Next Article in Journal
Long-Term Whole Grain Wheat and Rye Intake Reflected by Adipose Tissue Alkylresorcinols and Breast Cancer: A Case-Cohort Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Intraintestinal Delivery of Tastants Using a Naso-Duodenal-Ileal Catheter Does Not Influence Food Intake or Satiety
Previous Article in Journal
Effects of Pharmacological Thermogenic Adipocyte Activation on Metabolism and Atherosclerotic Plaque Regression
Previous Article in Special Issue
Human Tongue Electrophysiological Response to Oleic Acid and Its Associations with PROP Taster Status and the CD36 Polymorphism (rs1761667)
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Varying the Color, Aroma, Bitter, and Sweet Levels of a Grapefruit-Like Model Beverage on the Sensory Properties and Liking of the Consumer

by
Andries G. S. Gous
1,
Valérie L. Almli
2,
Vinet Coetzee
3 and
Henrietta L. de Kock
1,*
1
Department of Consumer and Food Sciences, University of Pretoria, P O Box X20, Hatfield, Pretoria 0028, South Africa
2
Nofima AS, P O Box 210, 1431 Ås, Norway
3
Department of Genetics, University of Pretoria, P O Box X20, Hatfield, Pretoria 0028, South Africa
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2019, 11(2), 464; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11020464
Submission received: 14 December 2018 / Revised: 10 February 2019 / Accepted: 11 February 2019 / Published: 22 February 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Taste, Nutrition and Health)

Abstract

:
Color, aroma, sweet, and bitter tastes contribute to the sensory perception of grapefruit juice. Consumers differ about liking grapefruit. A reason is the bitter taste that characterize the fruit. The objective was to determine the effect of varying the color (red or yellow), aroma (two levels), bitterness (three levels), and sweetness (three levels) of a grapefruit-like model beverage, on consumers’ liking and perception of its sensory properties. The sensory profiles of thirty-six grapefruit-like beverages, created on the basis of a factorial design, has been described. Consumers rated their liking of color, aroma, and flavor of the twelve most diverse beverages. Bitter and sweet levels of the beverages had a significant effect on the flavor and aftertaste attributes. Aroma concentration had a significant effect on the majority of the sensory attributes. Color had a significant effect on perception of some of the aroma attributes, as well as the grapefruit’s flavor intensity. Consumers liked the red beverages more than the yellow ones, and those with low aroma over the high aroma intensity. Consumers preferred the low bitter/high sweet beverages. Pungent and grapefruit aroma were found to be negative drivers for liking of the aroma. Sweet and citrus flavors were found to be positive drivers and sour and bitter flavors were found to be negative drivers of flavor-preferences (or liking) of the tested beverages.

1. Introduction

The sensory properties of grapefruit (Citrus X paradisi) are distinctive characterizing components and play a key role in reasons why consumers choose or not choose to consume the fruit and its products, e.g., juice. Grapefruit is a rich source of vitamin C, and health-promoting citrus flavonoids and limonoids and has beneficial antioxidant and anti-inflammatory properties [1,2]. Its appearance, aroma, flavor, and mouthfeel properties contribute to the sensory perception of the fruit.
Consumers differ widely in opinions on liking or disliking grapefruit and part of this individual preference is attributed to liking or disliking of the bitter taste that characterizes the fruit. Excessive bitterness of the juice was considered to be an important problem in commercial grapefruit juice production [3]. Naringin and limonin are mainly responsible for the bitter taste commonly associated with grapefruit [4]. The consumption of fresh grapefruit, and grapefruit products has been declining [5] and plant breeders are working on ways to select for desirable sensory traits. A better understanding of the impact of the different sensory modalities contributing to the sensory perception of grapefruit products (e.g., juice) might assist product developers to optimize formulations and improve uptake of the products among consumers, thereby, maintaining or enhancing profitability for the role-players, along the grapefruit value chain.
Flavor perception is complex, due to the simultaneous stimulation of a number of senses. It is the result of processes that respond to sensory signals, from the activation of multiple sensory modalities, including smell (retronasal olfaction), mouthfeel (somatosensation), as well as taste (gustation), and to some extent also sight. When different senses are stimulated, concurrently, and perceptually interact with each other, the perceived flavor is the result of the cross-modal sensory interaction [6]. Cross-modal interactions can change the intensity and perceived character of individual tastes and aromas, and even the overall flavor [7].
The present study aimed broadly, to determine the relations between the stimulus components of a model beverage (formulated to be similar to grapefruit juice) and their effects on the perceived sensory properties and hedonic responses. We factorially combined, in the same acidified neutral base, each of three possible levels of bitter naringin (low, medium, and high) with each of three levels of sweet sucrose (low, medium, and high), two levels of grapefruit aroma (low and high) and two color variants (red and yellow). We hypothesized that perceived bitterness of the model grapefruit-like beverage will drive consumers dislike for the beverage but that bitterness perception will be a function of cross-modal color–taste, aroma–taste, and sweet–bitter taste interactions.
The color of the natural juice extracted from the grapefruit depends on the variety used and ranges from greenish-yellow to pale yellow, pink, and light red [8]. We hypothesized that a rose red grapefruit-like beverage would be perceived as sweeter than a pale yellow option. Previously, it was reported [9] that a red color decreased the perception of bitter taste intensity of a caffeine + water solution, with the yellow and green color having had no effect. The color of food and drinks impacts subsequent perception of taste, flavor, and overall sensory perception. It has been reported in several studies that the color of a solution greatly impacts the ability to identify its flavor and also affects the liking responses [7].
We hypothesized that a beverage with high, compared to a low grapefruit aroma, would suppress the bitter taste perception and enhance the taste of sweetness [10]. A new study [10] reported that lemon extract, sucrose, and citric acid, when presented separately and also together, affected the perception of sweet, sour, and citrus flavors. The aroma of the products can influence the perception of basic tastes and vice versa [11,12,13,14,15].
It is well-known that sucrose and other sweet tasting compounds can suppress bitterness. This is practically applied when bitter tasting coffee or tea is sweetened with sugar. Here the expectation was that sweetness would suppress bitterness but an enhancement effect on volatile aroma and flavor compounds was also expected. When sucrose was added to the fruit juices, not only were the perceived level of bitterness and sourness reduced and the sweet taste intensity increased, but the sweet aroma intensity rating also changed [16].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Preparation of the Grapefruit-Like Beverages

Thirty-six grapefruit-like beverages (Table 1) were manufactured, following a factorial design with deflavored, clarified, deionized, and acidified apple juice, as base, with an addition of naringin (three bitter levels), sucrose (three sweet levels), a grapefruit aroma compound mixture (two intensity levels) consisting of caryophyllene, citral, nootkatone, aldehyde C8 (octanal), aldehyde C9 (nonanal, aldehyde C10 (decanal), and two colorants (red or yellow). The addition of naringin was intended to reflect a low-level, in-between, and a high-level, based on the typical content in the grapefruit juice (218–340 mg/kg) [17]. The low level of sweetness was based on the industry minimum requirement for export purposes, with incrementally higher levels added to reflect medium and high sweetness. These aroma compound mixture and levels used were selected in consultation with a flavorant supplier. The typical grapefruit juice color was copied using artificial colorants. The red color was a 0.001% solution blend of 30% sunset yellow and 70% ponceau red. The yellow color consisted of 0.0125% quinoline yellow. Standard preparation and mixing procedures were used for all added stimuli to ensure uniformity. The grapefruit-like beverages were filled in 250 mL plastic bottles, with lids, for easy handling and uniformity, and were kept frozen at −18 °C, until use. The beverages were defrosted overnight at an ambient temperature and kept at 14 °C, until served. A summary of the physico-chemical characterization of the 36 grapefruit-like beverages is presented in the Supplementary Material (Table S1).

2.2. Descriptive Sensory Analysis

The sensory profiles of the beverages were described by a sixteen-member trained sensory panel with one to two years of descriptive sensory analysis experience. The specific training for attribute and methodology development for the evaluation of the beverages consisted of two sessions of 2 h each, using the generic descriptive analysis method [18]. A total of 21 attributes were generated to characterize the aroma, flavor, and aftertaste of the grapefruit-like beverages (Table 2). Beverage samples (±30 mL) were served at ±14 °C, in 125 ml polystyrene cups with plastic lids, and marked with randomly selected three-digit numbers. Samples were evaluated in duplicates, 12 beverages per 2 h session per day and a total of six sessions. The presentation order of samples per day for the different panelists followed a Williams Latin square design. Reference standards were available during training and evaluation sessions.
Panel performance was monitored to test reproducibility and consistency of the panel ratings using PanelCheck 1.3.2 (www.panelcheck.com; Nofima, Ås, Norway).
The attributes were evaluated on a structured horizontal line scale (10 cm) with descriptors at the scale ends ranging from ‘not intense’ (at the left end of the scale, 0 cm) to ‘very intense’ (at the right end of the scale, 10 cm). Data was captured using Compusense® five release 4.6 software (Compusense Inc., Guelph, ON, Canada).

2.3. Consumer Evaluation

Ninety six young South African female consumers aged 18–24 years were recruited by trained fieldworkers. Each consumer completed an online screening survey and were invited to participate if in a self-reported good state of health, and if not limited by any food intolerance(s) and/or allergies. Participants were briefed and gave written consent before evaluating the beverages. Participants were requested not to eat, drink (except for water) or smoke for at least 1 h prior to the session.
The consumers (n = 90) evaluated liking of the color, aroma and flavor of the 12 most diverse beverages (selected on the basis of composition) (Table 1) using the Simplified Labeled Affective Magnitude (SLAM) scale [19], a 10 cm line scale labelled with descriptors ‘greatest imaginable dislike’ (at 0 cm), and ‘greatest imaginable like’ (at 10 cm). Sample preparation and presentation was the same as for the trained panel. The 12 samples were evaluated in one session and the order of presentation to different consumers followed a Williams design.
Data was captured using Compusense® five release 4.6 software (Compusense Inc., Guelph, ON, Canada).
Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the Faculty of Natural and Agricultural Sciences Ethics Committee at the University of Pretoria (EC 130827-088).

2.4. Statistical Analysis

An analysis of variance (ANOVA) model fitted using PROC GLM in SAS v9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) was used to determine the main effects of the panelists, the bitter level, the sweet level, the aroma level, and the color type, together with the respective two-way interactions on the sensory attributes of the beverages. Tukey’s HSD test (p = 0.05) was used to compare beverages that differed in an attribute. Principal component analysis (PCA) using XLSTAT 2014 (Addinsoft, Paris, France) was applied to the correlation matrix of the sensory panel mean ratings, for all attributes of all grapefruit-like beverages.
Consumer liking of the color, aroma, and flavor of the 12 most diverse beverages was analyzed by a three-way ANOVA model, including the effects of color, aroma level, and tastants (bitter and sweet levels in three combination). Means were compared using Fisher’s least significant difference test at p < 0.05. Data were analyzed using GenStat® (VSN International Ltd., Hertfordshire, UK). Consumer liking ratings (y) for color, aroma, and flavor of beverages were modeled as a function of the descriptive sensory attributes (x), using three separate partial least squares (PLS) regression models. Preliminary models were run with all sensory attributes and their squared terms. Variable importance (VIP), which measures how important a variable is in terms of modeling the liking attributes, was used to select a smaller number of linear and squared terms for the final model. The VIP values summarize the overall contribution of each X-variable to the PLS model, summed over all components, and weighted according to the Y variation, accounted for by each component. Only those linear terms with a VIP greater than 0.8, as well as the five squared terms with the highest contribution, were retained. The PLS models were used to determine the positive and negative drivers of color, aroma, and flavor liking, and also to predict consumer liking of the 24 samples that were profiled by the descriptive sensory panel, but not evaluated by the consumers. The SIMCA-P package (Umetrics, Umea, Sweden) was used for the PLS modeling.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Sensory Profiles of the Grapefruit-Like Beverages

Table 3 presents a summary of the main effects (color, aroma, bitter, and sweet) and two-way interaction ANOVA effects (provided in Supplementary Tables S2, S3 and S4) on sensory attributes of grapefruit-like beverages, as evaluated by the trained sensory panel. Means for each of the samples represent the average of duplicate ratings by 16 panelists. Color of the grapefruit-like beverages had a significant effect on perception of some aroma and flavor properties. The overall aroma and grapefruit, deteriorated/rotten, muddy/mouldy, fruity and sweet aroma, and grapefruit flavor of the red colored beverages were perceived as significantly (p < 0.05) more intense than the yellow colored beverages.
The level of aroma added had a significant effect on the majority of the sensory attributes, namely, overall aroma intensity and citrus, grapefruit, chemical, muddy/moldy, fruity, green/grassy, peely/peel oil, soapy, pungent, woody/spicy, and sweet aroma, with the lowest intensities perceived in the beverages with the low aroma level added. Aroma level had a significant effect on the bitter, astringent, and citrus flavor, and the bitter aftertaste perception, with the highest bitter and astringent flavor and bitter aftertaste being perceived in the beverages with a low aroma level and the highest citrus flavor being perceived in the beverages with a high aroma level.
Varying the naringin content (bitter level) of the beverages did not have any significant effect on any of the aroma attributes. It did, however, have a significant effect on the intensities of overall flavor and the astringent flavor, with the highest values observed for beverages with medium and high naringin concentrations. The naringin level had a significant effect on the intensities of sweet, sour, bitter, and grapefruit flavor, and the bitter aftertaste perception. The highest sweetness, but lowest sourness and grapefruit flavors were perceived in the beverages with low and medium naringin concentrations. Intensity of bitter flavor and bitter aftertaste followed the level of bitter compound addition.
Sweetness level contributed by sucrose had a significant effect on the perception of the many sensory properties of the grapefruit-like beverages. Significantly higher soapy aroma was perceived in the beverages with low and medium levels of sucrose, compared to a high sucrose addition. Sucrose level in the beverages had a significant effect on sour, sweet, bitter, astringent, and grapefruit flavor, and the bitter aftertaste intensities. Sour, bitter, astringent, and bitter aftertaste intensities decreased as the sweet level increased, while sweetness increased. A less intense grapefruit flavor was perceived in the high sweet level beverages, compared to the low and medium sweet levels.
Very few two-way interactions were significant. The detailed tables for the significant interaction effects are presented in the Supplementary Material (Tables S2–S4). The bitter level x aroma level interaction effect (Table S2) was significant for the perception of the intensity of chemical aroma and overall flavor intensity, the bitter flavor, and the bitter aftertaste. A trend was observed in that the chemical aroma was more intensely perceived in the beverages with high aroma, although only significantly so in the low and high bitter samples and not in the medium bitter samples. The overall flavor intensity was significantly but slightly lower in the high aroma/medium bitter, compared to the low aroma/medium bitter sample. Aroma level did not affect the overall flavor perception at the low or high bitter levels. Bitter flavor and bitter aftertaste were notably less intense in the high aroma samples, compared to the low aroma samples, but only significantly so for the medium and high bitter level beverages.
The bitter level x color type interaction effect (Table S2) was significant for the bitter aftertaste intensity. However, bitter aftertaste was essentially driven more by the bitter level than the color type. The bitter level x sweet level interaction effect (Table S3) was significant only for the pungent aroma. A significantly lower pungent aroma was noted between the medium sweet and low sweet beverages, at the medium bitter level.
The aroma level x color interaction effect (Table S3) was significant only for bitter flavor intensity. At a low aroma level, no difference in bitter flavor intensity was found between the two colors. However, at the high aroma level, the yellow beverage was perceived as being significantly bitterer.
The sweet level x aroma level interaction effect (Table S4) was not significant for any of the sensory aroma attributes. A sweet level x color interaction effect (Table S4) was significant for the astringent and citrus flavor perception. While no significant differences were found between the red and yellow beverages at the medium sweet level, the red beverage was perceived as significantly more astringent at low sweet and high sweet levels. A similar effect was found for the citrus flavor, although the red beverages were found to have a more intense citrus flavor, only at the low sweet level.
The multivariate differentiation of the beverages is presented in Figure 1 as a PCA map over a two-dimensional space. The first and second principal components (F1 and F2) explained 37% and 35%, respectively, of the variance across the samples. F1 clearly separated beverages based on intensity of overall aroma, peely/peel oil aroma, citrus aroma, sweet aroma, and pungent aroma. Beverages that were more intense in terms of the mentioned attributes are located on the right of the plot. Note that all of these beverages have an H as third letter, therefore, they have a high aroma level. The beverages with lower intensities are located on the left of the plot and notably has L as the third letter, therefore, with a low level aroma. F2 separated the beverages based on ‘taste’ perception, i.e., naringin (bitter)-sucrose (sweet) levels. Beverages with high and medium bitter levels and low sweet are positioned at the top, and beverages with low bitter level and medium and high sweet levels, are at the bottom. Beverages at the top, namely HLLY and HLLR with a high level of naringin and MLHY with a medium level, were characterized by more intense astringency, sour, and bitter tastes, and with grapefruit and overall flavor intensities. Beverages (e.g., LHHR) with a low naringin level (at the bottom), were characterized by a more intense sweet taste. The attributes citrus flavor, chemical aroma, and muddy/moldy aroma in the middle of the plot, did not discriminate beverages on the first two PCs.

3.2. Consumer Evaluation of the Grapefruit-Like Beverages

The effects of color, aroma level, and bitter/sweet levels of the grapefruit-like beverages on mean liking ratings for the color, aroma, and flavor, as evaluated by the consumers, are presented in Table 4. Two-way interaction effects were not significant.
The standardized PLS regression coefficients for attributes as part of the prediction models are presented in Table 5. PLS regression (PLSR) models were used to predict liking of the color, aroma, and flavor of the 36 beverages, including the beverages that were not evaluated by consumers (Table 6). Expected errors of prediction for the models were low, lying between ±1.288 for the aroma model to ±2.458 for the color model, and ±2.678 for the flavor model, with a 95% confidence interval, indicating reliable prediction estimations of the liking variables.
Liking of the color of the red grapefruit-like beverages were rated, on average, slightly higher than the yellow ones (p < 0.05) (Table 4). Whether the beverage was colored yellow or red, it did not affect the liking of the aroma or the flavor. Predicted mean liking of the color for the highest and lowest liked of the 36 beverages differed, however, only by a maximum of 12.2 scale units (Table 6). Notably the research found no significant sensory attribute drivers for liking of the color of the grapefruit-like beverages (Table 5).
Liking of the aroma of beverages with a low added-aroma level, was higher (p < 0.05) than for those with a high added-aroma level (Table 4). Aroma level did not have an effect on the liking of the color of the beverage. Aroma level also did not affect the liking of the flavor of the beverage. The predicted mean liking of the aroma for the highest and the lowest liked beverages, differed by 16.5 scale units (Table 6). Main effects and squared effects are indicated as ‘2’. Positive attribute drivers for liking of the aroma of the grapefruit-like beverages were the square term of fruity aroma (noted fruity aroma2), citrus flavor, and sweet flavor, while negative drivers were sweet aroma 2, sweet flavor2, and pungent aroma (Table 5).
As expected, the level of the gustatory flavorants, the naringin, and the sucrose, did not affect the liking of the color of the beverages (Table 4). Surprisingly the non-volatile taste level did have a significant effect (p < 0.05) on the liking of the aroma of the beverages. The aroma of the most bitter/least sweet beverages was liked significantly less than the other two taste combination levels. Not surprisingly, liking of the flavor of the beverages decreased significantly (p < 001) as the bitter level increased and the sweet level decreased. Predicted mean ratings for liking of the flavor, the highest liked and the lowest liked of the 36 beverages, differed by 27.5 scale units. Positive drivers for liking of the flavor of the grapefruit-like beverages were sweet taste, squared term for chemical aroma (noted as ‘chemical aroma2), and citrus flavor intensities, while the negative drivers were intensity of soapy aroma, bitter aftertaste, and sour taste (Table 6).

4. Discussion

The research studied the effect of varying the bitterness, sweetness, color, and aroma intensity of grapefruit-like beverages on the cross-modal perception of sensory properties and its effects on consumer liking. A model grapefruit-like beverage standard formulation was created and a sensory lexicon with a total of 21 attributes and definitions were generated to characterize the aroma, the flavor, and the aftertaste of the grapefruit-like model beverage with variations in color, aroma, and gustatory flavorant levels.
Color hue of the grapefruit-like beverage affected the perception and description of the aroma and flavor sensory properties, as evaluated by the trained human panelists. Color of the beverages, and, in particular, the sample with the rose-red hue had a significant enhancing effect on perception of overall aroma intensity and grapefruit, deteriorated/rotten, muddy/moldy, fruity, and sweet aroma intensities. It also corresponded to the consumer liking—the red beverages were liked more than the yellow ones. The cross modal effect of the beverage color on aroma and flavor of the beverages, however, did not lead to significant differences in the liking of aroma or a liking of the flavor of the red and yellow beverages. The difference in methodology followed and the cognitive tasks employed by the two groups of panels might be the reason. When the group of consumers evaluated the liking of the color of the beverages, solely based on appearance, a slight but significant preference for the red-colored beverages was noted. This preference was solely driven by visual cues, since the consumers did not yet smell or taste the beverages. After smelling and tasting the beverages, it is likely that the opinion and preference might have changed, based on the cross-modal, color-aroma/flavor sensory interaction, as demonstrated by the results for the trained panel in this study. Considering that the consumers first evaluated the liking of the color, then the aroma (retronasally), and lastly the flavor (after consumption) of each sample, sequentially, it cannot be excluded that some form of learning, anticipation, and association might have occurred over the evaluation of the sequence of twelve samples, of which 50% were red and 50% were yellow.
A study [20] reported that the red color decreased the perception of the bitter taste sensitivity of a water solution. Coloring a clear bitter solution red, decreased the perception of the bitter taste, while the addition of yellow and green coloring had no such effect [9]. Other researchers [21] suggested that color-induced olfactory enhancement observed when odorous solutions are smelled orthonasally, might be the result of a conditioned olfactory percept caused by the color. Conditioned expectations predict that certain colors would be strongly associated with particular flavors, e.g., red with cherry, orange with orange, and green with lime [22]; yellow with lemon, blue with spearmint, and red with strawberry, raspberry, and cherry [23]. In South Africa, the location for the study, both yellow and red/pink grapefruit are marketed. The Star Ruby variety with a red color is the most planted (84%) grapefruit variety in South Africa, followed by the white variety Marsh (16%) (the juice of this type of grapes is pale yellow) [24]. In another study [25] it was found that the relationship of green and yellow colors in the lemon and lime-flavored sucrose solutions was altered; such color changes were found to have an impact on the perceived sweetness ratings. In another study, results showed that color–odor solution pairings were rated as having more intense odors with color cues than without, regardless of the color–odor pairing appropriateness [21]. This cross-modal effect presumably results from the color-cue setting up an expectation concerning the likely identity and intensity of a food or drink’s taste or flavor [20]. No significant sensory attribute drivers for liking of the color of the grapefruit-like beverages was identified, since the trained sensory panel did not evaluate the appearance attributes.
Aroma level added to the model beverage had a significant enhancing effect, on the majority of the aroma and flavor sensory attributes. The enhancement of overall aroma and characteristic aroma qualities, including citrus flavor, as a function of the level of aroma added, was expected and confirmed. When consumers evaluated liking of the aroma of the beverages, solely based on orthonasal inspection, surprisingly the beverages with low aroma were slightly preferred over those with high aroma. It is possible that the higher aroma level was more distinctive and clearly reminiscent of grapefruit and possibly evoked a stronger cue for those disliking grapefruit. An interesting and unexpected finding was the apparent suppression of bitter and astringent gustatory sensations, due to a higher load of olfactory stimuli (high aroma level). Previous studies have found that aroma–taste interactions can result in complicated changes in the perceived flavor. The addition of an aroma can, e.g., elevate the bitter-detection threshold [26,27]. The perceived intensity of tastes in solutions was increased by volatile compounds, especially when there was a logical association between them, such as between sweetness and fruitiness [28]. Apple and strawberry aromas evoked both sweetness and sourness. A study found that tasteless aromas, namely green tea and coffee, predominantly evoked bitterness, while the vanilla aroma predominantly evoked sweetness [29]. The grapefruit aroma consisted of a blend of caryophyllene, citral, nootkatone, and various aldehydes; octanal, nonanal, and decanal. No study could be found that specifically indicated that any of these compounds evoked bitterness. Nootkatone at the above threshold concentrations was reported as tasting bitter [30]. Consumption of a beverage results in the simultaneous perception of aroma and taste, coupled with tactile sensations, all of which contribute to an overall impression of flavor. Compounds that stimulate taste perception (e.g., naringin contributing a bitter taste) can increase the apparent intensity of aromas. In this study, the grapefruit flavor was enhanced by the naringin addition. The aroma compound (containing a citral component) of the grapefruit-like beverages had an enhancing effect on the citrus aroma intensity. An additive effect of the sweet components with citral or limonene volatiles having a ‘citrus’-like aroma was reported by [31] but was not observed in this study. The suppression of bitterness in the high aroma beverages, however, did not affect the liking of the flavor, since there was no difference found in the liking of the flavor of beverages with low or high aroma levels. Positive drivers for liking of the aroma of the grapefruit-like beverages were fruity aroma2, citrus flavor, and sweet flavor, while negative drivers were sweet aroma2, sweet flavor2, and pungent aroma.
The low bitter/high sweet beverages were preferred over the high bitter/low sweet samples. A study [32] reported that with an increase in the ratio of °Brix/acidity of reconstituted grapefruit juice, the consumer perception of sweetness increased and bitterness and aroma intensity decreased. Some bitterness in processed grapefruit products is acceptable for consumers, but excessive bitterness is one of the major consumer objections to such products [28,31]; this was confirmed in this study. The variation in sensitivity of the individual consumers to bitter compounds in grapefruit beverages could be explored further to identify whether subgroups might have different preferences. As expected, the contribution of varying concentrations of naringin affecting the bitterness of the grapefruit-like beverages did not have a significant effect on any of the aroma attributes. Similarly, [32] reported that consumers did not find any difference in aroma with increased levels of naringin in processed grapefruit juice. However, the concentration of bitterness of the grapefruit-like beverages had a significant effect on the flavor attributes (astringent, sweet, sour, bitter and grapefruit flavor, and the bitter aftertaste). A study [32] has also reported that an increase of limonin (also a bitter compound) in processed grapefruit juice, increased the perceived bitterness and tartness, while decreasing the sweetness.
In a previous study, an increase in the °Brix with sucrose, enhanced the taste of sweetness, and had a decreasing effect on the sour, bitter, astringent, and grapefruit flavors, and the bitter aftertaste. When sucrose was added to fruit juices, not only were the perceived levels of bitterness and sourness reduced (as was also found in this research) but the sweet aroma intensity rating also changed [16] (although this was not found here). Sucrose was also reported to mask the bitter taste of sinigrin, goitrin, and quinine [33]. In the complex beverage model, increasing sucrose did not have the often reported enhanced effect on the perceived fruity aroma. Increasing the sugar concentration of blueberry and cranberry fruit juices, increased their fruitiness (evaluated by sipping), even though no difference in aroma was perceived by sniffing alone [16]. Sucrose in the mouth significantly enhanced the “citrus” ratings, compared to when citral was inhaled alone [12]. Similarly, increases in the intensity of different ‘fruity’ aromas were perceived in a multichannel flavor delivery system [34], model dairy desserts [35], and custard desserts [36], when increasing the sweetness with sucrose. Sweet level also affected the soapy aroma of the grapefruit-like beverages. The reason for the effect on soapy aroma is unclear. It is possible that the aroma blend contributed a slight soapy aroma.
The effect of aroma level and color on the perceived sensory attributes, as observed in this study, are evidence of cross-modal sensory interactions. It was anticipated that the intensity and character of the aroma level of a grapefruit juice would increase the perception of the citrus flavor, a positive driver of grapefruit flavor liking and reduce the negative attributes, the bitter and astringent flavor, as well as the bitter aftertaste. Positive drivers for liking of the flavor of grapefruit-like beverages were the sweet taste, the chemical aroma, and the citrus flavor intensities, while negative drivers were intensity of soapy aroma, bitter aftertaste, and sour taste.

5. Conclusions

This study indicated that aroma, bitterness, and sweetness levels, and also product color (hue) influences the perception of grapefruit-like beverages, as well as their hedonic value. A grapefruit-like beverage model was created and a lexicon to describe the sensory properties of the cross-modal interaction of stimulus components of the model beverage was developed. From the descriptive sensory profiles, prediction models for liking of the color, aroma, and flavor of grapefruit-like beverages were developed. In the next phase, the models should be applied to a wide range of grapefruit juice samples to determine validity and reliability in real juices. The models can then be optimized for application in grapefruit quality control and product development programs.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2072-6643/11/2/464/s1. Table S1: Physico-chemical characterization (means ± standard deviation) of the 36 grapefruit-like beverages. Table S2: Summary of sensory attribute mean values1 [± standard error of means (SEM)] and significance of bitter x aroma and bitter x color two-way ANOVA interactions of the model grapefruit-like beverages as evaluated by a trained sensory panel (n = 16). Table S3: Summary of sensory attribute mean values1 [± standard error of means (SEM)] and significance of bitter x sweet and aroma x color two-way ANOVA interactions of the model grapefruit-like beverages as evaluated by a trained sensory panel (n = 16). Table S4: Summary of sensory attribute mean values1 [± standard error of means (SEM)] and significance of sweet x aroma and sweet x color two-way ANOVA interactions of the model grapefruit-like beverages as evaluated by a trained sensory panel (n = 16).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.G.S.G.; Formal analysis, A.G.S.G. and V.L.A.; Funding acquisition, A.G.S.G. and H.L.d.K.; Investigation, A.G.S.G.; Methodology, A.G.S.G.; Resources, A.G.S.G.; Supervision, V.C. and H.L.d.K.; Writing—original draft, A.G.S.G., V.L.A., V.C., and H.L.d.K.; Writing—review & editing, H.L.d.K.

Funding

This work is based on research supported, in part, by the National Research Foundation of South Africa, Grant Number: 76905. V.L.A. is thankful for funding from the Norwegian Agriculture and Food Industry Research Funds, FoodSMaCK strategic program.

Acknowledgments

The technical and research support of Karien Kotze, Melanie Richards-Dennil, Marise Kinnear, Leandri de Kock is acknowledged.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results. Any opinion, finding, and conclusion or recommendation expressed in this material is that of the author(s) and the NRF does not accept any liability in this regard.

References

  1. Zargar, S.; Al-Majed, A.-R.A.; Wani, T.A. Potentiating and synergistic effect of grapefruit juice on the antioxidant and anti-inflammatory activity of aripiprazole against hydrogen peroxide induced oxidative stress in mice. BMC Complement. Altern. Med. 2018, 18, 106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Cristóbal-Luna, J.M.; Álvarez-González, I.; Madrigal-Bujaidar, E.; Chamorro-Cevallos, G. Grapefruit and its biomedical, antigenotoxic and chemopreventive properties. Food Chem. Toxicol. 2018, 112, 224–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Manlan, M.; Matthews, R.F.; Rouseff, R.L.; Littell, R.C.; Marshall, M.R.; Moye, H.A.; Teixeira, A.A. Evaluation of the properties of Polystyrene Divinylbenzene adsorbents for debittering grapefruit juice. J. Food Sci. 1990, 55, 440–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Ribeiro, I.A.; Ribeiro, M.H.L. Naringin and naringenin determination and control in grapefruit juice by a validated HPLC method. Food Control 2008, 19, 432–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Obenland, D.; Campisi-Pinto, S.; Arpaia, M.L. Determinants of sensory acceptability in grapefruit. Sci. Hortic. 2018, 231, 151–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Delahunty, C.M.; Drake, M.A. Sensory character of cheese and its evaluation. In Cheese: Chemistry, Physics and Microbiology; Fox, P.F., McSweeney, P.L.H., Cogan, T.M., Guinee, T.P., Eds.; Elsevier Academic Press: London, UK, 2004; pp. 517–545. [Google Scholar]
  7. Delwiche, J. The impact of perceptual interactions on perceived flavor. Food Qual. Prefer. 2004, 15, 137–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Rosales, C.K.; Suwonsichon, S. Sensory lexicon of pomelo fruit over various cultivars and fresh-cut storage. J. Sens. Stud. 2015, 30, 21–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Maga, J.A. Influence of color on taste thresholds. Chem. Senses 1974, 1, 115–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Veldhuizen, M.G.; Siddique, A.; Rosenthal, S.; Marks, L.E. Interactions of lemon, sucrose and citric acid in enhancing citrus, sweet and sour flavors. Chem. Senses 2018, 43, 17–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Forde, C.G.; Delahunty, C.M. Understanding the role cross-modal sensory interactions play in food acceptability in younger and older consumers. Food Qual. Prefer. 2004, 15, 715–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Fujimaru, T.; Lim, J. Effects of stimulus intensity on odor enhancement by taste. Chemosens. Percept. 2013, 6, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Green, B.G.; Nachtigal, D.; Hammond, S.; Lim, J. Enhancement of retronasal odors by taste. Chem. Senses 2012, 37, 77–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Lim, J.; Fujimaru, T.; Linscott, T.D. The role of congruency in taste-odor interactions. Food Qual. Prefer. 2014, 34, 5–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Niimi, J.; Eddy, A.I.; Overington, A.R.; Heenan, S.P.; Silcock, P.; Bremer, P.J.; Delahunty, C.M. Aroma-taste interactions between a model cheese aroma and five basic tastes in solution. Food Qual. Prefer. 2014, 31, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Von Sydow, E.; Moskowitz, H.; Jacbs, H.; Meiselman, H. Odor taste interaction in fruit juices. Leb. Wissenchaft Technol. 1974, 7, 9–16. [Google Scholar]
  17. Pichaiyongvongdee, R.; Haruenkit, S. Comparative studies of limonin and naringin distribution in different parts of pummelo [Citrus grandis (L) Osbeck] cultivars grown in Thailand. Kasetsat J. Nat. Sci. 2009, 43, 28–36. [Google Scholar]
  18. Einstein, M.A. Descriptive techniques and their hybridiztion. In Sensory Science: Theory and Application in Food; Lawless, H.T., Klein, B.P., Eds.; Marcel Dekker: New York, NY, USA, 1991; pp. 317–338. [Google Scholar]
  19. Lawless, H.T.; Cardello, A.V.; Chapman, K.W.; Lesher, L.L.; Given, Z.; Schutz, H.G. A comparison of the effectiveness of hedonic scales and end-anchor compression effects. J. Sens. Stud. 2010, 25, 18–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Spence, C.; Levitan, C.A.; Shankar, M.U.; Zampini, M. Does food color influence taste and flavor perception in humans? Chemosens. Percept. 2010, 3, 68–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Zellner, D.A.; Kautz, M.A. Color affects perceived odor intensity. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1990, 16, 391–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. DuBose, C.N.; Cardello, A.V.; Maller, O. Effects of colorants and flavorants on identification, perceived flavor intensity, and hedonic quality of fruit-flavored beverages and cake. J. Food Sci. 1980, 45, 1393–1399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Zampini, M.; Spence, C. Modifying the multisensory perception of a carbonated beverage using auditory cues. Food Qual. Prefer. 2005, 16, 632–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Department of Agriculture, Forestry and Fisheries. A Profile of the South African Citrus Market Value Chain 2015; Department of Agriculture, Forestry and Fisheries: Pretoria, South Africa, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  25. Roth, H.A.; Radle, L.J.; Gifford, S.R.; Clydesdale, F.M. Psychophysical relationships between perceived sweetness and color in lemon- and lime-flavored drinks. J. Food Sci. 1988, 53, 1116–1119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Gaudette, N.J.; Pickering, G.J. Modifying bitterness in functional food systems. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 2013, 53, 464–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Haraguchi, T.; Yoshida, M.; Hazekawa, M.; Uchida, T. Synergistic effects of sour taste and low temperature in suppressing the bitterness of Aminoleban® EN. Chem. Pharm. Bull. (Tokyo) 2011, 59, 536–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Noble, A.C. Taste-aroma interactions. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 1996, 7, 439–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Mukai, J.; Tokuyama, E.; Ishizaka, T.; Okada, S.; Uchida, T. Inhibitory effect of aroma on the bitterness of branched-chain amino acid solutions. Chem. Pharm. Bull. (Tokyo) 2007, 55, 1581–1584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Berry, R.E.; Wagner, C.J., Jr.; Moshonas, M.G. Flavor Studies of Nootkatone in Grapefruit Juice. J. Food Sci. 1967, 32, 75–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hewson, L.; Hollowood, T.; Chandra, S.; Hort, J. Taste–aroma interactions in a citrus flavoured model beverage system: Similarities and differences between acid and sugar type. Food Qual. Prefer. 2008, 19, 323–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Fellers, P.J.; Carter, R.D.; de Jager, G. Influence of Limonin on consumer preference of processed grapefruit juice. J. Food Sci. 1987, 52, 741–743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Beck, T.K.; Jensen, S.; Bjoern, G.K.; Kidmose, U. The masking effect of sucrose on perception of bitter compounds in Brassica vegetables. J. Sens. Stud. 2014, 29, 190–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hort, J.; Hollowood, T.A. Controlled continuous flow delivery system for investigating taste-aroma interactions. J. Agric. Food Chem. 2004, 52, 4834–4843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Lethuaut, L.; Weel, K.G.C.; Boelrijk, A.E.M.; Brossard, C.D. Flavor Perception and Aroma Release from Model Dairy Desserts. J. Agric. Food Chem. 2004, 52, 3478–3485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Tournier, C.; Sulmont-Rossé, C.; Sémon, E.; Vignon, A.; Issanchou, S.; Guichard, E. A study on texture-taste-aroma interactions: Physico-chemical and cognitive mechanisms. Int. Dairy J. 2009, 19, 450–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Principal Component Analysis (PCA) of the sensory profiles of the 36 grapefruit-like beverages. The vectors indicate the loadings for sensory attributes while the position of the sample codes indicate the score values. The four-letter codes indicate levels of naringin (1st letter: L = Low, M = Medium, or H = High), sucrose (2nd letter: L = Low, M = Medium, or H = High), aroma (3rd letter: L = Low, or H = High) and color (4th letter: R = red or Y = yellow). Sensory attributes 1AT = Aftertaste, 2Fl = Flavor, 3Ar = Aroma. Beverages in green font were selected for the consumer tests.
Figure 1. Principal Component Analysis (PCA) of the sensory profiles of the 36 grapefruit-like beverages. The vectors indicate the loadings for sensory attributes while the position of the sample codes indicate the score values. The four-letter codes indicate levels of naringin (1st letter: L = Low, M = Medium, or H = High), sucrose (2nd letter: L = Low, M = Medium, or H = High), aroma (3rd letter: L = Low, or H = High) and color (4th letter: R = red or Y = yellow). Sensory attributes 1AT = Aftertaste, 2Fl = Flavor, 3Ar = Aroma. Beverages in green font were selected for the consumer tests.
Nutrients 11 00464 g001
Table 1. Factorial design for the 36 grapefruit-like beverages.
Table 1. Factorial design for the 36 grapefruit-like beverages.
NumberCode 1Bitter Level Naringin mg/kgSweet Level Sucrose BrixAroma2 Level mg/kgColor3
1LMHR158 low10 medium10 high.Red
2MMHR315 medium10 medium10 highRed
3HMHR473 high10 medium10 high.Red
4LHHR158 low12 high10 highRed
5MHHR315 medium12 high10 high.Red
6HHHR473 high12 high10 high.Red
7LLHR158 low8 low10 high.Red
8MLHR315 medium8 low10 high.Red
9HLHR473 high8 low10 highRed
10LMLR158 low10 medium2.5 lowRed
11MMLR315 medium10 medium2.5 lowRed
12HMLR473 high10 medium2.5 lowRed
13LHLR158 low12 high2.5 lowRed
14MHLR315 medium12 high2.5 lowRed
15HHLR473 high12 high2.5 lowRed
16LLLR158 low8 low2.5 lowRed
17MLLR315 medium8 low2.5 lowRed
18HLLR473 high8 low2.5 lowRed
19LMHY158 low10 medium10 high.Yellow
20MMHY315 medium10 medium10 high.Yellow
21HMHY473 high10 medium10 high.Yellow
22LHHY158 low12 high10 high.Yellow
23MHHY315 medium12 high10 high.Yellow
24HHHY473 high12 high10 high.Yellow
25LLHY158 low8 low10 high.Yellow
26MLHY315 medium8 low10 high.Yellow
27HLHY473 high8 low10 high.Yellow
28LMLY158 low10 medium2.5 lowYellow
29MMLY315 medium10 medium2.5 lowYellow
30HMLY473 high10 medium2.5 lowYellow
31LHLY158 low12 high2.5 lowYellow
32MHLY315 medium12 high2.5 lowYellow
33HHLY473 high12 high2.5 lowYellow
34LLLY158 low8 low2.5 lowYellow
35MLLY315 medium8 low2.5 lowYellow
36HLLY473 high8 low2.5 lowYellow
1 Code: 1st letter = bitter level (High, Medium, or Low); 2nd letter = sweet level (High, Medium, or Low); 3rd letter = aroma level (High or Low); 4th letter = color (Red or Yellow). Samples in bold italics were used for consumer evaluation. 2 Aroma blend = Caryophyllene, citral, nootkatone, aldehyde C8 (octanal), aldehyde C9 (nonanal), aldehyde C10 (decanal). 3 Red color = 0.001% solution (30% Sunset yellow and 70% Ponceau red); Yellow color = 0.0125% Quinoline yellow.
Table 2. Definitions of attributes used for describing the aroma, flavor, and aftertaste of the grapefruit-like beverages.
Table 2. Definitions of attributes used for describing the aroma, flavor, and aftertaste of the grapefruit-like beverages.
AttributeDefinition (References Indicated Where Applicable)
Aroma
Overall aroma intensityThe aroma of the beverage upon taking the first few sniffs
Citrus aromaThe aroma associated with the general impression of citrus fruits
Grapefruit aromaThe aroma of fresh grapefruit
Chemical aromaA very general term associated with many different types of compounds, such as solvents and cleaning compounds
Deteriorated/rotten aromaAroma associated with rotten, deteriorated, and decayed fruit/material
Muddy/moldy aromaAromatic characteristic of damp soil, wet foliage, or slightly undercooked boiled potato
Fruity aromaAroma associated with a mixture of non-specific fruits (apples, pears, melons, and guava)
Green/grassy aromaAromatic characteristic of freshly cut leaves, grass, or green vegetables (green beans)
Peely/peel oil aromaAroma associated with grapefruit peel or skin; Ref: Grapefruit oil extracted from grapefruit
Soapy aromaAroma associated with unscented soap
Pungent aromaAroma causing a sharp sensation of the nasal mucous membranes; Ref: vinegar
Woody/spicy aromaAroma associated with dry, fresh-cut wood; balsamic or bark-like; Ref: 10 ppm alpha-humulone in water
Sweet aromaAroma associated with high sugar content vegetables;Ref: Freshly boiled sweet corn
Flavor
Overall flavorThe intensity of the flavor that is released from the beverage upon taking the first sip
Sour tasteBasic taste on tongue stimulated by acids; Ref: citric acid in water
Sweet tasteTaste on the tongue stimulated by sugars;Ref: 5% sugar (sucrose) in water
Bitter tasteTaste on tongue stimulated by bitter solutions;Ref: 473 mg/kg naringin in water
Astringent flavorThe chemical feeling factor on the tongue or surface of the oral cavity described as puckering/dry and associated with tannins;Ref: Strong black tea
Citrus flavorFlavor associated with the general impression of citrus fruits;Ref: Cut lemon fruit and lime cordial
Grapefruit flavorThe flavor of fresh grapefruit; Ref: Cut red and white grapefruit flesh
Bitter aftertasteBitter taste remaining in the mouth after swallowing the beverage
Table 3. Summary of the significance of varying the main effects of color hue, aroma, bitter, and sweet levels of a model grapefruit-like beverage, on the mean values 1 (±SEM) for sensory attributes as evaluated by a trained sensory panel (n = 16).
Table 3. Summary of the significance of varying the main effects of color hue, aroma, bitter, and sweet levels of a model grapefruit-like beverage, on the mean values 1 (±SEM) for sensory attributes as evaluated by a trained sensory panel (n = 16).
AttributesColor 2Aroma 3 mg/kgBitter (Naringin mg/kg)Sweet (Sucrose mg/kg)
RedYellow 2.510 158 Low315 Medium473 High 8 Brix10 Brix12 Brix
Overall aroma intensity6.06a (0.05)5.83b (0.05)**5.52b (0.05)6.37a (0.05)***5.87a ² (0.06)6.04a (0.06)5.93a (0.06)NS5.96a (0.06)5.91a (0.06)5.96a (0.06)NS
Citrus aroma4.54a (0.05)4.55a (0.05)NS4.23b (0.05)4.86a (0.05)***4.51a (0.06)4.58a (0.06)4.55a (0.06)NS4.51a (0.06)4.58a (0.06)4.55a (0.06)NS
Grapefruit aroma4.40a (0.05)4.15b (0.05)**4.06b (0.05)4.49a (0.05)***4.28a (0.06)4.23a (0.06)4.32a (0.06)NS4.23a (0.06)4.35a (0.06)4.25a (0.06)NS
Chemical aroma4.09a (0.06)4.03a (0.06)NS3.86b (0.06)4.25a (0.06)***3.99a (0.07)4.13a (0.07)4.06a (0.07)NS4.06a (0.07)4.03a (0.07)4.09a (0.07)NS
Deteriorated/rotten aroma2.14a (0.04)2.00b (0.04)**2.09a (0.04)2.05a (0.04)NS2.08a (0.05)2.04a (0.05)2.09a (0.05)NS2.05a (0.05)2.04a (0.05)2.12a (0.05)NS
Muddy/moldy aroma2.20a (0.03)2.09b (0.03)**2.09b (0.03)2.20a (0.03)**2.12a (0.04)2.13a (0.04)2.18a (0.04)NS2.18a (0.04)2.13a (0.04)2.13a (0.04)NS
Fruity aroma3.93a (0.05)3.78b (0.05)*3.71b (0.05)4.00a (0.05)***3.89a (0.06)3.83a (0.06)3.85a (0.06)NS3.83a (0.06)3.90a (0.06)3.83a (0.06)NS
Green/grassy aroma3.13a (0.04)3.14a (0.04)NS2.91b (0.04)3.36a (0.04)***3.12a (0.05)3.09a (0.05)3.19a (0.05)NS3.12a (0.05)3.18a (0.05)3.10a (0.05)NS
Peely/peel oil aroma3.53a (0.05)3.47a (0.05)NS3.20b (0.05)3.79a (0.05)***3.53a (0.06)3.47a (0.06)3.49a (0.06)NS3.49a (0.06)3.55a (0.06)3.46a (0.06)NS
Soapy aroma3.29a (0.05)3.23a (0.05)NS3.16b (0.05)3.36a (0.05)**3.27a (0.06)3.20a (0.06)3.31a (0.06)NS3.41a (0.06)4.33a (0.06)3.04b (0.06)**
Pungent aroma3.16a (0.05)3.11a (0.05)NS2.84b (0.05)3.43a (0.05)***3.10a (0.06)3.14a (0.06)3.18a (0.06)NS3.17a (0.06)3.13a (0.06)3.11a (0.06)NS
Woody/spicy aroma2.49a (0.04)2.40a (0.04)NS2.33b (0.04)2.57a (0.04)***2.43a (0.04)2.44a (0.04)2.47a (0.04)NS2.48a (0.04)2.48a (0.04)2.37a (0.04)NS
Sweet aroma3.79a (0.05)3.62b (0.05)*3.56b (0.05)3.86a (0.05)***3.73a (0.06)3.69a (0.06)3.70a (0.06)NS3.69a (0.06)3.72a (0.06)3.72a (0.06)NS
Overall flavor intensity6.39a (0.05)6.34a (0.05)NS6.37a (0.05)6.36a (0.05)NS6.14b (0.06)6.51a (0.06)6.46a (0.06)***6.41a (0.06)6.29a (0.06)6.40a (0.06)NS
Sour flavor5.09a (0.06)5.17a (0.06)NS5.14a (0.06)5.11a (0.06)NS4.90b (0.08)5.08b (0.08)5.40a (0.08)***5.93a (0.08)5.07b (0.08)4.38c (0.08)***
Sweet flavor4.48a (0.05)4.43a (0.05)NS4.38a (0.05)4.53a (0.05)NS4.64a (0.06)4.51a (0.06)4.21b (0.06)***3.04c (0.06)4.50b (0.06)5.82a (0.06)***
Bitter flavor4.56a (0.07)4.48a (0.07)NS4.77a (0.07)4.27b (0.07)***3.94c (0.08)4.46b (0.08)5.17a (0.08)***5.24a (0.08)4.43b (0.08)3.89c (0.08)***
Astringent flavor4.95a (0.06)4.81a (0.06)NS4.97a (0.06)4.79b (0.06)*4.62b (0.07)4.91a (0.07)5.12a (0.07)***5.35a (0.07)4.88b (0.07)4.41c (0.07)***
Citrus flavor4.47a (0.05)4.43a (0.05)NS4.30b (0.05)4.60a (0.05)***4.52a (0.06)4.44a (0.06)4.39a (0.06)NS4.43a (0.06)4.45a (0.06)4.48a (0.06)NS
Grapefruit flavor4.53a (0.05)4.36b (0.05)*4.49a (0.05)4.40a (0.05)NS4.19b (0.07)4.41b (0.07)4.73a (0.07)***4.53a (0.07)4.54a (0.07)4.26b (0.07)**
Bitter aftertaste4.32a (0.07)4.26a (0.07)NS4.49a (0.07)4.08b (0.07)***3.69c (0.08)4.26b (0.08)4.91a (0.08)***4.90a (0.08)4.25b (0.08)3.71c (0.08)***
1 Attribute intensity scale from ‘not intense’ (0) to ‘very intense’ (10); 2 Red = 0.001% solution (30% Sunset yellow and 70% Ponceau red); Yellow = 0.0125% Quinoline yellow. 3 Aroma blend {caryophyllene, citral, nootkatone, aldehyde C8 (octanal), aldehyde C9 (nonanal), and aldehyde C10 (decanal)}. abc—different letters indicate significantly different mean values across design variable levels. Means represent the average of duplicate ratings by 16 panelists; * p ≤ 0.05, ** p ≤ 0.01, *** p ≤ 0.0001; NS = not significantly different.
Table 4. The effect of varying the color, aroma, and bitter/sweet gustatory flavorants on mean liking ratings 1 (±standard deviation) for color, aroma, and flavor of grapefruit-like beverages by (n = 90) consumers.
Table 4. The effect of varying the color, aroma, and bitter/sweet gustatory flavorants on mean liking ratings 1 (±standard deviation) for color, aroma, and flavor of grapefruit-like beverages by (n = 90) consumers.
Color 2 Aroma 3 mg/kg 3 Bitter-Sweet Naringin mg/kg /Sucrose Brix
RedYellow 2.510 mg/kg 158/12 Low/High315/10 Medium/Medium473/8 High/Low
Liking of color64b (30)60a (30)**63a (30)61a (31)NS62a 1 (30)62a (31)61a (30)NS
Liking of aroma51a (30)51a (30)NS53a (29)49b (31)***50ab (30)53a (30)49b (29)*
Liking of flavor45a (33)45a (34)NS45a (34)45a (34)NS55a (33)46b (33)34c (32)***
1 Simplified Labelled Affective Magnitude Scale (SLAM); 0 = greatest imaginable dislike, 100 = greatest imaginable liking. abc—different letters indicate significantly different mean values across the design variable levels. NS = not significant, * p ≤ 0.05, ** p ≤ 0.01, *** p ≤ 0.001. 2 Red = 0.001% solution (30% Sunset yellow and 70% Ponceau red); Yellow = 0.0125% Quinoline yellow.. 3 Aroma blend {caryophyllene, citral, nootkatone, aldehyde C8 (octanal), aldehyde C9 (nonanal), and aldehyde C10 (decanal)}.
Table 5. Standardized partial least squares (PLS) regression coefficients for factors to summarize the relationship between predictors (X, consumer liking variables) and Y, sensory response variables. Only selected important variables (main effects and squared effects, noted as ‘2’) from the refined models are shown.
Table 5. Standardized partial least squares (PLS) regression coefficients for factors to summarize the relationship between predictors (X, consumer liking variables) and Y, sensory response variables. Only selected important variables (main effects and squared effects, noted as ‘2’) from the refined models are shown.
Liking of the Color R2 = 0.871Liking of the Aroma R2 = 0.970Liking of the Flavor R2 = 0.982
Overall aroma intensity 20.23Fruity aroma 20.08Sweet aroma0.16
Citrus aroma 20.16Citrus flavor0.03Chemical aroma 20.15
Sweet aroma0.16Sweet flavor0.03Citrus flavor0.12
Astringent flavor 20.15Astringent flavor0.02Deteriorated/rotten aroma0.03
Green/grassy aroma 20.10Grapefruit flavor0.02Green/grassy aroma 2−0.01
Fruity aroma0.00Bitter aftertaste−0.01Greed/grassy aroma−0.01
Overall aroma intensity0.00Bitter flavor−0.01Chemical aroma−0.01
Grapefruit aroma0.00Sour flavor−0.02Woody/spicy aroma 2−0.04
Green/grassy aroma−0.01Overall flavor intensity−0.04Overall flavor intensity−0.04
Astringent flavor−0.04Deteriorated/rotten aroma−0.05Bitter flavor−0.07
Peely/peel oil aroma−0.04Soapy aroma−0.05Grapefruit flavor−0.07
Pungent aroma−0.05Chemical aroma−0.06Woody/spicy aroma−0.08
Citrus aroma−0.06Woody/spicy aroma−0.06Fruity aroma−0.08
Citrus flavor−0.07Sweet aroma−0.07Muddy/moldy aroma−0.10
Muddy/moldy aroma−0.08Fruity aroma−0.07Bitter flavor−0.11
Woody/spicy aroma−0.09Peely/peel oil aroma−0.08Astringent flavor−0.11
Soapy aroma−0.13Pungent aroma 2−0.08Sour flavor−0.12
Chemical aroma 2−0.18Citrus aroma−0.09Bitter aftertaste−0.12
Chemical aroma−0.19Grapefruit aroma−0.09Soapy aroma−0.19
Overall aroma intensity−0.10
Green/grassy aroma−0.11
Muddy/moldy aroma−0.12
Bitter flavor−0.14
Pungent aroma−0.14
Sweet flavor 2−0.16
Sweet aroma 2−0.17
Table 6. Partial least square regression (PLSR) model predicted liking ratings for color, aroma, and flavor of the grapefruit-like beverages.
Table 6. Partial least square regression (PLSR) model predicted liking ratings for color, aroma, and flavor of the grapefruit-like beverages.
Color LikingAroma LikingFlavor Liking
Number 1Code 2Observed 3PredictedObservedPredictedObservedPredicted
2MMHR61ab (31)6151ab (30)5144abc (31)45
4LHHR64ab (29)6445b (30)4554a (32)52
9HLHR63ab (33)6447ab (30)4735bc (33)36
11MMLR66a (29)6457a (29)5648a (33)48
13LHLR67a (30)6855ab (29)5555a (34)55
18HLLR62ab (30)6151ab (29)5032c (29)32
20MMHY62ab (32)6152ab (29)5248a (35)48
22LHHY55b (32)5648ab (33)4854a (34)54
27HLHY59ab (30)6049ab (31)4935bc (34)34
29MMLY60ab (32)6054ab (31)5446ab (34)47
31LHLY61ab (29)6252ab (29)5355a (33)56
36HLLY61ab (28)6250ab (28)5135bc (32)34
1LMHR 59 44 41
3HMHR 62 49 41
5MHHR 63 47 47
6HHHR 60 46 45
7LLHR 62 46 41
8MLHR 60 47 38
10LMLR 61 53 47
12HMLR 64 54 45
14MHLR 61 52 50
15HHLR 63 52 48
16LLLR 62 53 43
17MLLR 59 48 32
19LMHY 59 49 44
21HMHY 62 41 38
23MHHY 62 52 57
24HHHY 62 52 51
25LLHY 60 48 36
26MLHY 61 45 40
28LMLY 63 55 56
30HMLY 62 54 40
32MHLY 66 55 56
33HHLY 63 52 46
34LLLY 68 57 53
35MLLY 60 52 39
1 Refer to Table 1 for number. 2 Code: 1st letter = bitter level (High, Medium, or Low); 2nd letter = sweet level (High, Medium, or Low); 3rd letter = aroma level (High or Low); 4th letter = color (Red or Yellow). Samples in bold italic were used for consumer evaluation. 3 Values are means (± standard deviation); Observed means in a column with different letters are significantly different (p < 0.05).

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gous, A.G.S.; Almli, V.L.; Coetzee, V.; de Kock, H.L. Effects of Varying the Color, Aroma, Bitter, and Sweet Levels of a Grapefruit-Like Model Beverage on the Sensory Properties and Liking of the Consumer. Nutrients 2019, 11, 464. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11020464

AMA Style

Gous AGS, Almli VL, Coetzee V, de Kock HL. Effects of Varying the Color, Aroma, Bitter, and Sweet Levels of a Grapefruit-Like Model Beverage on the Sensory Properties and Liking of the Consumer. Nutrients. 2019; 11(2):464. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11020464

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gous, Andries G. S., Valérie L. Almli, Vinet Coetzee, and Henrietta L. de Kock. 2019. "Effects of Varying the Color, Aroma, Bitter, and Sweet Levels of a Grapefruit-Like Model Beverage on the Sensory Properties and Liking of the Consumer" Nutrients 11, no. 2: 464. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11020464

APA Style

Gous, A. G. S., Almli, V. L., Coetzee, V., & de Kock, H. L. (2019). Effects of Varying the Color, Aroma, Bitter, and Sweet Levels of a Grapefruit-Like Model Beverage on the Sensory Properties and Liking of the Consumer. Nutrients, 11(2), 464. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11020464

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop