Who Would Taste It? Exploring Decision-Making Styles and Intention to Eat Insect-Based Food among Italian University Students
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Sustainability Goals and the Search for Alternative Protein Sources
1.2. Attitudes towards IBFs in Western Countries
1.3. Explicit Attitudes and Automatic Associations: Insights into IBF
1.4. Decision-Making Style, Attitudes, and Eating Behaviors
1.5. The Present Study
- Explicit attitudes, as measured by self-report scales with different semantic content. We included the following dimensions: ‘Bad’ vs. ‘Good’, ‘Risky’ vs. ‘Safe’, ‘Harmful’ vs. ‘Healthy’, and ‘Disgusting’ vs. ‘Tasty’. Based on previous literature, we expected to find:
- -
- Hp 1: negative attitudes, especially in the disgusting/tasty dimension.
- Implicit attitudes, as measured by IAT. We predicted the presence of:
- -
- Hp 2: automatic adverse reactions toward IBFs and favorable associations for traditional foods.
- Intention to taste, as measured regarding a specific item. We anticipated:
- -
- Hp 3: an average low propensity regarding taste. However, considering the age of our sample, we also expected to find several participants who were curious and inclined to taste IBF.
- 4.
- The identification of psychological profiles that could determine specific and differential dispositions towards IBFs. To achieve this, we used a person-centered approach [54], which involved categorizing individuals based on their similarities, enabling researchers to examine individuals more comprehensively than traditional approaches focused on isolated individual components.
- -
- Hp 3: more positive attitudes and a higher willingness to try IBFs in those participants low in food neophobia and higher in trust in science and scientists;
- -
- Hp 4: more positive attitudes and a higher willingness to try IBFs in those participants high in the rational style. We believe that the analysis of pros and cons could more easily lead to favorable opinions and a higher intention to taste. We also predicted a worse disposition in those high in intuitive/spontaneous style, since it could be more related on emotional/instinctive components.
- -
- RQ: we expected to find different combinations of the profiling variables determining specific patterns in the outcomes.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedure
2.3. Measures
2.3.1. Socio-Demographic Variables and Diet
2.3.2. Profiling Variables
2.3.3. Outcome Variables
- Grinding grasshoppers can produce flour for making bread, pizza, protein bars, or smoothies. Would you consider trying these grasshopper flour-based recipes in the future?
- In some restaurants, it is possible to taste burgers made with cricket flour. Would you like to try them in the future?
- There are cookies on the market produced using dried moth larvae. Would you like to try them in the future?
- It is already possible to buy crackers made from dried insects. Would you like to try them in the future?
2.4. Data Analysis
2.4.1. Preliminary Analyses
2.4.2. Identification of Psychological Profiles
- Cluster analyses were performed on the continuous scores of the psychological traits (FNS, TSSI, GDMS), following the recommendations of Bergman and colleagues [70]. First, all variables were standardized. Additionally, a residue analysis was conducted (average squared Euclidean distance—ASED—less than 0.5). Ten multivariate outliers were identified (6.3% of the sample) and removed from the subsequent analyses. A two-step clustering procedure was used, which combined Ward’s hierarchical and nonhierarchical k-means methods. In the hierarchical method, different solutions were explored based on the magnitude of the change in the explained error sum of squares percentage (%EESS) value between adjacent cluster solutions. Subsequently, each solution was employed as the initial cluster center for a nonhierarchical k-means clustering procedure.
- Descriptive statistics were calculated on all profiling variables.
- Differences in age and gender distribution were investigated. For gender analysis, χ2 test was run on cluster and gender variables. For age analysis, a univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed with age as a dependent variable, and cluster as an independent variable.
2.4.3. Differences between Clusters on Outcome Variables
- Descriptive statistics were calculated on the outcome variables.
- Three separate univariate ANOVAs were conducted using mean explicit attitudes, automatic attitudes, and the intention to taste IBFs as dependent variables. The independent variable in each analysis was cluster. Post hoc Tukey tests were used for comparisons when variances were equal, while the Games–Howell method was used when variances were unequal. Before conducting the analyses, the normal distribution of the variables was confirmed through assessments of skewness and kurtosis, and the homogeneity of variances was evaluated using Levene’s test.
3. Results
3.1. Preliminary Analyses
3.2. Identification of Psychological Profiles
3.2.1. Cluster Analyses
3.2.2. Descriptive Statistics
Mean (SD) | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cluster | n (%) | Mean Age (SD) | n Male (%) | GDMSr | GDMSi | GDMSd | GDMSa | GDMSs | TSSI | FNS |
1 | 46 (31%) | 21.7 (1.37) | 19 (41.3%) | 18.8 (1.48) | 18.5 (1.82) | 16.9 (3.02) | 12.7 (3.37) | 15.2 (2.75) | 4.10 (0.31) | 31.7 (9.28) |
2 | 38 (25.7%) | 21.5 (0.95) | 12 (31.6%) | 20.5 (1.48) | 15.9 (1.62) | 19.2 (2.80) | 13.9 (3.33) | 11.3 (2.01) | 3.84 (0.26) | 37.2 (9.07) |
3 | 30 (20.3%) | 21.4 (1.04) | 13 (43.3%) | 20.5 (1.74) | 15.0 (2.78) | 21.5 (2.69) | 19.2 (3.09) | 11.1 (2.10) | 4.37 (0.35) | 24.4 (8.90) |
4 | 34 (23%) | 22.4 (2.22) | 21 (61.8%) | 21.6 (1.60) | 16.0 (2.46) | 17.5 (2.80) | 10.4 (2.41) | 10.6 (1.84) | 4.57 (0.29) | 25.6 (8.57) |
3.2.3. Gender Differences
3.3. Differences between Clusters on Outcome Variables
3.3.1. Explicit Attitudes towards IBFs
3.3.2. Automatic Attitudes towards IBFs
3.3.3. Intention to Taste IBFs
4. Discussion
- The ‘gut feeling’ profile is mainly characterized by a decision-making style with a combination of spontaneous and intuitive and not at all rational. We are, therefore, faced with people who make decisions solely based on their feelings and emotions, in a very quick way. The cluster is characterized by rather negative attitudes, both implicit and explicit, and a modest intention to taste. Not surprisingly, avoidance of analytical reasoning regarding pros and cons, and trust in one’s own instincts, can lead to avoidance behavior toward IBFs. In fact, several previous contributions have highlighted the crucial role of emotional aspects, and particularly of disgust, toward IBFs. For example, in the previously cited work by La Barbera and colleagues [28], food neophobia and the emotion of disgust were found to negatively and independently affect the intention to eat IBFs. The explanatory power of disgust was even greater. This important finding underscores how, although the two constructs may be similar, they do not overlap and thus may contribute specifically and differentially to the outcomes. The modest declared intention to taste may be more determined by the tendency to try and not back down, but it does not seem very promising, since it is not supported by favorable attitudes. For the ‘gut feeling’ profile, we can speculate that the emotion of disgust might be one of the determinants of aversion to IBF, and that a more intuitive/impulsive system for decision-making could be adopted. Although not statistically significant, this profile was the one with the highest percentage of women. This finding is also reflected in the literature, as it has been found that women are generally more reluctant to accept IBFs and report higher FN and disgust scores [17,75,76].
- ‘The suspicious’ profile finds support in previous literature, as it is characterized by high food neophobia, negative attitudes, and low intention to taste IBFs. This profile is similar to one of the two profiles identified by Junges and colleagues [29] in their qualitative/quantitative work. The segments identified were ‘consumers with a favorable attitude toward insect-based foods’ and ‘consumers with an unfavorable attitude toward insect-based foods.’ The main characteristic of people belonging to the second segment, in addition to negative attitudes and low intention to eat IBFs, were high food neophobia scores and suspiciousness toward these novel foods. The negative relationship between food neophobia and willingness toward IBF has already been widely confirmed in the literature [23,24,25]. In Verbeke’s work [17], it was found that the increase of just one unit in food neophobia scores led to an 84% decrease in the likelihood of being ready to adopt a diet that includes IBFs. A very interesting perspective is offered by the work of Jaeger and colleagues [77], which showed that people with higher FN scores rated the emotional impact of food more negatively and with greater arousal. ‘The suspicious’ profile, moreover, is characterized by very low trust in science. This result, which has no previous findings in the entomophagy literature, is in line with our hypotheses, as it was, instead, identified with other sustainable foods. In their work investigating openness to try cultured meat, Lewish and Riefler [34] found that distrust of scientists was negatively related to behavioral intention. Similar findings emerged on the acceptance of genetically modified foods [32].
- ‘The vicarious’ profile is characterized by a fair overt disposition toward IBF in terms of both explicit attitudes and intention to taste. However, this good disposition is not matched by automatic attitudes and the scores are comparable to those of the two less favorable profiles. How can this discrepancy be explained? The cluster is characterized by the concurrence of two decision-making styles: avoidant and dependent. The avoidant style is prone to postponing any decision and correlates negatively with rationality in decision-making [56]. The dependent needs confirmation and seeks external references to make decisions, such as advice from trusted people, but also from what authorities suggest. More interestingly, both the avoidant and dependent profiles are positively associated with indecisiveness, as opposed to the rational style [56]. At the same time, they present low food neophobia. This aspect is very important, since it indicates how low food neophobia is not sufficient to develop totally favorable dispositions, as already argued, nor to explain this ambivalence in cluster 3. It is therefore possible to hypothesize that the indecisiveness that characterizes both these styles may have led people to respond relatively positively to explicit questions, either because they did not have to think too much (avoidant) or because of social desirability (dependent), but still manifest a low propensity toward a more automatic level. In this case, it is possible to hypothesize a conflict between the two systems.
- Finally, ‘the mind’ profile, characterized by a rational decision-making style and high trust in science, has more positive attitudes than the other profiles toward IBFs and a higher intention to taste them. These characteristics are partially reflected in the literature. In a previous study, Vernau and colleagues [45] investigated the intention of an Italian and a Danish sample to include IBFs in their diet by performing market segmentation based on their scores on the Food-Related Lifestyle Scale. Although they used different tools than those employed in the present work, the researchers identified a ‘rational food consumer’ profile, corresponding to an informed person who gathers information about the products they buy and considers multiple factors at once when shopping. Again, this profile was the one that declared a more favorable intention. Indeed, the main characteristics of the rational decision maker [56] involve a logical evaluation of possible alternatives and a meticulous search for information, as also confirmed by eye-tracking data on product labels [78]. In addition, a positive correlation between rational style and cognitive engagement has previously been revealed [56]. A more positive propensity toward IBFs is not only explicitly stated by ‘the mind’ cluster, but also emerges from the reaction times of the IAT, albeit not statistically significantly. This profile is the only one with a higher percentage of men respondents. This result has a basis in previous literature, since men have been found to be more accepting of IBFs than women [76,79]. An Italian study [80] demonstrated that men were 2.55 times more likely to be open to insect consumption. However, the analysis of gender differences in previous research has produced mixed results [81,82] and so deserves a more thorough exploration in future studies.
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- O’Neill, D.W.; Fanning, A.L.; Lamb, W.F.; Steinberger, J.K. A Good Life for All within Planetary Boundaries. Nat. Sustain. 2018, 1, 88–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- FAO. Greenhouse Gas Emissions from Agrifood Systems; FAO: Rome, Italy, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Michel, P.; Begho, T. Paying for Sustainable Food Choices: The Role of Environmental Considerations in Consumer Valuation of Insect-Based Foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 2023, 106, 104816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rockström, J.; Steffen, W.; Noone, K.; Owens, S. A safe operating space for humanity (2009). In The Future of Nature; Robin, L., Sörlin, S., Warde, P., Eds.; Documents of Global Change; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 2013; pp. 491–505. ISBN 978-0-300-18461-7. [Google Scholar]
- van Huis, A.; Oonincx, D.G.A.B. The Environmental Sustainability of Insects as Food and Feed. A Review. Agron. Sustain. Dev. 2017, 37, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bodenheimer, F.S. Insects as Human Food. In Insects as Human Food: A Chapter of the Ecology of Man; Bodenheimer, F.S., Ed.; Springer Netherlands: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1951; pp. 7–38. ISBN 978-94-017-6159-8. [Google Scholar]
- FAO. Looking at Edible Insects from a Food Safety Perspective; FAO: Rome, Italy, 2021; ISBN 978-92-5-134196-4. [Google Scholar]
- Maffei, G.; Tacchini, G. Un Insetto nel Piatto [A Bug on the Plate]; Red Edizioni: Cornaredo (MI), Italy, 2016; ISBN 978-88-573-0701-5. [Google Scholar]
- Berggren, Å.; Jansson, A.; Low, M. Approaching Ecological Sustainability in the Emerging Insects-as-Food Industry. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2019, 34, 132–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stone, H.; FitzGibbon, L.; Millan, E.; Murayama, K. Curious to Eat Insects? Curiosity as a Key Predictor of Willingness to Try Novel Food. Appetite 2022, 168, 105790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rumpold, B.A.; Schlüter, O.K. Nutritional Composition and Safety Aspects of Edible Insects. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 2013, 57, 802–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nakagaki, B.J.; Defoliart, G.R. Comparison of Diets for Mass-Rearing Acheta Domesticus (Orthoptera: Gryllidae) as a Novelty Food, and Comparison of Food Conversion Efficiency with Values Reported for Livestock. J. Econ. Entomol. 1991, 84, 891–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guiné, R.P.F.; Correia, P.; Coelho, C.; Costa, C.A. The Role of Edible Insects to Mitigate Challenges for Sustainability. Open Agric. 2021, 6, 24–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Müller-Maatsch, J.; Gras, C. The “Carmine problem” and potential alternatives. In Handbook on Natural Pigments in Food and Beverages; Carle, R., Schweiggert, R.M., Eds.; Woodhead Publishing Series in Food Science, Technology and Nutrition; Woodhead Publishing: Cambridge, UK, 2016; pp. 385–428. ISBN 978-0-08-100371-8. [Google Scholar]
- Faccio, E.; Guiotto Nai Fovino, L. Food Neophobia or Distrust of Novelties? Exploring Consumers’ Attitudes toward GMOs, Insects and Cultured Meat. Appl. Sci. 2019, 9, 4440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roma, R.; Ottomano Palmisano, G.; De Boni, A. Insects as Novel Food: A Consumer Attitude Analysis through the Dominance-Based Rough Set Approach. Foods 2020, 9, 387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verbeke, W. Profiling Consumers Who Are Ready to Adopt Insects as a Meat Substitute in a Western Society. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 39, 147–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bigliardi, B.; Galati, F. Innovation Trends in the Food Industry: The Case of Functional Foods. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2013, 31, 118–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verbeke, W. Consumer Acceptance of Functional Foods: Socio-Demographic, Cognitive and Attitudinal Determinants. Food Qual. Prefer. 2005, 16, 45–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mascarello, G.; Pinto, A.; Rizzoli, V.; Tiozzo, B.; Crovato, S.; Ravarotto, L. Ethnic Food Consumption in Italy: The Role of Food Neophobia and Openness to Different Cultures. Foods 2020, 9, 112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegrist, M.; Hartmann, C.; Keller, C. Antecedents of Food Neophobia and Its Association with Eating Behavior and Food Choices. Food Qual. Prefer. 2013, 30, 293–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pozharliev, R.; De Angelis, M.; Rossi, D.; Bagozzi, R.; Amatulli, C. I Might Try It: Marketing Actions to Reduce Consumer Disgust toward Insect-Based Food. J. Retail. 2023, 99, 149–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mancini, S.; Sogari, G.; Menozzi, D.; Nuvoloni, R.; Torracca, B.; Moruzzo, R.; Paci, G. Factors Predicting the Intention of Eating an Insect-Based Product. Foods 2019, 8, 270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sogari, G.; Menozzi, D.; Mora, C. The Food Neophobia Scale and Young Adults’ Intention to Eat Insect Products. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2019, 43, 68–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vartiainen, O.; Elorinne, A.-L.; Niva, M.; Väisänen, P. Finnish Consumers’ Intentions to Consume Insect-Based Foods. J. Insects Food Feed 2020, 6, 261–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- La Barbera, F.; Amato, M.; Fasanelli, R.; Verneau, F. Perceived Risk of Insect-Based Foods: An Assessment of the Entomophagy Attitude Questionnaire Predictive Validity. Insects 2021, 12, 403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baker, M.A.; Shin, J.T.; Kim, Y.W. An Exploration and Investigation of Edible Insect Consumption: The Impacts of Image and Description on Risk Perceptions and Purchase Intent. Psychol. Mark. 2016, 33, 94–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- La Barbera, F.; Verneau, F.; Amato, M.; Grunert, K. Understanding Westerners’ Disgust for the Eating of Insects: The Role of Food Neophobia and Implicit Associations. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 64, 120–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Junges, J.R.; do Canto, N.R.; de Barcellos, M.D. Not as Bad as I Thought: Consumers’ Positive Attitudes Toward Innovative Insect-Based Foods. Front. Nutr. 2021, 8, 631934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Slovic, P.; Fischhoff, B.; Lichtenstein, S. Facts and Fears: Understanding Perceived Risk. In Societal Risk Assessment; Schwing, R.C., Albers, W.A., Eds.; General Motors Research Laboratories; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegrist, M.; Cvetkovich, G. Perception of Hazards: The Role of Social Trust and Knowledge. Risk Anal. 2000, 20, 713–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Deng, H.; Hu, R. A Crisis of Consumers’ Trust in Scientists and Its Influence on Consumer Attitude toward Genetically Modified Foods. Br. Food J. 2019, 121, 2454–2476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lusk, J.L.; Roosen, J.; Bieberstein, A. Consumer Acceptance of New Food Technologies: Causes and Roots of Controversies. Annu. Rev. Resour. Econ. 2014, 6, 381–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewisch, L.; Riefler, P. Behavioural Intentions towards Cultured Meat: The Role of Personal Values, Domain-Specific Innovativeness and Distrust in Scientists. Br. Food J. 2023, 125, 1769–1781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ajzen, I. The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Menozzi, D.; Sogari, G.; Veneziani, M.; Simoni, E.; Mora, C. Eating Novel Foods: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour to Predict the Consumption of an Insect-Based Product. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 59, 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thu Thu Aung, M.; Dürr, J.; Klink-Lehmann, J.; Borgemeister, C. Predicting Consumers’ Intention towards Entomophagy Using an Extended Theory of Planned Behavior: Evidence from Myanmar. Int. J. Trop. Insect. Sci. 2023, 43, 1189–1206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bae, Y.; Choi, J. Consumer Acceptance of Edible Insect Foods: An Application of the Extended Theory of Planned Behavior. Nutr. Res. Pract. 2021, 15, 122–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Songa, G.; Russo, V. IAT, Consumer Behaviour and the Moderating Role of Decision-Making Style: An Empirical Study on Food Products. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 64, 205–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacoby, L.L.; Lindsay, D.S.; Toth, J.P. Unconscious Influences Revealed: Attention, Awareness, and Control. Am. Psychol. 1992, 47, 802–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strack, F.; Deutsch, R. Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Social Behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2004, 8, 220–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greenwald, A.G.; Banaji, M.R. Implicit Social Cognition: Attitudes, Self-Esteem, and Stereotypes. Psychol. Rev. 1995, 102, 4–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greenwald, A.G.; McGhee, D.E.; Schwartz, J.L.K. Measuring Individual Differences in Implicit Cognition: The Implicit Association Test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 74, 1464–1480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maison, D.; Greenwald, A.G.; Bruin, R.H. Predictive Validity of the Implicit Association Test in Studies of Brands, Consumer Attitudes, and Behavior. J. Consum. Psychol. 2004, 14, 405–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verneau, F.; La Barbera, F.; Amato, M.; Riverso, R.; Grunert, K.G. Assessing the Role of Food Related Lifestyle in Predicting Intention towards Edible Insects. Insects 2020, 11, 660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kröger, T.; Dupont, J.; Büsing, L.; Fiebelkorn, F. Acceptance of Insect-Based Food Products in Western Societies: A Systematic Review. Front. Nutr. 2022, 8, 759885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verneau, F.; La Barbera, F.; Kolle, S.; Amato, M.; Del Giudice, T.; Grunert, K. The Effect of Communication and Implicit Associations on Consuming Insects: An Experiment in Denmark and Italy. Appetite 2016, 106, 30–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shelomi, M. The Meat of Affliction: Insects and the Future of Food as Seen in Expo 2015. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2016, 56, 175–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bénard, M.; Bellisle, F.; Kesse-Guyot, E.; Julia, C.; Andreeva, V.A.; Etilé, F.; Reach, G.; Dechelotte, P.; Tavolacci, M.-P.; Hercberg, S.; et al. Impulsivity Is Associated with Food Intake, Snacking, and Eating Disorders in a General Population. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2019, 109, 117–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- French, S.A.; Epstein, L.H.; Jeffery, R.W.; Blundell, J.E.; Wardle, J. Eating Behavior Dimensions. Associations with Energy Intake and Body Weight. A Review. Appetite 2012, 59, 541–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bavoľár, J.; Bačíková-Slešková, M. Do Decision-Making Styles Help Explain Health-Risk Behavior among University Students in Addition to Personality Factors? Stud. Psychol. 2018, 60, 71–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Driver, M.J.; Rowe, A.J. Decision-making styles: A new approach to management decision making. In Behavioral Problems in Organizations; Cooper, C.L., Ed.; Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1979; pp. 141–182. [Google Scholar]
- Scott, S.G.; Bruce, R.A. Decision-Making Style: The Development and Assessment of a New Measure. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1995, 55, 818–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Magnusson, D. The logic and implications of a person-oriented approach. In Methods and Models for Studying the Individual; Sage Publications, Inc: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1998; pp. 33–64. ISBN 978-0-7619-1451-8. [Google Scholar]
- Cook, C.; Gonzales, H. Australian Individual Decision Styles, Intuitive and Rational Decision Making in Business. Int. Proc. Econ. Dev. Res. 2016, 86, 93–100. [Google Scholar]
- Curşeu, P.L.; Schruijer, S.G.L. Decision Styles and Rationality: An Analysis of the Predictive Validity of the General Decision-Making Style Inventory. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2012, 72, 1053–1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Atta, M.; Malik, N.I.; Makhdoom, I.F.; Fiaz, N.; Scholar, M.P. Decision-Making Styles Predicting Decisional-Procrastination among College Principals. Indian J. Econ. Bus. 2021, 20, 1635–1647. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2013; ISBN 978-1-4832-7648-9. [Google Scholar]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.-G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A Flexible Statistical Power Analysis Program for the Social, Behavioral, and Biomedical Sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gambetti, E.; Fabbri, M.; Bensi, L.; Tonetti, L. A Contribution to the Italian Validation of the General Decision-Making Style Inventory. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2008, 44, 842–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plohl, N.; Musil, B. Modeling Compliance with COVID-19 Prevention Guidelines: The Critical Role of Trust in Science. Psychol. Health Med. 2021, 26, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Proserpio, C.; Laureati, M.; Bertoli, S.; Battezzati, A.; Pagliarini, E. Determinants of Obesity in Italian Adults: The Role of Taste Sensitivity, Food Liking, and Food Neophobia. Chem. Senses 2016, 41, 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Backer, C.J.S.; Hudders, L. Meat Morals: Relationship between Meat Consumption Consumer Attitudes towards Human and Animal Welfare and Moral Behavior. Meat Sci. 2015, 99, 68–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nadelson, L.; Jorcyk, C.; Yang, D.; Jarratt Smith, M.; Matson, S.; Cornell, K.; Husting, V. I Just Don’t Trust Them: The Development and Validation of an Assessment Instrument to Measure Trust in Science and Scientists. Sch. Sci. Math. 2014, 114, 76–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pliner, P.; Hobden, K. Development of a Scale to Measure the Trait of Food Neophobia in Humans. Appetite 1992, 19, 105–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maggino, F.; Mola, T. Il Differenziale Semantico per la Misura degli Atteggiamenti: Costruzione, Applicazione e Analisi; Universita Degli Studi Firenze: Florence, Italy, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Greenwald, A.G.; Nosek, B.A.; Banaji, M.R. Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test: I. An Improved Scoring Algorithm. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 197–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cronbach, L.J. Coefficient Alpha and the Internal Structure of Tests. Psychometrika 1951, 16, 297–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hu, L.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria versus New Alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bergman, L.R.; Nurmi, J.-E.; Eye, A.A. von I-States-as-Objects-Analysis (ISOA): Extensions of an Approach to Studying Short-Term Developmental Processes by Analyzing Typical Patterns. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2012, 36, 237–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vargha, A.; Torma, B.; Bergman, L.R. ROPstat: A General Statistical Package Useful for Conducting Person-Oriented Analyses. J. Pers.-Oriented Res. 2015, 1, 87–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- West, S.G.; Finch, J.F.; Curran, P.J. Structural Equation Models with Nonnormal Variables: Problems and Remedies. In Structural Equation Modeling: Concepts, Issues, and Applications; Sage Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1995; pp. 56–75. ISBN 978-0-8039-5317-8. [Google Scholar]
- Castro, M.; Chambers, E. Consumer Avoidance of Insect Containing Foods: Primary Emotions, Perceptions and Sensory Characteristics Driving Consumers Considerations. Foods 2019, 8, 351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tuccillo, F.; Marino, M.G.; Torri, L. Italian Consumers’ Attitudes towards Entomophagy: Influence of Human Factors and Properties of Insects and Insect-Based Food. Food Res. Int. 2020, 137, 109619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Egolf, A.; Siegrist, M.; Hartmann, C. How People’s Food Disgust Sensitivity Shapes Their Eating and Food Behaviour. Appetite 2018, 127, 28–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruby, M.B.; Rozin, P. Disgust, Sushi Consumption, and Other Predictors of Acceptance of Insects as Food by Americans and Indians. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 74, 155–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jaeger, S.R.; Chheang, S.L.; Roigard, C.M.; Cardello, A.V. Individual Differences in Food Neophobia and Private Body Consciousness Influence Product-Elicited Emotional Valence and Arousal. Food Qual. Prefer. 2022, 99, 104566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ares, G.; Mawad, F.; Giménez, A.; Maiche, A. Influence of Rational and Intuitive Thinking Styles on Food Choice: Preliminary Evidence from an Eye-Tracking Study with Yogurt Labels. Food Qual. Prefer. 2014, 31, 28–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lammers, P.; Ullmann, L.M.; Fiebelkorn, F. Acceptance of Insects as Food in Germany: Is It about Sensation Seeking, Sustainability Consciousness, or Food Disgust? Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 77, 78–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cicatiello, C.; De Rosa, B.; Franco, S.; Lacetera, N. Consumer Approach to Insects as Food: Barriers and Potential for Consumption in Italy. Br. Food J. 2016, 118, 2271–2286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szakály, Z.; Kovács, B.; Soós, M.; Kiss, M.; Balsa-Budai, N. Adaptation and Validation of the Food Neophobia Scale: The Case of Hungary. Foods 2021, 10, 1766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tuorila, H.; Lähteenmäki, L.; Pohjalainen, L.; Lotti, L. Food Neophobia among the Finns and Related Responses to Familiar and Unfamiliar Foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 2001, 12, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, T.D.; Lindsey, S.; Schooler, T.Y. A Model of Dual Attitudes. Psychol. Rev. 2000, 107, 101–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Vanutelli, M.E.; Adorni, R.; Leone, P.A.; Luperini, A.; D’Addario, M.; Steca, P. Who Would Taste It? Exploring Decision-Making Styles and Intention to Eat Insect-Based Food among Italian University Students. Nutrients 2024, 16, 3458. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16203458
Vanutelli ME, Adorni R, Leone PA, Luperini A, D’Addario M, Steca P. Who Would Taste It? Exploring Decision-Making Styles and Intention to Eat Insect-Based Food among Italian University Students. Nutrients. 2024; 16(20):3458. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16203458
Chicago/Turabian StyleVanutelli, Maria Elide, Roberta Adorni, Paolo Alberto Leone, Aldo Luperini, Marco D’Addario, and Patrizia Steca. 2024. "Who Would Taste It? Exploring Decision-Making Styles and Intention to Eat Insect-Based Food among Italian University Students" Nutrients 16, no. 20: 3458. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16203458
APA StyleVanutelli, M. E., Adorni, R., Leone, P. A., Luperini, A., D’Addario, M., & Steca, P. (2024). Who Would Taste It? Exploring Decision-Making Styles and Intention to Eat Insect-Based Food among Italian University Students. Nutrients, 16(20), 3458. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16203458