The Effects of Nutritional Interventions on the Cognitive Development of Preschool-Age Children: A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Nutrients and Cognitive Development
1.2. Nutritional Interventions during the Preschool Years and Cognitive Outcomes
2. Methods
2.1. Search Strategy
2.2. Data Extraction
2.3. Risk of Bias
3. Results
3.1. Selection of Studies
3.2. Description of Studies
3.3. Study Quality
3.4. Study Participants
3.5. Nutritional Interventions
3.6. Cognitive Tests
3.7. Major Cognitive Outcomes
3.7.1. Single Nutrient Supplementation
3.7.2. Multiple-Micronutrient Supplementation
3.7.3. Food-Based Interventions
3.7.4. Effects of Nutritional Intervention Combined with Psychosocial Stimulation
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Population | Intervention | Comparison | Outcome |
---|---|---|---|
Preschool children (2–6 years of age) | Multiple-micronutrient (MMN) food fortification, supplement-based, or food-based nutritional interventions | Placebo or control group | Cognitive outcomes using cognitive assessment tests |
Database | Searched Terms |
---|---|
PubMed | (nutrient OR nutrition OR micronutrient OR macronutrient OR diet OR dietary OR “food intake” OR “meal diversity”) AND (“child development” OR cognition OR focus OR brain OR attentiveness OR attention OR memory OR verbal OR vocabulary OR learning OR literacy OR neuro* OR problem-solving OR reasoning OR “school performance” OR “school achievement” OR “academic achievement” OR “educational measurement” OR “academic success” OR “academic performance”) AND (randomized controlled trial [pt] OR controlled clinical trial [pt] OR randomized [tiab] OR placebo [tiab] OR clinical trials OR randomly [tiab] OR trial [ti]) AND (child OR children OR preschool OR “pre-school”) |
CENTRAL | (nutrient* OR nutrition* OR micronutrient* OR macronutrient* OR diet* OR “food intake” OR “meal diversity”) AND (“child development” OR cognition OR focus OR brain OR attentiveness OR attention OR memory OR verbal OR vocabulary OR learning OR literacy OR neuro* OR problem-solving OR reasoning OR “school performance” OR “school achievement” OR “academic achievement” OR “educational measurement”) AND (child OR children OR preschool OR “pre-school”) |
PsycInfo and Academic Search Complete | (nutrient* OR nutrition* OR micronutrient* OR macronutrient* OR diet* OR “food intake” OR “meal diversity”) AND (“child development” OR cognition OR focus OR brain OR attentiveness OR attention OR memory OR verbal OR vocabulary OR learning OR literacy OR neuro* OR problem-solving OR reasoning OR “school performance” OR “school achievement” OR “academic achievement” OR “educational measurement”) AND (random* OR control* OR trial* OR placebo* OR “double-blind”) AND (child OR children OR preschool OR “pre-school”) |
References
- De Onis, M.B.M. Quantifying the Health Impact at National and Local Levels. Available online: https://www.who.int/quantifying_ehimpacts/publications/MalnutritionEBD12.pdf (accessed on 31 August 2021).
- World Health Organization (WHO). Malnutrition Fact Sheets. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/malnutrition (accessed on 20 July 2021).
- De Onís, M.; Monteiro, C.; Akré, J.; Glugston, G. The Worldwide Magnitude of Protein-Energy Malnutrition: An Overview from the WHO Global Database on Child Growth. Bull. World Health Organ. 1993, 71, 703–712. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- World Health Organization. UNICEF/WHO/The World Bank Group Joint Child Malnutrition Estimates: Levels and Trends in Child Malnutrition: Key Findings of the 2021 Edition. Available online: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240025257 (accessed on 31 July 2021).
- Grantham-McGregor, S. A Review of Studies of the Effect of Severe Malnutrition on Mental Development. J. Nutr. 1995, 125 (Suppl. S8), 2233S–2238S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wachs, T.D. Relation of Mild-to-Moderate Malnutrition to Human Development: Correlational Studies. J. Nutr. 1995, 125 (Suppl. S8), 2245S–2254S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cusick, S.E.; Georgieff, M.K. The Role of Nutrition in Brain Development: The Golden Opportunity of the “First 1000 Days”. J. Pediatr. 2016, 175, 16–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Black, R.E.; Victora, C.G.; Walker, S.P.; Bhutta, Z.A.; Christian, P.; de Onis, M.; Ezzati, M.; Grantham-McGregor, S.; Katz, J.; Martorell, R.; et al. Maternal and Child Nutrition Study Group. Maternal and Child Undernutrition and Overweight in Low-Income and Middle-Income Countries. Lancet 2013, 382, 427–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sigman, M.; McDonald, M.A.; Neumann, C.; Bwibo, N. Prediction of Cognitive Competence in Kenyan Children from Toddler Nutrition, Family Characteristics and Abilities. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 1991, 32, 307–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martins, V.J.B.; Toledo Florêncio, T.M.M.; Grillo, L.P.; do Carmo P Franco, M.; Martins, P.A.; Clemente, A.P.G.; Santos, C.D.L.; de Fatima A Vieira, M.; Sawaya, A.L. Long-Lasting Effects of Undernutrition. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2011, 8, 1817–1846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mehta, N.M.; Corkins, M.R.; Lyman, B.; Malone, A.; Goday, P.S.; Carney, L.N.; Monczka, J.L.; Plogsted, S.W.; Schwenk, W.F.; American Society for Parenteral and Enteral Nutrition Board of Directors. Defining Pediatric Malnutrition: A Paradigm Shift toward Etiology-Related Definitions: A Paradigm Shift toward Etiology-Related Definitions. JPEN J. Parenter. Enter. Nutr. 2013, 37, 460–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Olsen, E.M. Failure to Thrive: Still a Problem of Definition. Clin. Pediatr. 2006, 45, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Homan, G.J. Failure to Thrive: A Practical Guide. Am. Fam. Physician 2016, 94, 295–299. [Google Scholar]
- Bourre, J.M. Effects of Nutrients (in Food) on the Structure and Function of the Nervous System: Update on Dietary Requirements for Brain. Part 1: Micronutrients. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2006, 10, 377–385. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Monk, C.; Georgieff, M.K.; Osterholm, E.A. Research Review: Maternal Prenatal Distress and Poor Nutrition—Mutually Influencing Risk Factors Affecting Infant Neurocognitive Development: Maternal Prenatal Distress and Poor Nutrition. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 2013, 54, 115–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pollitt, E.; Gorman, K.S.; Engle, P.L.; Rivera, J.A.; Martorell, R. Nutrition in Early Life and the Fulfillment of Intellectual Potential. J. Nutr. 1995, 125 (Suppl. S4), 1111S–1118S. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Muñoz, P.; Humeres, A. Iron Deficiency on Neuronal Function. Biometals 2012, 25, 825–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Todorich, B.; Pasquini, J.M.; Garcia, C.I.; Paez, P.M.; Connor, J.R. Oligodendrocytes and Myelination: The Role of Iron. Glia 2009, 57, 467–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Youdim, M.B.; Yehuda, S. The Neurochemical Basis of Cognitive Deficits Induced by Brain Iron Deficiency: Involvement of Dopamine-Opiate System. Cell. Mol. Biol. 2000, 46, 491–500. [Google Scholar]
- Abbaspour, N.; Hurrell, R.; Kelishadi, R. Review on Iron and Its Importance for Human Health. J. Res. Med. Sci. 2014, 19, 164–174. [Google Scholar]
- McCann, S.; Perapoch Amadó, M.; Moore, S.E. The Role of Iron in Brain Development: A Systematic Review. Nutrients 2020, 12, 2001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Black, M.M. Zinc Deficiency and Child Development. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1998, 68 (Suppl. S2), 464S–469S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Golub, M.S.; Keen, C.L.; Gershwin, M.E.; Hendrickx, A.G. Developmental Zinc Deficiency and Behavior. J. Nutr. 1995, 125 (Suppl. S8), 2263S–2271S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prasad, A.S. Impact of the Discovery of Human Zinc Deficiency on Health. J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 2009, 28, 257–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Prasad, A.S. Discovery of Human Zinc Deficiency and Studies in an Experimental Human Model. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1991, 53, 403–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strain, J.J.; McSorley, E.M.; van Wijngaarden, E.; Kobrosly, R.W.; Bonham, M.P.; Mulhern, M.S.; McAfee, A.J.; Davidson, P.W.; Shamlaye, C.F.; Henderson, J.; et al. Choline Status and Neurodevelopmental Outcomes at 5 Years of Age in the Seychelles Child Development Nutrition Study. Br. J. Nutr. 2013, 110, 330–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gámiz, F.; Gallo, M. A Systematic Review of the Dietary Choline Impact on Cognition from a Psychobiological Approach: Insights from Animal Studies. Nutrients 2021, 13, 1966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Albright, C.D.; Friedrich, C.B.; Brown, E.C.; Mar, M.H.; Zeisel, S.H. Maternal Dietary Choline Availability Alters Mitosis, Apoptosis and the Localization of TOAD-64 Protein in the Developing Fetal Rat Septum. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 1999, 115, 123–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albright, C.D.; Tsai, A.Y.; Friedrich, C.B.; Mar, M.-H.; Zeisel, S.H. Choline Availability Alters Embryonic Development of the Hippocampus and Septum in the Rat. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 1999, 113, 13–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kapil, U. Health Consequences of Iodine Deficiency. Sultan Qaboos Univ. Med. J. 2007, 7, 267–272. [Google Scholar]
- Mahan, L.K.; Raymond, J.L. Krause’s Food & The Nutrition Care Process, 14th ed.; Elsevier Inc.: St. Louis, MO, USA, 2017; pp. 257–260. [Google Scholar]
- Crider, K.S.; Yang, T.P.; Berry, R.J.; Bailey, L.B. Folate and DNA Methylation: A Review of Molecular Mechanisms and the Evidence for Folate’s Role. Adv. Nutr. 2012, 3, 21–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De-Regil, L.M.; Fernández-Gaxiola, A.C.; Dowswell, T.; Peña-Rosas, J.P. Effects and Safety of Periconceptional Folate Supplementation for Preventing Birth Defects. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2010, CD007950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dror, D.K.; Allen, L.H. Effect of Vitamin B12 Deficiency on Neurodevelopment in Infants: Current Knowledge and Possible Mechanisms. Nutr. Rev. 2008, 66, 250–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winje, B.A.; Kvestad, I.; Krishnamachari, S.; Manji, K.; Taneja, S.; Bellinger, D.C.; Bhandari, N.; Bisht, S.; Darling, A.M.; Duggan, C.P.; et al. Does Early Vitamin B12 Supplementation Improve Neurodevelopment and Cognitive Function in Childhood and into School Age: A Study Protocol for Extended Follow-Ups from Randomised Controlled Trials in India and Tanzania. BMJ Open 2018, 8, e018962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Weiser, M.J.; Butt, C.M.; Mohajeri, M.H. Docosahexaenoic Acid and Cognition throughout the Lifespan. Nutrients 2016, 8, 99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Clandinin, M.T.; Van Aerde, J.E.; Merkel, K.L.; Harris, C.L.; Springer, M.A.; Hansen, J.W.; Diersen-Schade, D.A. Growth and Development of Preterm Infants Fed Infant Formulas Containing Docosahexaenoic Acid and Arachidonic Acid. J. Pediatr. 2005, 146, 461–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bourre, J.M. Roles of Unsaturated Fatty Acids (Especially Omega-3 Fatty Acids) in the Brain at Various Ages and during Ageing. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2004, 8, 163–174. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- FHI Solutions LLC. Why 1000 Days. Available online: https://thousanddays.org/why-1000-days/ (accessed on 31 August 2021).
- Lenroot, R.K.; Giedd, J.N. Brain Development in Children and Adolescents: Insights from Anatomical Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006, 30, 718–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bryan, J.; Osendarp, S.; Hughes, D.; Calvaresi, E.; Baghurst, K.; van Klinken, J.-W. Nutrients for Cognitive Development in School-Aged Children. Nutr. Rev. 2004, 62, 295–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petry, N.; Olofin, I.; Boy, E.; Donahue Angel, M.; Rohner, F. The Effect of Low Dose Iron and Zinc Intake on Child Micronutrient Status and Development during the First 1000 Days of Life: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Nutrients 2016, 8, 773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Derbyshire, E.; Obeid, R. Choline, Neurological Development and Brain Function: A Systematic Review Focusing on the First 1000 Days. Nutrients 2020, 12, 1731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, T.T.; Jernigan, T.L. Brain Development during the Preschool Years. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2012, 22, 313–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Welsh, J.A.; Nix, R.L.; Blair, C.; Bierman, K.L.; Nelson, K.E. The Development of Cognitive Skills and Gains in Academic School Readiness for Children from Low-Income Families. J. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 102, 43–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Grantham-McGregor, S.; Cheung, Y.B.; Cueto, S.; Glewwe, P.; Richter, L.; Strupp, B. Developmental Potential in the First 5 Years for Children in Developing Countries. Lancet 2007, 369, 60–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stith, A.Y.; Gorman, K.S.; Choudhury, N. The Effects of Psychosocial Risk and Gender on School Attainment in Guatemala. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 52, 614–629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gupta, R.P.-S.; de Wit, M.L.; McKeown, D. The Impact of Poverty on the Current and Future Health Status of Children. Paediatr. Child. Health 2007, 12, 667–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kern, M.L.; Friedman, H.S. Early Educational Milestones as Predictors of Lifelong Academic Achievement, Midlife Adjustment, and Longevity. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 30, 419–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 Statement: An Updated Guideline for Reporting Systematic Reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aslam, S.; Emmanuel, P. Formulating a Researchable Question: A Critical Step for Facilitating Good Clinical Research. Indian J. Sex. Transm. Dis. AIDS 2010, 31, 47–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- The Cochrane Collaboration. The Cochrane Highly Sensitive Search Strategies for Identifying Randomized trials in MEDLINE. Available online: https://handbook-5-1.cochrane.org/chapter_6/6_4_11_1_the_cochrane_highly_sensitive_search_strategies_for.htm (accessed on 1 September 2021).
- Solomon, D.; Aderaw, Z.; Tegegne, T.K. Minimum Dietary Diversity and Associated Factors among Children Aged 6–23 Months in Addis Ababa, Ethiopia. Int. J. Equity Health 2017, 16, 181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Handu, D.; Moloney, L.; Wolfram, T.; Ziegler, P.; Acosta, A.; Steiber, A. Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics Methodology for Conducting Systematic Reviews for the Evidence Analysis Library. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2016, 116, 311–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rauh-Pfeiffer, A.; Handel, U.; Demmelmair, H.; Peissner, W.; Niesser, M.; Moretti, D.; Martens, V.; Wiseman, S.; Weichert, J.; Heene, M.; et al. Three-Month B Vitamin Supplementation in Pre-School Children Affects Folate Status and Homocysteine, but Not Cognitive Performance. Eur. J. Nutr. 2014, 53, 1445–1456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Demmelmair, H.; Øyen, J.; Pickert, T.; Rauh-Pfeiffer, A.; Stormark, K.M.; Graff, I.E.; Lie, Ø.; Kjellevold, M.; Koletzko, B. The Effect of Atlantic Salmon Consumption on the Cognitive Performance of Preschool Children—A Randomized Controlled Trial. Clin. Nutr. 2019, 38, 2558–2568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Øyen, J.; Kvestad, I.; Midtbø, L.K.; Graff, I.E.; Hysing, M.; Stormark, K.M.; Markhus, M.W.; Baste, V.; Frøyland, L.; Koletzko, B.; et al. Fatty Fish Intake and Cognitive Function: FINS-KIDS, a Randomized Controlled Trial in Preschool Children. BMC Med. 2018, 16, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Metallinos-Katsaras, E.; Valassi-Adam, E.; Dewey, K.G.; Lönnerdal, B.; Stamoulakatou, A.; Pollitt, E. Effect of Iron Supplementation on Cognition in Greek Preschoolers. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2004, 58, 1532–1542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ryan, A.S.; Nelson, E.B. Assessing the Effect of Docosahexaenoic Acid on Cognitive Functions in Healthy, Preschool Children: A Randomized, Placebo-Controlled, Double-Blind Study. Clin. Pediatr. 2008, 47, 355–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kvestad, I.; Vabø, S.; Kjellevold, M.; Nøstbakken, O.J.; Midtbø, L.K.; Hysing, M.; Markhus, M.W.; Madsen, L.; Handeland, K.; Graff, I.E.; et al. Fatty Fish, Hair Mercury and Cognitive Function in Norwegian Preschool Children: Results from the Randomized Controlled Trial FINS-KIDS. Environ. Int. 2018, 121 Pt 2, 1098–1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aboud, F.E.; Bougma, K.; Lemma, T.; Marquis, G.S. Evaluation of the Effects of Iodized Salt on the Mental Development of Preschool-Aged Children: A Cluster Randomized Trial in Northern Ethiopia: Iodized Salt and Child Development in Ethiopia. Matern. Child. Nutr. 2017, 13, e12322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roy Choudhury, D.; Nair Krishnapillai, M.; Nagalla, B.; Vijaya Kankipati, R.; Ghosh, S.; Buwade, J.; Fernandez-Rao, S. Guava with an Institutional Supplementary Meal Improves Iron Status of Preschoolers: A Cluster-Randomized Controlled Trial. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2021, 1492, 82–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, N.; Geiser, E.; Gosoniu, L.M.; Wibowo, Y.; Gentile-Rapinett, G.; Tedjasaputra, M.S.; Sastroasmoro, S. A Combined Dietary and Cognitive Intervention in 3–5-Year-Old Children in Indonesia: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ogunlade, A.O.; Kruger, H.S.; Jerling, J.C.; Smuts, C.M.; Covic, N.; Hanekom, S.M.; Mamabolo, R.L.; Kvalsvig, J. Point-of-Use Micronutrient Fortification: Lessons Learned in Implementing a Preschool-Based Pilot Trial in South Africa. Int. J. Food Sci. Nutr. 2011, 62, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Black, M.M.; Fernandez-Rao, S.; Nair, K.M.; Balakrishna, N.; Tilton, N.; Radhakrishna, K.V.; Ravinder, P.; Harding, K.B.; Reinhart, G.; Yimgang, D.P.; et al. A Randomized Multiple Micronutrient Powder Point-of-Use Fortification Trial Implemented in Indian Preschools Increases Expressive Language and Reduces Anemia and Iron Deficiency. J. Nutr. 2021, 151, 2029–2042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roberts, S.B.; Franceschini, M.A.; Silver, R.E.; Taylor, S.F.; de Sa, A.B.; Có, R.; Sonco, A.; Krauss, A.; Taetzsch, A.; Webb, P.; et al. Effects of Food Supplementation on Cognitive Function, Cerebral Blood Flow, and Nutritional Status in Young Children at Risk of Undernutrition: Randomized Controlled Trial. BMJ 2020, 370, m2397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masud Parvez, G.M.; Uzzaman, S.; Akanda, K.M.; Mehjabin, S. Open Access: Toxicology & Research A short review on a Nutritional Fruit: Guava Research Article. Available online: https://www.biocoreopen.org/oatr/A-short-review-on-a-Nutritional-Fruit--Guava.pdf (accessed on 16 January 2022).
- Hallberg, L.; Brune, M.; Rossander, L. The Role of Vitamin C in Iron Absorption. Int. J. Vitam. Nutr. Res. Suppl. 1989, 30, 103–108. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Sachdev, H.P.S.; Gera, T.; Nestel, P. Effect of Iron Supplementation on Mental and Motor Development in Children: Systematic Review of Randomised Controlled Trials. Public Health Nutr. 2005, 8, 117–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lam, L.F.; Lawlis, T.R. Feeding the Brain—The Effects of Micronutrient Interventions on Cognitive Performance among School-Aged Children: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Clin. Nutr. 2017, 36, 1007–1014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- World Health Organization. Guidelines on Food Fortification with Micronutrients. Available online: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9241594012 (accessed on 1 August 2021).
Criteria | Study Design | Population | Intervention | Outcome |
---|---|---|---|---|
Include | RCTs conducted after the year 2000 | Preschool Children (2–6 years of age) | Nutritional intervention including food-based, single, and multiple micronutrient supplementation intervention/s | Cognitive outcomes measured using cognitive assessment tests |
Healthy children and children suffering from undernutrition, anemia, parasitic infections, or HIV | Nutritional intervention/s provided after the first 1000days and in children <6 years of age | Cognition was measured after the first 1000 days or in children <6 years of age | ||
All other study designs and animal studies RCTs with a sample size <60 subjects | Newborns, infants, primary school-aged children, adolescents, adults, elderly | Nutritional intervention/s not provided to preschool-aged children | Cognitive outcomes not measured in preschool-aged children | |
Exclude | Children with specific diseases, such as cystic fibrosis, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), epilepsy, phenylketonuria, autism, and gluten-related neurological disorders |
Reference, Year, Country | Sample Size | Age | Subject Characteristics at Baseline | Intervention Group/s | Control Group/s | Duration | Cognitive Tests | Cognitive Domain Assessed | Major Cognitive Outcomes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Rauh-Pfeiffer et al. [55], 2014, Germany | 250 | 4–6 y | Urban area, high socioeconomic status, healthy children with low but not insuficient total folate catabolite concentrations (<34 nmol/mmol creatinine) | Children received flavorless powder containing folic acid (220 μg), riboflavin (1.1 mg), pyridoxine (0.73 mg), cobalamin (1.2 μg) and calcium lactate pentahydrate (130 mg) | Children received flavorless powder in sachets matching the intervention product in taste and appearance containing only 130 mg of calcium | 3 months | WPPSI-III & (K-ABC) | Verbal I.Q., short-term memory, and processing speed | No significant difference between groups |
Aboud et al. [61], 2017, Ethiopia | 1602 | 4–6 y | Rural, low socioeconomic status, two control and four intervention districts had high UIC levels at baseline | Children had access to iodized salt for 8 to 10 months. Children received iodized salt via assistance from regular salt distributors | Children had access to non-iodized salt for 4 months, and 4 to 6 months of iodized salt. Iodized salt was introduced by market forces | 10 months | WPPSI | Verbal and nonverbal reasoning, and school readiness | No significant difference between groups |
Demmelmair et al. [56], 2019, Germany | 205 | 4–6 y | Urban area, high socioeconomic status, healthy children | Children received three meals weekly containing 50 g Atlantic salmon per meal | Children received three meals weekly containing 50 g of meat per meal | 4 months | WPPSI-III & 9-HPT | Fine-motor skills, verbal reasoning, vocabulary, word and matrix reasoning, picture concepts, processing speed, coding, and symbol search | Intervention children displayed superior outcomes in WPPSI-III FIQ and PIQ. No significant changes were found in the WPPSI-III I.Q. scale scores between groups |
Choudhury et al. [62], 2021, India | 352 | 3–5 y | Rural, low socioeconomic status, ICDS beneficiaries only, and children with hemoglobin concentration >7 g/dL | Children received 25 g of guava with a supplementary meal (guava group) or 25 g of banana with a meal (banana group) | Children did not receive any fruits with the meal (cucumber was given with meal if caregivers of participants wished) | 8 months | MSEL | Visual reception, expressive language development, and fine-motor coordination | No significant difference between groups |
Øyen et al. [57], 2018, Norway | 232 | 4–6 y | Urban area, high socioeconomic status, healthy children | Children received three lunch meals per week with fatty fish (herring/mackerel), with a mean (SD) of 15.2 (14.2) mg/g EPA + DHA | Children received three lunch meals per week with meat (chicken/lamb/beef) with mean (SD) of 0.21 (0.15) mg/g EPA + DHA | 4 months | WPPSI-III & 9-HPT | Fine-motor skills, verbal reasoning, vocabulary, word and matrix reasoning, picture concepts, processing speed, coding, and symbol search | Intervention children improved speed of processing and fine-motor coordination in a sub-analysis adjusting for dietary compliance. No significant difference was found between in main analysis of total I.Q. scores (WPPSI-III) |
Schneider et al. [63], 2018, Indonesia | 192 | 3–5 y | Urban, an upper middle-income country, children with a below-average level of stimulation at home, normal cognitive development, and weight for height within 2 SD from the median z-score | Children consumed milk powder (477.7kcal) fortified with zinc (8), iron (11.4), magnesium (141), thiamin (1), niacin (11), pyridoxine (1.7), biotin (0.0177), Vitamin C (97.3), AHA (556.6) mg/100 g, and performed psychosocial stimulation 3 times a week | Children consumed 72 g of unfortified skimmed milk powder diluted in 180 mL of warm water (467.8 kcal) and did not receive psychosocial stimulation | 6 months | WPPSI-IV, CBCL 1.5–5 & PICCOLO | Cognitive functioning, cognitive development, memory, language, psychomotor skills, problem-solving, and attention | Children in the intervention group displayed increased cognitive performance and full-scale I.Q. composite score (WPPSI-IV), and reduction in attention problems (CBCL 1.5–5) |
Metallinos-Katsaras et al. [58], 2004, Greece | 124 | 3–4 y | Urban, high-income country, children with birth weight ≥ 2500 g, I.Q. ≥ 1 s.d. below the age-adjusted mean, blood Pb ≤ 200 ppb, weight and head circumference for the age ≥ 10th percentile | Children received 15 mg of iron and a multivitamins supplement (MV) five days per week at their respective day care center | Children received only the multivitamins supplement (MV) five days per week at their respective day care center | 2 months | Simple reaction time test, CPT & O.L. tasks | Speed of information processing, speed of discrimination, the accuracy of discrimination, and rate of conceptual learning | Iron-deficient children who received iron supplementation showed 14% increase in discrimination speed and 8% improvement in the accuracy domain. No effects in cognitive functioning were seen in good iron status children |
Ogunlade et al. [64], 2011, South Africa | 151 | 3–6.5 y | Urban, low socioeconomic status, children with Hb ≤ 12.5 g/dl, all children received anthelmintic | Children consumed 35 g of stiff maize-meal porridge with added micronutrient powder (8 g) containing amylase-rich light malted barley flour 5 days per week | Children consumed 28 g of soft maize-meal with added placebo powder (8 g) 5 days per week | 2.7 months | MPI, KABC-II, Atlantis & NVI | Learning abilities, sequential and simultaneous processing, and intellectual functioning | Intervention children showed significantly higher conceptual thinking abilities, higher MPI and NVI scores |
Ryan and Nelson [59], 2008, USA | 175 | 4 y | High-income country, healthy children consuming <6 oz of fish per week, English speakers, between 10th and 95th percentiles for weight and height, and currently not taking LC-PUFA supplements or consuming LC-PUFA fortified foods | Children received 400 mg of DHA supplementation as two 200-mg bubblegum-flavored softgel chewable | Children received capsules or placebo of high-oleic sunflower oil supplied as 2 soft capsules | 4 months | PPVT & kCPT | Memory, attention, vocabulary, processing speed, response time, listening skills, and verbal ability | There was no significant difference between groups in Leiter-R Test of Sustained Attention, (PPVT), Day-Night Stroop Test, and (kCPT). Regression analysis showed a significant positive association between levels of DHA in capillary whole blood and improved listening comprehension and vocabulary (PPVT) |
Black et al. [65], 2021, India | 321 | 3–5 y | Rural, low socioeconomic status, children living in a district with prevalence of anemia >70%, >50% of children consumed <50% of the recommended intake of several essential micronutrients, high-quality and low-quality preschools were included | Children received 300g of cooked food fortified with MNP (13 mg iron, 5 mg zinc, 20 μg folic acid, 150 μg vitamin A, 20 mg vitamin C, 0.5 μg vitamin B-12, and 0.5 mg riboflavin | Children received 300 g of cooked food containing 0.5 mg riboflavin (no effects on outcome measures) | 8 months | MSEL & BSID-III | Fine motor skills, gross motor skills, visual reception, receptive language, expressive language, and social-emotional behaviors | For children attending low-quality preschools, MNP fortification improved expressive language and marginally improved inhibitory control and social-emotional development in comparison to children attending control low-quality preschools. MNP fortification did not impact any area of cognitive development in children attending high-quality preschools |
Kvestad et al. [60], 2018, Norway | 232 | 4–6 y | High-income country, healthy children with no food allergies | Children received lunch meals containing 50–80 g of fatty fish (herring/mackerel) three times per week | Children received lunch meals containing 50–80 g of meat (chicken/lamb/beef) three times per week | 4 months | WPPSI-III | Information, vocabulary, block design, word and matrix reasoning, picture concepts, coding, and symbol search | No significant difference between groups |
Roberts et al. [66], 2020, Guinea-Bissau | 1059 | 1.3–7 y | Rural, low socioeconomic status, children living in one of the 10 rural villages in the Oio and Cacheu regions of Guinea-Bissau, children with severe acute malnutrition or relevant food allergies were excluded from the study | One group of children received NEWSUP (≈310 kcal) for breakfast as a raw paste containing 98% of recommended daily micronutrients for children under 4 y, the second group received FBF (≈310 kcal) for breakfast served as a corn soy blend with cooked porridge, fortified oil, sugar, and salt containing an average of 16% recommended daily micronutrients | Children received white rice cooked in water, soybean oil, and salt (≈310 kcal) containing an average of 1% of recommended daily micronutrients | 5.7 months | Working Memory Task Test | Working memory | Intervention children younger than 4y receiving NEWSUP displayed increased working memory compared to control children |
First Author, Year of Publication (Reference) | Rauh-Pfeiffer, 2014 [55] | Demmelmair, 2019 [56] | Ogunlade, 2011 [64] | Choudhury, 2021 [62] | Black, 2021 [65] | Kvestad, 2018 [60] | Roberts, 2020 [66] | Katsaras, 2004 [58] | Aboud, 2017 [61] | Øyen, 2018 [57] | Schneider, 2018 [63] | Ryan, 2008 [59] |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Primary Research QCC | ||||||||||||
1. Was the research question clearly stated? | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y |
2. Was the selection of study subjects/patients free from bias? | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y |
3. Were study groups comparable? | Y | Y | N | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | N | Y | Y | Y |
4. Was method of handling withdrawals described? | Y | Y | Y | N | Y | Y | Y | N | N | N | N | Y |
5. Was blinding used to prevent introduction of bias? | Y | N | Y | Y | Y | Y | N | Y | N | Y | N | Y |
6. Were intervention/exposure factor or procedure and any comparison(s) described in detail? | Y | N | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y |
7. Were outcomes clearly defined and the measurements valid and reliable? | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | N |
8. Was the statistical analysis appropriate for the study design and type of outcome indicators? | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | N | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y |
9. Were conclusions supported by results with biases and limitations taken into consideration? | N | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y |
10. Is bias due to study’s funding or sponsorship unlikely? | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | ? | Y | Y | N | Y |
OVERALL QUALITY | (+) | (+) | (Ø) | (Ø) | (+) | (+) | (+) | (Ø) | (Ø) | (Ø) | (Ø) | (+) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Roberts, M.; Tolar-Peterson, T.; Reynolds, A.; Wall, C.; Reeder, N.; Rico Mendez, G. The Effects of Nutritional Interventions on the Cognitive Development of Preschool-Age Children: A Systematic Review. Nutrients 2022, 14, 532. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14030532
Roberts M, Tolar-Peterson T, Reynolds A, Wall C, Reeder N, Rico Mendez G. The Effects of Nutritional Interventions on the Cognitive Development of Preschool-Age Children: A Systematic Review. Nutrients. 2022; 14(3):532. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14030532
Chicago/Turabian StyleRoberts, Marina, Terezie Tolar-Peterson, Abby Reynolds, Caitlin Wall, Nicole Reeder, and Gina Rico Mendez. 2022. "The Effects of Nutritional Interventions on the Cognitive Development of Preschool-Age Children: A Systematic Review" Nutrients 14, no. 3: 532. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14030532
APA StyleRoberts, M., Tolar-Peterson, T., Reynolds, A., Wall, C., Reeder, N., & Rico Mendez, G. (2022). The Effects of Nutritional Interventions on the Cognitive Development of Preschool-Age Children: A Systematic Review. Nutrients, 14(3), 532. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14030532