100 words) can be sent to the Editorial Office for announcement on this website.

Submitted manuscripts should not have been published previously, nor be under consideration for publication elsewhere (except conference proceedings papers). All manuscripts are thoroughly refereed through a double-blind peer-review process. A guide for authors and other relevant information for submission of manuscripts is available on the Instructions for Authors page. Languages is an international peer-reviewed open access monthly journal published by MDPI.

Please visit the Instructions for Authors page before submitting a manuscript. The Article Processing Charge (APC) for publication in this open access journal is 1400 CHF (Swiss Francs). Submitted papers should be well formatted and use good English. Authors may use MDPI's English editing service prior to publication or during author revisions.

Keywords

  • clause
  • conversation
  • (a)typicality
  • overt mention
  • predicate

Benefits of Publishing in a Special Issue

  • Ease of navigation: Grouping papers by topic helps scholars navigate broad scope journals more efficiently.
  • Greater discoverability: Special Issues support the reach and impact of scientific research. Articles in Special Issues are more discoverable and cited more frequently.
  • Expansion of research network: Special Issues facilitate connections among authors, fostering scientific collaborations.
  • External promotion: Articles in Special Issues are often promoted through the journal's social media, increasing their visibility.
  • e-Book format: Special Issues with more than 10 articles can be published as dedicated e-books, ensuring wide and rapid dissemination.

Further information on MDPI's Special Issue polices can be found here.

Published Papers (4 papers)

Order results
Result details
Select all
Export citation of selected articles as:

Research

20 pages, 271 KiB  
Article
The Atypicality of Predicates with Two Explicit Arguments in Indonesian Conversation
by Michael C. Ewing
Languages 2025, 10(2), 28; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10020028 - 1 Feb 2025
Viewed by 256
Abstract
While transitive clauses with a subject and object have long been a fundamental focus of grammatical analyses across languages of the world, more recently, it has become apparent that naturally occurring language-in-use is in fact overwhelmingly intransitive and transitive clauses with two arguments [...] Read more.
While transitive clauses with a subject and object have long been a fundamental focus of grammatical analyses across languages of the world, more recently, it has become apparent that naturally occurring language-in-use is in fact overwhelmingly intransitive and transitive clauses with two arguments have a relatively low frequency. In this study, I examine conversational Indonesian and focus on one construction type, a transitive predicate with two explicit core arguments. This grammatical configuration is considered atypical due to its very low frequency in conversational interaction. The goal of the study is to begin to understand when and why expressions of this type appear. It is found that these atypical configurations regularly occur at points where there is a change in footing, including changes in topic, participation framework, or referentiality. It is further shown that the contrast between explicit and unexpressed arguments in Indonesian conversational grammar contributes to the reasons why predicates elaborated with two arguments tend to appear when there is a change in footing. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue (A)typical Clauses across Languages)
19 pages, 536 KiB  
Article
Analysis of Three Complement-Taking Predicates in Spoken Turkish: bil, san, and zannet
by Deniz Zeyrek
Languages 2025, 10(2), 27; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10020027 - 29 Jan 2025
Viewed by 412
Abstract
This paper presents a corpus-based synchronic investigation of three Turkish mental–cognitive verbs in the present tense: san ‘guess, believe’ and zannet ‘suppose, assume’ (in the first person) and bil ‘know’ (in the second person), in spoken discourse. These verbs, known as complement-taking predicates [...] Read more.
This paper presents a corpus-based synchronic investigation of three Turkish mental–cognitive verbs in the present tense: san ‘guess, believe’ and zannet ‘suppose, assume’ (in the first person) and bil ‘know’ (in the second person), in spoken discourse. These verbs, known as complement-taking predicates (CTPs), tend to appear without clausal complements in discourse and function as idiomatic units conveying epistemic modality. This paper characterizes the distribution of occurrences of the three predicates based on frequency to determine the extent to which usage frequency has led to routinization and greater autonomy for the CTPs under investigation, showing that the clause-medial variants deserve attention, as they clearly demonstrate the parenthetical behavior, autonomy, capacity to fulfill pragmatic functions, and projective force of foreshadowing new information of the predicates. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue (A)typical Clauses across Languages)
Show Figures

Figure 1

17 pages, 491 KiB  
Article
The Structure and Functioning of Clauses in Upper Kuskokwim Conversational Discourse
by Andrej A. Kibrik
Languages 2025, 10(2), 26; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10020026 - 29 Jan 2025
Viewed by 357
Abstract
Upper Kuskokwim (Athabaskan, Alaska) is a polysynthetic language with morphologically complex verbs involving pronominal affixes denoting clause arguments. One goal of this paper is to see how clauses in this kind of language are organized and operate in conversational discourse. This study is [...] Read more.
Upper Kuskokwim (Athabaskan, Alaska) is a polysynthetic language with morphologically complex verbs involving pronominal affixes denoting clause arguments. One goal of this paper is to see how clauses in this kind of language are organized and operate in conversational discourse. This study is based on a dataset of transcribed conversations, arranged as sequences of elementary discourse units. The issues explored in this article include the structure of clauses, their functioning in discourse, the composition and expression of clause arguments and other participants, as well as an assessment of more and less typical clauses. I find that clauses are strongly aligned with elementary discourse units; that there is a preference for verb-centered, independent, and one-place clauses; and that lexically expressed arguments are rare. Overall, the clause is a viable notion for the description of Upper Kuskokwim conversational discourse. The specifics of clause structure and clause functioning in Upper Kuskokwim can be explained by a combination of general principles of discourse production and the typological features of the language. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue (A)typical Clauses across Languages)
19 pages, 298 KiB  
Article
Low Transitive Constructions as Typical Clauses in English: A Case Study of the Functions of Clauses with the Nonverbal Predicate be in Stance Displays
by Patricia Mayes
Languages 2024, 9(12), 372; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9120372 - 6 Dec 2024
Viewed by 650
Abstract
Low transitive constructions are ubiquitous in English conversation and as such can be considered “typical” clauses. This article furthers this claim by showing that these constructions are also most frequent in a different genre: arguments between participants at organized protests. It has been [...] Read more.
Low transitive constructions are ubiquitous in English conversation and as such can be considered “typical” clauses. This article furthers this claim by showing that these constructions are also most frequent in a different genre: arguments between participants at organized protests. It has been argued that one reason these constructions are so frequent is that they function to display participants’ stances. Arguments are a type of interaction where stance displays abound. In fact, they are defined as sequences of utterances that display opposing stances. Thus, the study goes on to examine how the most frequent of the low transitive constructions in the data—clauses with the nonverbal predicate be—function to display opposition across utterances. Du Bois has analyzed stance as resonance across utterances, created from structural parallelism, and he argues that slight changes between linguistic forms can create differing focal points that index contrasts. This framework is used to analyze how participants use the multiple semantic functions of be clauses across interactional sequences to display and modify their stances in response to their opponent. It is suggested that the versatility and ambiguity of be clauses are especially useful in arguments where participants do not share a set of common beliefs. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue (A)typical Clauses across Languages)
Back to TopTop