Next Article in Journal
An Evaluation of the Pea Pod System for Assessing Body Composition of Moderately Premature Infants
Next Article in Special Issue
Vitamin D in Fibromyalgia: A Causative or Confounding Biological Interplay?
Previous Article in Journal
Dietary Impact of Adding Potassium Chloride to Foods as a Sodium Reduction Technique
Previous Article in Special Issue
Vitamin D and Sunlight Exposure in Newly-Diagnosed Parkinson’s Disease
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Vitamin D and Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Literature Review

1
Institute of Food Science and Technology—School of Food and Nutrition, Massey University, Palmerston North 4474, New Zealand
2
Department of Emergency Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02114, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2016, 8(4), 236; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu8040236
Submission received: 14 March 2016 / Revised: 8 April 2016 / Accepted: 14 April 2016 / Published: 21 April 2016

Abstract

:
Low vitamin D status in early development has been hypothesised as an environmental risk factor for Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), given the concurrent increase in the prevalence of these two conditions, and the association of vitamin D with many ASD-associated medical conditions. Identification of vitamin D-ASD factors may provide indications for primary and secondary prevention interventions. We systematically reviewed the literature for studies on vitamin D-ASD relationship, including potential mechanistic pathways. We identified seven specific areas, including: latitude, season of conception/birth, maternal migration/ethnicity, vitamin D status of mothers and ASD patients, and vitamin D intervention to prevent and treat ASD. Due to differences in the methodological procedures and inconsistent results, drawing conclusions from the first three areas is difficult. Using a more direct measure of vitamin D status—that is, serum 25(OH)D level during pregnancy or childhood—we found growing evidence for a relationship between vitamin D and ASD. These findings are supported by convincing evidence from experimental studies investigating the mechanistic pathways. However, with few primary and secondary prevention intervention trials, this relationship cannot be determined, unless randomised placebo-controlled trials of vitamin D as a preventive or disease-modifying measure in ASD patients are available.

1. Introduction

1.1. Description of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

ASD is a neurodevelopment disorder characterized by impairment in socio-communicative functioning and by limited interests and repetitive and stereotypic behaviours [1]. Depending on the child’s predominant symptomatology, ASD is comprised of different subgroups, including autistic disorder, Asperger Syndrome and Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS) [2]. ASD usually appears during the first three years of life, but some cases appear later in life when social demands increase (regressive subtype) [1]. Individuals with ASD have: (1) difficulty in expressing and understanding certain emotions; (2) difficulty in understanding others’ mood; (3) impairment in expressive language; (4) abnormal eye contact; (5) prefer minimal changes to routine; and (6) restricted ways of using toys and preference for isolated play, all of which make it difficult for individuals to establish relationships with others, to act in an appropriate way and to live independently [2]. Children with ASD also frequently experience behavioural and medical symptoms. Evidence of abnormalities in inflammatory markers, autoantibodies to brain and glutathione in subsets of ASD patients has been reported; together, these findings suggest that ASD is a systemic condition with the likelihood of being a disease of inflammation, autoimmunity, and/or oxidative stress [3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16].
Over the past few years, the prevalence of ASD has increased dramatically. While previous prevalence studies identified less than 10 in 10,000 individuals [17], recent estimates suggest rates of 90 to 250 in 10,000 individuals [1,18,19,20]. Although this increase is, in part, attributed to the increased awareness and reporting of the disorder, as well as improved diagnostic criteria [21], a complex of genetic and environmental risk factors has also been implicated. It is now believed that ASD is genetically driven and can be triggered by environmental risk factors [22]. In 2008, Cannell published the hypothesis that low vitamin D status, either during foetal life or early childhood, is an important environmental risk factor for ASD [23]. Since then, the role of vitamin D in the aetiology of ASD has drawn epidemiologists’ attention, but intervention research on the role of vitamin D as a preventive and disease-modifying measure is still in its infancy.

1.2. Vitamin D: Metabolism, Biomarker, and Optimum Level

Vitamin D, a fat-soluble vitamin, is a general name for a collection of steroid-like substances including ergocalciferol (D2) and cholecalciferol (D3) [24]. Vitamin D is present in the diet in limited amounts and is obtained mainly from exposure of skin to UVB radiation. During sun exposure, 7-dihydroxycholesterol which is present mainly in the layers of epidermis and to a lesser extent in dermis [25] absorbs UVB radiation and is converted to pre-vitamin D3 [24]. Then pre-vitamin D3 undergoes heat-induced isomerisation and forms vitamin D3. Vitamin D3 is derived from a cholesterol metabolite, and vitamin D2 is the product of irradiation of plants [24]. Vitamin D binding protein (DBP) in the circulation is responsible for the transport of both vitamins D2 and D3. Carried by DBP, these vitamin D metabolites enter the liver where they are hydroxylated and converted to their respective 25-hydroxyvitamin D metabolites (e.g., 25(OH)D3) [24]. These metabolites are inactive until they are converted to 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D [1,25(OH)2D] by 25-hydroxyvitamin D-1α-hydroxylase (1-OHase), and become biologically active [26]. At this point, vitamin D can bind to the vitamin D receptor (VDR)—found in most body tissues and organs [24,26]—and exert its musculoskeletal and non-musculoskeletal role in the body. However, because 1,25(OH)2D synthesis is tightly regulated, circulating 25(OH)D has been suggested to be the best single indicator of vitamin D status; it reflects both dietary vitamin D intake and vitamin D synthesised by UVB radiation [27,28].
Vitamin D deficiency and insufficiency are controversial terms but generally defined as serum 25(OH)D concentration <25 nmol/L and 25–49.9 nmol/L, respectively [29]. Other cut-offs for vitamin D deficiency and insufficiency are used by other groups, e.g., <50 and 50–74.9 nmol/L, respectively [30]. Until there is more clarity on these terms, we believe it is best to report the actual values rather to rely on ambiguous terms.

1.3. Objective

We aimed to investigate the relationship between vitamin D and ASD, and to systematically review the literature for all studies on this relationship, including studies on the mechanistic pathways that might underlie this relationship.

2. Methods

Firstly, we reviewed previous reviews [23,31,32] to identify vitamin D-related risk factors related to ASD. We grouped the identified factors into seven specific areas including (1) latitude; (2) season of birth and conception; (3) maternal migration and ethnicity; (4) vitamin D status of mothers; (5) vitamin D status of ASD patients; (6) maternal vitamin D intervention to prevent ASD; and (7) vitamin D intervention to treat ASD. According to their relationship with prevention stages, each of these areas were assigned to one of the three major areas of; (1) vitamin D-ASD-related areas providing indications for primary prevention; (2) vitamin D-ASD-related areas providing indications for both primary and secondary prevention; and (3) vitamin D-ASD-related areas providing indications for secondary prevention.
We performed a literature search covering studies published up to January 31 2016 in PubMed, the Web of Knowledge, EBSCO OVID, MEDLINE, PsycARTICLES, PsycINFO, SocINDEX and Google Scholars databases. Because it was impossible to cover all specific areas with one fixed term search we employed a distinct search strategy for each specific area. The search strategy was as follows: autism or Asperger or “Autism Spectrum Disorder” or ASD or “Pervasive Developmental Disorder” or PDD AND the following distinct search terms for each specific area;
  • “incidence” or “prevalence” to search the literature in relation to latitude. For studies published between 1992 and 2012, we identified literature from a previous systematic review of autistic disorder and PDD prevalence worldwide [33] to have a manageable data for search. We excluded studies published before 1992 because case ascertainment was based on DSM-III and ICD-9.
  • “season” or “month” AND “birth” or “conception” to search the literature in relation to Season of conception or birth.
  • “migrant” or “immigrant” or “migration” or “immigration” AND “maternal” or “mother” to search the literature in relation to maternal migration and ethnicity.
  • “vitamin D” or ergocalciferol or “vitamin D2” or “cholecalciferol” or “vitamin D3”or “25-hydroxyvitamin D” or “25(OH)D” or “25ohd” to search the literature in relation to maternal vitamin D status, vitamin D status in ASD patients, maternal vitamin D intervention to prevent ASD, and vitamin D intervention to treat ASD.
We followed this summary with a presentation of the mechanistic pathways by which vitamin D may exert its role in either aetiology or pathobiology of ASD. The selection criteria for each of these specific areas varied and, for maximal clarity, the criteria are presented at the beginning of each specific area.

3. Three Major Areas of Research

3.1. Vitamin D—ASD-Related Areas Providing an Indication for Primary Prevention

In this section, we will discuss vitamin D-related risk factors related to ASD which can be targeted for preventive purposes before ASD occurs. We found five specific areas including latitude, season of conception and birth, maternal migration and ethnicity, vitamin D status of mothers, and maternal vitamin D intervention to prevent ASD.

3.1.1. Risk of ASD—Latitude

We included studies that met the following criteria: (1) reported a prevalence estimate for either of the following: Autistic Disorders (AD), ASD (including AD, Asperger’s Syndrome, PDD-NOS and PDD (ASD plus Rett’s disorder and Childhood Disintegrative Disorder); (2) case ascertainment was based on either Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) or International Classification of Diseases and related health problems—Tenth Edition (ICD-10); and (3) included children <8 years of age. Studies that used medical registry or records only were excluded. There are several methodological factors that affect the prevalence estimates of ASD and these include classification systems, case ascertainment and participants’ age. In studies using DSM—III, the prevalence is much lower than in those using DSM-IV or the Chinese classification system of Medical Disorders (CCMD) [34]. Also, some individuals previously diagnosed with ASD according to DSM-IV might miss out on a diagnosis with the new criteria outlined in DSM-5 because the definition of autism has gone through three major changes [35]. Several separate categories in DSM-IV including AD, Childhood Disintegrative Disorder, Asperger’s Syndrome and PDD-NOS has been included into the one overarching category named “ASD”, the three domains of impairments in social interaction, impairments in communication and stereotyped behaviour in DSM-IV have become two, with the merging of the first two domains (deficits in social-communicative functioning), and finally the diagnosis age of three has been dropped and included manifestation of disorders either currently or by history [2].
Our literature search identified prevalence data for eight countries [17,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45]. Four studies were from Asia (latitudes 24.46–39.91°N) [36,42,43,45], one from South America (34.28°S) [44] and six from Europe (51.24–63.91°N) [17,37,38,39,40,41]. We did not identify any study from the US, Africa and Australia/New Zealand that met our inclusion criteria. Among the included studies, different tools were employed, including eight screening instruments and seven diagnostic tools. There were seven studies that reported prevalence estimate for AD [17,36,37,38,39,40,41], four for PDD [42,43,44,45] and three for both AD and PDD [17,37,38]. Data regarding the prevalence estimate for AD and PDD are presented in Table 1, respectively. The AD prevalence estimate per 10,000 ranged from 10.9 in China [36] to 60.0 in Sweden [40]. The PDD prevalence estimate ranged from 6.3 in Iran [43] to 181.0 in Japan [45], and, on average, was >30 in Europe.
Two systematic reviews of epidemiological studies illustrated a regional variation in the prevalence estimate for autism [46], and a four-fold increase in the prevalence estimate for infantile autism in moving from Israel to Sweden latitudinally [47]. Consistent with these findings, we also observed a latitudinal variation in the prevalence estimate for AD and PDD, with those in the lower latitudes reporting lower prevalence estimates and vice versa. There were, however, a few exceptions; compared to Sweden [39], Finland had lower AD prevalence estimate despite being located at the quite similar latitude [41]. Mandatory vitamin D food fortification of certain foods and dietary supplementation policies may have improved vitamin D status in Finish populations [48,49,50], and may explain the lower prevalence estimate for AD in this country. The higher prevalence estimate for PDD in Japan compared with European countries may also, in part, be explained by the higher prevalence of low vitamin D status among Japanese women than European women. Approximately 95% of Japanese pregnant women have been reported to have 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L; the corresponding figures in the UK and Latin America have been reported to be 55% and 37%, respectively [51,52], a finding confirmed by a systematic review of global maternal and new born vitamin D status [53]. This may be due to lower dietary vitamin D intake and more sun—avoidance behaviour in Japanese population [54].
The latitude differences in prevalence estimates for autism should be interpreted with caution since they assume that latitude is a reasonable proxy measure for vitamin D status. If low vitamin D status is a risk factor for autism, it is expected to see high prevalence of autism in countries with high prevalence of low vitamin D status and vice versa. This relationship is well illustrated by both low prevalence estimates for PDD and vitamin D deficiency/insufficiency in Argentina (34.28°S). The prevalence estimate for PDD was 13.1 per 10,000, and for 25(OH)D levels <25 and <50 nmol/L was reported to be only 3% and 33% in young children and adults, respectively [55].
However, the low prevalence estimate for AD in China and PDD in Iran and UAE despite having high prevalence of low vitamin D status [56,57,58] may be due to methodological issues (screening and diagnostic characteristics) and underreporting. Translation of a screening or diagnostic instrument may adversely affect its psychometric properties and therefore its sensitivity in picking up the cues for autism [43]. This is an issue for non—English speaking developing countries such as Iran and UAE. Employing different screening cut-off ranges can also be a driving factor; using a wider cut-off range includes more children for the final diagnosis and thus fewer children are likely to miss the diagnosis. Furthermore, a shortage of service delivery, stigmatising effect of autism on parents, parental tolerance and expectations of children’s behaviour and cultural influences and norms for atypical behaviours may be associated with underreporting of autism in developing countries [59].
Within each study population, it is also important to consider UVB doses reaching the population, differences in skin pigmentation, sun—avoidance or seeking behaviours, clothing style, and racial and ethnic differences. The prevalence of infantile autism in cohorts born before 1985 was reported to be in the earlier months to the North, perhaps partly, it is associated with the lower UVB doses and therefore lower vitamin D production [47]. Grant et al. (2013) reported an inverse relationship between the solar UVB radiation doses and the prevalence of ASD [60]. Accordingly, living at higher latitudes might be associated with lower doses of UVB radiation, if not compensated by more sun exposure or ingestion of vitamin D rich foods or supplements, and consequently increased risk of ASD.

3.1.2. Risk of ASD—Migration and Ethnicity

We included studies that (1) reported odds ratio (OR), prevalence ratio or relative risk for AD, ASD and/or PDD; (2) reported mother’s country of birth or race/ethnicity; and (3) case ascertainment was based on formal diagnoses according to disease classification systems.
Our literature search identified 10 studies [61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70], with seven studies from Europe [61,62,63,64,65,67,70], two from the US [68,69] and one from Australia [66] (Table 2). The number of cases ranged from 250 in a study from Sweden [67] to 7540 in a study from the US [69]. Depending on the study design, statistical analysis were adjusted for a range of confounding factors, ranging from only one factor [63] to several factors in other studies [61,62,64,65,66,67,68,69,70], though the same factors were not considered by all studies.
Most studies showed increased risk of ASD among children of migrant parents [61,62,63,64,65,66,67,69,70], while some reported no association [61,65,68,69] or even decreased risk [61,68,69,70]. Williams et al. (2008) reported that the odds of having a child with ASD among mothers born outside Australia significantly increased compared with those born in Australia, 1.4 (95% CI, 1.0–1.9; p = 0.009) [66]. The mothers’ original countries were not reported. Keen et al. (2010) reported an adjusted OR ranging from 1.2 (95% CI, 0.8–1.9) in children of European mothers born outside the UK to 10.0 (95% CI, 5.5–18.1) in children of Caribbean mothers compared with mothers born in the UK [63]. Magnusson et al. (2012) also reported an increased risk of having a child with ASD among mothers born outside Sweden as compared to those born in Sweden [61].
Magnusson et al. (2012) reported an increased risk of low functioning autism and decreased risk of high functioning autism in children of migrant parents in Sweden, 1.2 (1.0–1.4) and 0.5 (0.4–0.6), respectively [61], a finding confirmed by Williams et al. 2008 [66] and Becerra et al. (2014) [69]. The risk of having a child diagnosed with both autism and mental retardation increased two-fold in foreign-born black, Vietnamese and Filipino mothers [69]. In a study from the US, the proportion of children with ASD with intellectual disability was significantly different between black, Hispanic and white children: 48%, 38% and 25%, respectively [71]. A possible explanation is that mothers with darker skin are at increased risk of low vitamin D status and moving to northern latitudes with lower solar UVB radiation may exacerbate the condition and thereby lead to more severe disability among their children. It should be pointed out that having 25(OH)D levels in this study would address that explanation.
van der Ven et al. (2013), however, reported a decreased risk for ASD in children whose mothers were born in developing countries compared with those born in developed countries [70]. Within the ASD subtypes, a significant increased risk for AD and a decreased risk for Asperger Syndrome and PDD-NOS combined was reported in mothers of developing countries. These findings are consistent with several other studies reporting an increased risk for AD [62,64,65,67,68,69] and decreased risk for Asperger Syndrome [67] in children of mothers born outside the reference country. These findings are mirrored in studies investigating the effect of child’s ethnicity on prevalence estimate of ASD. While children of black ethnicity had increased risk for AD, 2.6 (1.3–5.0), the risk for Asperger Syndrome and PDD-NOS decreased in these children 0.5 (0.2–1.0) [72]. An explanation is that the aetiology of AD might be different from those of Asperger Syndrome and PDD-NOS in different race/ethnic group and that low vitamin D status which is highly prevalent in some race/ethnic groups, could exacerbate the condition, and cause AD.
The question remains why migration status is associated with no effect or decreased risk in some studies. Those showing no or decreased effect of maternal migration status mainly compared the prevalence estimates for autism among mothers born in the same region or of white ethnicity [61,64,65,69]. These populations share quite homogeneous characteristics with those of the reference country—the majority, if not all, have white skin colour and quite similar culturally accepted dressing code and attitudes toward sun exposure—that may affect vitamin D status of these populations.
The risk of AD was reported to be higher in children of mothers born in Mexico and in East Asian countries living in the US than those of mothers born in the US [68,69]. The decreased association reported in these studies may, in part, be attributed to the statistical analysis and the characteristics of minority groups. Although the relative risk of having a child with AD in mothers born in Mexico was 0.6 (95% CI, 0.5–0.7) in the Croen et al. study [68], it was 1.1 (95% CI, 1.0–1.2) in another study by Becerra and colleagues [69]. In the latter study, statistical analysis was adjusted for more variables (13 vs. 7 variables, respectively), including diagnostic variability [68,69]. It is well documented that diagnostic variability contributes significantly to the prevalence estimates [34]. The effect of covariates on final findings is also illustrated by the lower crude rates for ASD in black and US-born Hispanics that was similar or slightly higher than the US-born whites when the statistical analysis was controlled for several covariates [69]. Furthermore, minorities might be under-identified for ASD due to lower socioeconomic status, cultural differences in views of typical and atypical behaviours, viewing communication and social skill delays as temporary that disappear with age, parent-health professional communication gap and lack of culturally sensitive screening and assessment tools [73]. Finally, not all studies controlled for sun exposure variables; therefore it is difficult to relate these results directly to vitamin D-ASD relationship.

3.1.3. Risk of ASD-Season of Conception and Birth

We included all studies that relied on formal diagnoses of autism (according to disease classification systems) and reported season/month of conception or birth in patients with autism (infantile autism, AD, and ASD).
Our literature search identified 20 studies (Table 3); with one study reporting the season/month of conception only [74], three reporting both the season/month of conception and birth [75,76,77], and 16 reporting the season/month of birth only [64,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92]. The number of cases ranged from 54 in a Turkish study [79] to 19,328 in a US study [75]. Most participants in the control groups were live births (13 studies), two included siblings [78,85], one, both siblings and live births [89], three healthy age and gender matched children [78,79,81], and one compared cases with other neuro-developmental disorders [86]. Most of these studies did not control for any covariates in the statistical analysis [76,78,79,81,84,86,87,88,89,91,92]. Eight studies made adjustments for covariates ranging from two [82,83] to 12 covariates [64], though different factors were considered in these studies.
Children conceived during the winter months, when sun exposure is limited and vitamin D status is lower, were reported to have a higher risk of developing autism later in life than those conceived during the summer months [74]. Masumdar et al. (2012) [74] reported an increased rate of children later diagnosed with AD conceived in winter (final three weeks of November and first weeks of December) in California from 1994 to 1996 (p < 0.05), though the seasonality finding disappeared between 1996 to 2000. The authors did not control for maternal residence at birth. While controlling for several variables including year of conception and maternal residence at birth, Zerbo et al. (2011) reported a 6% (OR 1.06, 95% CI 1.02–1.10) increase in the risk of AD in children conceived during winter compared with summer months in California between 1990 and 2002 [75]. The incidence rate of AD increased steadily from the conception month of August to March, decreased from March to April and then remained unchanged from April to July. The rate of AD in November births was significantly higher compared to April.
By contrast, Hebert et al. (2010) reported an increased rate of children later diagnosed with ASD in those conceived during summer in the UK; with a conception rate of 9.5/1000 vs. 5.7/1000 in summer (June—August) and winter (December-February) months, respectively, with a corresponding peak in spring births, an OR of 1.86 (1.01–3.37) [76]. The study had a small sample size of 86 cases, and the age, duration and follow up measures were not clearly stated. Moreover, there was no adjustment for potential confounding.
Lastly, Atladottir et al. (2007) found no association between season of conception or birth on incidence rate of ASD and ASD subcategories in a cohort born in Denmark between 1990 and 1999 [77]. The sample size was smaller (n = 1860) than the sample size in the Masumdar et al. and Zerbo et al. studies (n = 8074 and 19,328, respectively), and the authors did not control for several variables included in the latter studies.
Season/month of birth and risk of autism has also been a research focus among epidemiologists. Spring birth has been associated with increased risk of autism in studies conducted in Canada [92], Japan [91], the US [82], Sweden [78,90], and the UK [76]. March birth as a risk factor for autism has also been reported by several studies from Canada [92], Denmark [80,88], the US [85] and Israel [87]. Lee et al. (2008) reported three peaks during spring, summer and autumn months in singleton and multiple births concordant with ASD (n = 1051), and an 87% decreased risk for multiple births concordant for ASD in males in winter (December) compared to other seasons [relative risk 0.18, 95% CI 0.04, 0.82; p < 0.05) [82]. Consistent with these findings, Konstantareas et al. (1986) and Gillberg et al. (1990) also reported a significant deviation of season/month of autistic births from that of the general population among males, a pattern that was not evident in females (n = 36 and 25, respectively) [90,92]. Konstantreas et al. commented that this finding could be due to the sampling artefact. With a male: female ratio of 4:1 and an ASD multiple birth sample size of 161 [82], the results related to gender differences in the Lee et al. study (2008) may also be a sampling artefact.
In contrast, Maimburg et al. (2010) reported increased risk for infantile autism in those born in winter (October to March) [80]. Aggregation of different months into seasons or half years can result in different results or even loss of information. Bolton et al. (1992) reported different results by moving one month from a season and including it in the next month, for instance inclusion of December in the winter or autumn months [89]. A seasonal effect of birth on autism was reported when December was included in the winter; however, this pattern disappeared when December was included as an autumn month. As such, inclusion of both December and March in winter in the Maimburg et al. study (2010) [80], may explain the increased rate of autism births during this season.
Not all studies showed an association between season of birth and risk of autism [64,77,79,81,83,84,85,86]. The absent association in these studies may be attributed, at least in part, to the definition of season (discussed previously). Hultman et al. (2002) [64] categorised months into two seasons; four months in one season versus eight months in other season, a method that may have obscured the association. Furthermore, the small sample size in the Meguid et al. (2010), Ugar et al. (2014) and Fernell et al. (2015) studies (all n < 70) may have contributed to their non-significant findings.
The characteristics of the populations included in case and control groups, and case ascertainment may also mask the seasonal variability among groups. For instance, Fennell et al. (2015) did not find any seasonal deviations for ASD populations from the typically developing controls of Middle Eastern/African origin, though the season of birth difference was significant in cases and controls of Swedish origin (38% in spring vs. 18% in summer; p < 0.05) [78]. This could be due to the narrow range and low year-round mean 25(OH)D concentrations in these populations (28.5–33.3 nmol/L in winter and summer, respectively) [93]. Kolevzon et al. (2006), identified cases through a screening programme for military services in Israel and Orthodox women were exempted from the military service. Therefore, some autistic cases might have been missed [83]. Landau et al. (1999) included cases with autism from different regions with different environmental influences and controls were individuals with mental retardation [86]. Disorders of neurodevelopment may share the same environmental influences and inclusion of these disorders in the control group may mask the seasonal variability among groups.
Interestingly, some studies did not find a season/month of birth effect on aggregated samples but reported seasonal deviations from the general population in autism with comorbidities such as language and developmental delay [84,85]. For example, Yeates Frederiks et al. (2000) did not find an effect of season of birth on autism rate in the aggregated sample, but they found a significant seasonal trend for low functioning autism (autism with IQ < 35) (increased rates for autistic births in the second quarter of the year) [84]. This reflects a trend similar to the Konstantreas et al. (1986) [92] and Stevens et al. (2000) [85] findings of a more pronounced seasonal effect on low functioning and or nonverbal autism. The differences in the seasonal effect of birth and risk of low and high functioning autism might be attributed to different etiological factors or to the same etiological factors but of different intensity. Whitehouse et al. (2012) reported a significant positive relationship between the severity of maternal vitamin D deficiency and the severity of the language impairment (mild and moderate to severe) in children at five and 10 years of age [94]. As such, the lack of season of birth variation does not exclude the possibility of an association if autism with comorbidities was considered.
Finally, seasonal of birth is a crude proxy measure for sun exposure and, more specifically, vitamin D status; many other potentially relevant factors vary by season. These environmental factors include application of pesticides [95], infection [96], nutritional factors [97] and maternal biochemistry [98], all of which have been hypothesised to be implicated in the aetiology of ASD, and need to be taken into consideration when interpreting the literature on season of conception/birth and risk of ASD.

3.1.4. Risk of ASD-Vitamin D Status in Mothers

We included all studies reporting maternal vitamin D status and the risk for AD, Asperger Syndrome and or ASD in offspring. Our literature search identified two articles investigating the relationship between maternal vitamin D status and autism among offspring. Fernell et al. (2010) compared maternal 25(OH)D concentration of Swedish mothers with and without a child with autism (n = 12 with vs 14 without) and Somali mothers with and without a child with autism (n = 14 with vs 17 without) in autumn and spring [99]. More than 60% and 85% of mothers of Somali origin had 25(OH)D <25 nmol/L in spring and autumn, respectively, compared to only one mother of Swedish origin in both seasons. The difference in 25(OH)D concentrations was not significantly different between mothers with and without a child with autism for either ethnicity. This study is limited, however, by its small sample size and the long time period since pregnancy (average age of more than 4 years). Furthermore, a lack of difference could be also attributed to the very low and narrow maternal 25(OH)D concentration among mothers of Somali origin which does not allow the relationship between vitamin D status of mothers and risk of ASD to be addressed.
To overcome these limitations, Whitehouse et al. (2013) investigated the relationship between maternal 25(OH)D concentrations at 18 weeks of pregnancy and autism phenotype in the offspring at ages 5, 8, 10, and 14 years follow ups [100]. The authors did not find any difference in 25(OH)D concentrations among mothers of children with and without clinical ASD (n = 929) while controlling for several variables (maternal education, socioeconomic status, maternal race, age at conception, maternal smoking, alcohol intake during pregnancy, parity, infant health and sex). It should be noted that only three males were clinically diagnosed with ASD at follow up and extrapolating these results to the broader ASD population should be done with caution. Maternal 25(OH)D concentrations were not associated with the total score and four out of five subscales of Autism Like Quotient (autistic-like traits) among 406 offspring. However, compared to children of mothers in the higher tertiles, children of mothers in the lower tertile (25(OH)D <45 nmol/L) had higher scores on attention switching subscale (OR 5.46, 95% CI, 1.29–23.05, p < 0.05) while controlling for several potential confounders, including season of blood collection. A limitation of this study is that only 14% of offspring from the original cohort completed the study. The high attrition rate was biased toward the loss of mothers with lower socioeconomic status. It was shown that total autistic like trait score was higher among offspring of mothers with low socioeconomic status and of mothers with low education levels. On the other hand, evidence shows that both lower socioeconomic status and lower maternal education are associated with lower vitamin D status [101,102].
Consistent with these findings, Whitehouse et al. (2012) and Morale et al. (2012) also showed that lower maternal 25(OH)D concentration in pregnancy (weeks 18 and 13.5, respectively) was associated with more language difficulties and mental and psychomotor outcomes in offspring, respectively [94,103]. While controlling for several variables, maternal 25(OH)D concentration <46 nmol/L was associated with significant language difficulties in the offspring at ages 5 and 10 years compared with those offspring of mothers with 25(OH)D concentrations >70 nmol/L [94]. Maternal 25(OH)D concentration >75 nmol/L in pregnancy was associated with increased mental and psychomotor scores in infants at 14 months compared to those with 25(OH)D concentrations <50 nmol/L [103].
By contrast, Gale et al. (2008) could not find any relationship between maternal 25(OH)D concentration during pregnancy and child’s intelligence and psychological health at ages 9 months and 9 years [104]. The inconsistent findings could be, in part, attributed to important methodological issues and the timing of blood collection during pregnancy. At the follow-up, the sample size was smaller (n = 178) and the attrition rate was higher (70%) in the Gale et al. study [104] than the Whitehouse et al. and Morales et al. studies (n = 412 and 50% and n = 1826 and 25%, respectively) [94,103]. Maternal 25(OH)D concentrations during pregnancy and infants’ neuropsychological health might have differed between the many lost to follow-up and this could have biased the results.
Furthermore, while the blood sampling was done in late pregnancy (week 32.6) in the Gale et al. study [104], blood samples were collected during weeks 18 and 13.5 ± 2.1 of pregnancy in the Whitehouse et al. and Morales et al. studies [94,103], respectively. These findings suggest that depending on the timing of the exposure to low vitamin D status, different brain areas might be affected and the consequence might be different neurodevelopment and cognitive outcomes in infants [105].

3.1.5. Vitamin D Intervention to Prevent ASD

We included all studies reporting using vitamin D supplements during or before pregnancy in relation to ASD in the offspring. We excluded those studies using vitamin D containing multivitamins or minerals only because those doses are relatively low and the effect of vitamin D per se cannot be determined from these interventions.
Our literature search identified only one study reporting on the effect of vitamin D supplementation during pregnancy on the recurrence rate of autism in newborn siblings [106] (Table 4). In an open-label prospective study, Stubbs, Henley and Green (2016) prescribed vitamin D at a daily dose of 5000 IU to mothers of children with ASD during pregnancy (majority during the second trimester, n = 19), 7000 IU during breastfeeding, and 1000 IU to their newborn infants during the first three year of life if they were not breastfed. The authors investigated the recurrence rate of autism in these children and compared it to other literature. The recurrence rate of autism was approximately four-fold less than that of other studies (5% vs. 20%). To note, this study had a small sample size and lacked a control arm.

3.2. Vitamin D-ASD-Related Areas Providing an Indication for both Primary and Secondary Prevention

In this section, we will synthesise the literature regarding a vitamin D-related measure—that is low vitamin D status in ASD patients—which can be a risk factor for ASD, and at the same time, a stage of ASD disease course.

Vitamin D Status in ASD Patients

We included all case-control studies of vitamin D status of individuals with ASD compared to healthy individuals, and participants of any age diagnosed with an ASD by either established diagnostic criteria, for example, DSM-IV or standardised established instruments.
Our literature search identified 16 studies reporting 25(OH)D concentrations in populations with ASD [78,79,81,107,108,109,110,111,112,113,114,115,116,117,118] (Table 5). Two of the 16 studies were excluded because either the full article was in Chinese [107], or cases were not compared with typically developing controls [119]. All 13 studies included in this review have been published over the past five years. Approximately half of the literature was from the Middle East [79,81,109,111,115,118], two from China [112,117], two from Europe [78,108], three from the US [113,114,116] and one from South America [110].
The number of cases ranged from 13 [115] to 254 [118]. Fernell et al. (2015) included siblings as controls [78], Saad et al. (2015) [111] and Kocovska et al. (2014) [108] included siblings, parents and typically developing children as controls, and the remaining studies included only non-family member typically developing children. Children included as controls were thoroughly examined for medical and/or mental and developmental conditions in three studies [81,111,112]. Controls were matched to cases on one or more attributes, including age, sex, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, season of birth, and/or season of blood collection. Season of blood collection was controlled in eight studies by either making adjustments in statistical analysis [108,113,114], performing all blood collections in one or two seasons [109,111,117], matching controls to cases on season of enrolment [79] or performing sub analysis for different seasons [112].
Of 14 studies, nine reported significant differences in 25(OH)D concentrations between cases and controls [78,81,108,109,110,111,112,117,118], one borderline difference [114], and four found no significant differences among study groups [79,113,115,116]. Tostes et al. (2012) reported that mean 25(OH)D concentration of children with ASD of different ethnic groups was lower by approximately 35 nmol/L than healthy age and sex matched controls (p < 0.001) [110]. Neither ethnicity nor season of blood sampling was considered in the statistical analysis.
Bener et al. (2014) investigated several lifestyle and biochemical risk factors in relation to autism [118]. Children with ASD had significantly lower 25(OH)D concentrations than healthy age, sex and ethnicity matched controls (46.0 ± 20.5 vs. 54.0 ± 21.0 nmol/L, respectively, p = 0.004). Circulating levels of 25(OH)D were inversely correlated with autism severity measured by Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS). Factors associated with autism were serum calcium, 25(OH)D, blood relative, BMI, physical activity, child order, and ferritin. Despite the large sample size of 254 in each group, this study had several limitations; (1) cases were selected from a cohort study and controls were not examined for autism manifestations; (2) potential covariates or confounders were not controlled for in statistical analysis despite significant differences in socio-demographic characteristics of cases and controls; and (3) season/month of blood sampling was not controlled for in statistical analysis.
Using siblings or family members as controls to minimize confounding by socio-demographic factors, Fernell et al. (2015) reported significantly lower mean 25(OH)D concentration at birth in autistic children of Scandinavian and other European origins than their non-ASD sibling controls [78]. On the other hand, the authors did not find any differences between cases and controls of Middle Eastern/African origin. The lack of association in Middle Eastern and African group may in part, be attributed to the small sample size and very low mean 25(OH)D concentrations in these two ethnic groups, 11.5 ± 7.1 and 7.0 ± 5.0 nmol/L, respectively. It should be also noted that season of birth was not significantly different between cases and controls in the latter ethnic groups, but it was significantly different across groups of Scandinavian and other European origins (10% vs. 35% of cases and controls in summer, respectively). Season of birth which corresponded to the season of blood sampling in this study was not controlled for in statistical analysis.
Kocovska et al. (2014) showed that ASD individuals had significantly lower 25(OH)D concentrations than their siblings, parents and healthy controls while adjusting for season of blood sampling (no one in ASD group had his/her blood sample drawn in winter) [108]. Furthermore, a larger proportion had 25(OH)D <50 nmol/L in cases than controls (88% in ASD group vs. 58% in sibling and parents each and 65% in healthy controls). A limitation to those studies including family members is that cases and controls cannot be matched on age and sex. Furthermore, inclusion of older siblings may confound the findings by younger parental age, and inclusion of apparently healthy younger siblings, on the other hand, may run the risk of including siblings with ASD who may develop ASD later in life in the control group [78,108]. For that reason, it is important that investigators examine family member controls more carefully for any autistic-like behaviour.
Examining both cases and controls for autistic-like features, Gong et al. (2013) reported a larger proportion of children with ASD having 25(OH)D concentrations <50 nmol/L as compared to healthy controls (58% vs. 33%, respectively p = 0.01) [112]. Autism severity—measured by Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS)—negatively correlated with 25(OH)D concentrations (r = −0.4, p < 0.001) while controlling for several covariates including age, sex, season, magnesium, BMI, calcium and phosphate. When vitamin D status of study groups was compared according to the four seasons, children with ASD had lower summer 25(OH)D concentrations than controls (p = 0.02). It should be noted that doing sub-analysis in studies with small sample size results in even smaller sample sizes for each group, which could compromise the validity of the latter findings. As a general rule, it’s preferable to control for season of blood sampling either in the statistical analysis or by limiting season/month of blood collection rather than doing this by subgroup analysis, especially in studies with a small starting sample size.
Meguid et al. (2010) [81] and Saad et al. (2015) [111] also thoroughly examined all children, including both cases and controls, for medical conditions and mental disorders, and reported that two groups of Egyptian children with ASD had significantly lower levels of 25(OH)D compared to age, sex and socio-economic status matched healthy controls. The season of birth was taken into account in both studies but no significant differences were found in either group. Mean 1,25(OH)2D and calcium concentrations was also lower in children with ASD than healthy controls [81], and serum 25(OH)D concentrations negatively correlated with CARS scores (r = −0.50, p < 0.0001) [111]. Children on the severe end of the spectrum had lower mean 25(OH)D concentration than those with mild/moderate ASD (30 ± 15 vs. 53 ± 21 nmol/L, respectively). To avoid seasonal variations in 25(OH)D concentrations, all blood samples in this study were drawn during same two months (May and June).
Similarly, Mostafa and Al-Ayadi (2012) collected all blood samples from Saudi Arabian children with ASD and healthy controls during summer months [109]. Children with ASD displayed significantly lower 25(OH)D concentration than healthy controls. While 40% of children with ASD had 25(OH)D concentrations <25 nmol/L, no one from control group had 25(OH)D concentrations in that range. Cases also had significantly higher anti-myelin-associated glycoprotein (anti-MAG) anti-autoantibodies than controls. Furthermore, 25(OH)D concentrations correlated negatively with CARS scores, (r = −0.84, p < 0.001) and anti-MAG anti-autoantibodies (r = −0.86, p < 0.001). Children on the severe end of the spectrum had significantly higher anti-MAG anti-autoantibodies levels than those with mild/moderate ASD. These findings point to a link between low vitamin D status, autoimmunity and autism.
In a recent large study from China, Feng et al. (2016) reported higher proportion of ASD children having 25(OH)D concentrations <25 nmol/L (13% vs. 0%, p = 0.03) and <75 nmol/L (71% vs. 62%, p = 0.03) than healthy typically developing controls [117]. To control for seasonal variation, the study lasted for six months; April—September corresponding to spring and summer months. To note, 25(OH)D concentrations of those entering the study in April could be different of those entering September due to a potentially limited sun exposure during winter months.
Neumeyer et al. (2013) [114] reported higher proportion of boys with ASD having 25(OH)D concentrations <80 nmol/L than healthy boys (77% vs. 37%, p = 0.02). Furthermore, cases had marginally lower serum 25(OH)D concentration than controls (67 ± 5 vs. 79 ± 4 nmol/L, p = 0.05). Despite comparable total energy intake and caloric intake from specific macronutrients among study groups, calcium and vitamin D intake was lower in cases than controls. Small sample size was a limitation of this study (n = 18) with the sample size calculated based on differences in bone mineral density rather than 25(OH)D concentrations.
Hashemzadeh et al. (2015) failed to show any difference in 25(OH)D concentrations between children with ASD aged 3–12 years (n = 13), and age and sex matched typically developing children (n = 14) [115]. This study had the smallest sample size, a limited statistical power to detect differences among groups. Three other studies that also failed to show any relationship had somewhat larger sample sizes (ranging from 54 to 71 per group) and controlled for several covariates [113] and matched controls to cases on several attributes [79,116]. Molloy et al. (2010) [113] included boys with catheters placed for tonsillectomies as controls. Children were suffering from some form of acute inflammation, which potentially had an effect on 25(OH)D levels [120,121].
Although Adams et al. (2011) reported statistically significant differences in several biomarkers indicative of increased oxidative stress, reduced capacity for detoxification and energy transport, and vitamin deficiency in children with ASD than controls, plasma 25(OH)D did not differ across study groups [116]. The lack of difference could in part be attributed to some methodological issues; neither cases nor controls were assessed for autism manifestations by researchers and confounders such as ethnicity, season of blood sampling, medical conditions and medication use were not controlled for in statistical analysis. Approximately half of the cases were on different medications including psychopharmaceuticals, central nerve system stimulants, anticonvulsants, gastrointestinal medication, asthma/allergies and insulin, and 11% of controls on either anti-inflammatory or anti-incontinence medications.
The study by Ugur et al. (2014) [79] was conducted in Turkey, where a national free vitamin D supplementation campaign has been implemented by government to eliminate vitamin D deficiency rickets [122]. The comparable levels of 25(OH)D among ASD cases and controls in this study could therefore be attributed to the nationwide vitamin D supplementation after initiation of ASD.
The cause and effect relationship from these case-control studies cannot be determined. Patients with ASD are likely to have medical conditions and use medications—affecting vitamin D metabolism or absorption—that have not been considered in these studies. Inflammation [120,121], gastrointestinal issues [123,124], antiepileptic drugs [125] and genetics are among these factors. Furthermore, children with ASD may receive less sunlight due to lower outdoor activity and have inadequate intake of vitamin D [114,118,126,127,128,129,130,131,132,133,134,135,136], i.e., reverse causality.
Along those lines, according to parental report, ASD children had significantly more sedentary behaviours than typically developing controls, a finding confirmed by Neumeyer et al. (2013) and Bener et al. (2014) [118]. Approximately 11% and 73% of ASD children and typically developing children had high physical activity levels, respectively [114], and fewer ASD children were exposed to sun than healthy controls (p = 0.002) [118].
Moreover, due to repetitive and restricted dietary behaviours, ASD children are at increased risk of low dietary vitamin D intake [128,129]. Evidence suggests that a large proportion of children with ASD do not meet vitamin D requirements [126,132,133,134,135,136]. Using three-day food records, Stewart et al. (2015) reported a probable risk of inadequate intake of six micronutrients in >40% of children with ASD, one of which was vitamin D [126]. Even though vitamin D intake was improved in response to vitamin D supplementation, it did not reach the adequate intake in 30%–40% of children. The scenario would be worse if more children of African American, mixed-race/ethnicity and less educated parents were enrolled because the socio-demographic characteristics of the study population was biased toward highly educated parents and those of white ethnicity who might have been more concerned about nutrition. Average vitamin D intake was 62% and 49% of dietary recommended intake (DRI) in boys with ASD aged 7–8 and 9–12 years, respectively [136]. Although these studies illustrated a large proportion of children with ASD not meeting the recommendations for vitamin D intake, they did not find any evidence that dietary intake of vitamin D in children with ASD differed from that of typically developing controls.
It has been argued that low dietary intake of vitamin D is a public health issue, and is not specific to children with ASD [131,137,138]. In a recent case-control study, Mari-Bausent et al. (2015) reported children with ASD having lower dietary vitamin D intake than healthy controls, though the difference was not statistically significant (79 ± 60 vs. 104 ± 97 IU/day, respectively, p = 0.06) [131]. A large proportion of both groups did not meet the dietary recommended intake for vitamin D (88% of children with ASD and 75% of typically developing children, p = 0.16), a finding confirmed by others [137,138].
However, several case-control studies have shown that children with ASD have lower vitamin D intakes than typically developing controls [114,128,129,130]. Emond et al. (2010) and Zimmer et al. (2012) reported that children with ASD had late introduction of solids after six months, were difficult to feed and very choosy from 15–54 months, had less varied diet from 24 months, and ate fewer food on average at older ages, all of which affected their nutritional status [128,129]. Both studies showed that children with ASD had significantly lower dietary intakes of vitamin D than controls. Daily vitamin D intake was 199 ± 176 IU and 320 ± 119 IU in ASD and typically developing children, respectively (p < 0.05) [129]. More children with ASD did not also meet the estimated average requirement (EAR) for vitamin D intake than healthy controls (79% vs. 55%, p = 0.01) [130], and more selective eaters with ASD were at increased risk for inadequate intake (58% vs. 5%, p = 0.01) [129]. Thus, low vitamin D status could be a risk factor for ASD if it occurs during critical period of development, or alternatively, could be a condition that appears as the disease progresses.

3.3. Vitamin D-ASD-Related Areas Providing an Indication for Secondary Prevention

Here, we will review the literature regarding a vitamin D-related measure—that is the treatment of low vitamin D status when ASD has already occurred—which can be considered as a disease modifier, and may reduce the symptoms of ASD in patients.

Vitamin D Intervention to Treat ASD

We included all studies reporting using vitamin D supplements among populations with ASD. We excluded those studies using vitamin D containing multivitamins or minerals only because those doses are relatively low and the effect of vitamin D per se cannot be determined from these interventions.
Our literature search identified six studies reporting on the effect of vitamin D supplementation on autism symptoms (Table 4); one clinical quality assurance practice [119], one case report [139], three open label intervention trials [111,117,141], and one randomised controlled trial [140]. The full text for one article was not available at the time of writing this review, as such the critical appraisal could not be employed and the data presented here are based on the abstract [140]. We found no randomised placebo controlled trials, but three ongoing studies from Egypt (NCT02550912), Ireland (NCT02508922) and New Zealand (ACTRN12615000144516). Four studies were among children with ASD [111,117,139,140], one among developmentally delayed children with and without ASD [141], and one in adult patients with ASD and other psychiatric disorders [119]. Humble et al. (2010), in a clinical quality assurance project, reported a considerable improvement in psychosis or depression with the effective treatment of vitamin D deficiency in several patients. In their clinical practice they used 1600–4000 IU vitamin D3 daily or 35,000–70,000 IU vitamin D2 once weekly [119]. Autistic and schizophrenic patients had the lowest median 25(OH)D concentrations compared to other psychiatric disorders, 32 (25th–75th percentile, 23–39) nmol/L and 35 (24–53) nmol/L, respectively, vs. >40 in other patients. No conclusion can be drawn from this study because it was not designed to assess the efficacy of vitamin D on autism and it was part of clinical practice, no placebo or control arm was included, different doses and types of vitamin D was used, and levels of 25(OH)D after treatment was not reported.
In a recent case report, Jia et al. (2015) reported that shifting serum 25(OH)D concentration in a 32 month old child with ASD from 31 nmol/L to 203 nmol/L after two months of high dose vitamin D3 supplementation (150,000 IU per month administered intramuscularly plus 400 IU per day orally) improved autistic core symptoms [139]. Scores on Autism Behaviour Checklist, Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) and Severity of Illness of Clinical Global Impression increased from 80, 35 and 6 at the baseline to 39, 28 and 4 at the follow up, respectively. The findings from this single patient cannot be generalised to all autistic patients but it does encourage further research in this area. Following on from this case report, Feng and colleagues (2016) assigned 37 ASD children from a pool of 215 children with ASD (25(OH)D <75 nmol/L) to receive the same dose of vitamin D supplementation for three months [117]. The authors reported a significant improvement in scores on CARS (total) and Autism Behaviour Checklist (total and all subscales apart from sensory scores), which was more pronounced in 3 year old or younger children than older children.
Saad et al. (2015), in an open label trial, assigned 106 children with ASD and 25(OH)D concentrations <75 nmol/L to receive daily 300 IU/Kg not exceeding daily 5000 IU for three months. This study was part of a case-control study investigating differences in 25(OH)D concentrations among autistic children and healthy age and sex matched controls (previously discussed). Symptoms of ASD were measured using CARS and Aberrant Behaviour Checklist (ABC). CARS scores decreased by 3.5–6.5 points in all 16 patients with final 25(OH)D >100 nmol/L, by 1.5–4.5 points in 31 out of 49 patients with 25(OH)D levels 75–97.5 nmol/L and no improvements in all 18 patients with 25(OH)D levels <75 nmol/L had improvements. With the exception of inappropriate speech subscale of ABC, all other measures (irritability, stereotypic behaviour, social withdrawal and hyperactivity) improved significantly after treatment. This study had a relatively large sample size and also limited seasonal effects by drawing all blood samples during the summer months, though the baseline and follow-up levels of 25(OH)D were not reported. Other limitations of this study were its drop-out rate of approximately 22% and the lack of a placebo or comparison arm.
In another study from Turkey, however, despite improvement in scores on ABC and Denver II in both treatment and control groups, Ucuz et al. (2015) failed to show any significant differences across groups [141]. To note, the improvement was more pronounced in those receiving vitamin D supplement. The authors recruited children with developmental delay with and without ASD, divided them into two groups; those with 25(OH)D concentration <50 nmol/L who received daily 5000 IU vitamin D supplement for one month and daily 400 IU vitamin D for further two months if 25(OH)D was not corrected at one month follow up, and ≥50 nmol/L who did not receive vitamin D supplements, and performed subcategory analysis for children with developmental delay with ASD and without ASD. This study had a small sample size of <11 participants per group. Furthermore, children were not randomly assigned to study groups and neither baseline nor follow up 25(OH)D concentrations were reported. It should be noted that the findings from this study cannot be extrapolated to children with ASD only. The brain chemistry of children with ASD may differ from that of children with developmental delay [142], and as such the response to vitamin D supplement—that may affect brain chemistry [143]—could vary.
In a randomised controlled trial including only children with ASD, Azzam et al. (2015), similarly, reported comparable improvements in scores on CARS, social IQ, and Autism Treatment Evaluation Checklist in both vitamin D supplemented (six months) and control groups [140]. Although no quality appraisal can be performed for this study, what is clear is that this study had a small sample size and lacked a placebo arm. In summary, although these early interventional studies provided encouraging results, no firm conclusions can be drawn until randomised placebo-controlled trials with sufficient sample size are undertaken.

4. Mechanistic Pathways

Vitamin D receptors (VDR) and enzymes involved in vitamin D metabolism have been identified in several regions of the brain including neurons and glial cells [144]. VDR has been shown to be present early in development, to increase during development, and to be present in the adult brain—all indicative of a role for vitamin D in the developing and adult brain [145]. Low vitamin D status has been implicated in pathophysiology of ASD in several ways. It has been hypothesised that ASD is a combination of both organ specific physiologic and systematic abnormalities such as de novo gene mutations, oxidative stress, impaired detoxification system, inflammation, immune dysregulation, abnormal neurotrophic factor and neurotransmitter levels, and seizures, at least in a subset of individuals with ASD [8]. Mounting evidence suggests that low vitamin D status is involved in the aetiology of the mentioned abnormalities [105].
Autism is considered to be driven genetically [146,147], though only small percentage of cases are clearly linked to genetic causes [22]. The only study linking vitamin D metabolic gene variants to ASD risk is by Schmidt et al. [148]. The authors illustrated that the risk for ASD was increased in children inheriting the AA genotype of the GC gene (D binding protein), the GG genotype of the CYP2R1 gene (a catalyst enzyme involved in the transformation of vitamin D to 25(OH)D), and paternal Taql and Bsml genotypes of the VDR gene, highlighting the possible aetiological role of low vitamin D in ASD.All of these results support an association between vitamin D status and ASD.
Vitamin D regulates cell profilation and differentiation and can protect the genome from daily life insults such as oxidative stress and toxins (for a comprehensive review refer to [149]). Treatment with vitamin D has been shown to decrease 8—hydroxy–2—deoxyguanosin, a marker of endogenous oxidative damage to DNA [150], increase Bax, a protein coding gene involved in apoptosis [150], regulates poly-ADP-ribose polymerase activity in the DNA damage response pathway [151], stabilize chromosomal structure and prevent double strand breaks [152].
There are presently several lines of evidence indicating oxidative stress and mitochondrial dysfunction are prevalent in individuals with ASD [3,10,11,12,13,14,15,16]. Recent evidence shows that children with ASD have lower levels of total glutathion and glutathione peroxidase (an enzyme involved in antioxidant defense and detoxification), higher levels of oxidised glutathione and F2—isoprostane (a marker of oxidative stress and lipid peroxidation) and reduced capacity for methylation than typically developing children [16,153]. Oxidative stress and reduced redox/antioxidant capacity in cerebellum and temporal cortex have been shown to have functional consequences for chronic inflammatory response, mitochondrial dysfunction and protein and DNA damage [3], and in peripheral tissue have been associated with anti-neuronal positivity [16] and clinical features of autism [15]. Evidence shows that 25(OH)D concentrations correlate significantly positively with glutathione levels in healthy adult populations [154], and low vitamin D status is associated with increased inflammatory, oxidative and endothelial activation biomarkers in obese individuals [155]. It has been documented that treatment with vitamin D has a protective effect on persistent biochemical features comparable to those reported clinically in autistic patients and ameliorates neurotoxicity, inflammation and DNA damage in propionic acid-intoxicated rats [156].
Exposure to heavy metals and impaired detoxification system, another abnormality in a subset of individuals with ASD, have also been involved in the aeitiology of ASD [157]. Alabdali et al. (2014) reported autistic children having higher levels of mercury and lead than healthy controls, and lower levels of glutathione-transferase and vitamin E in plasma [157]. Using cell culture models, Shinpo et al. (2000) and Garcion et al. (1999) indicated that pretreatment of cells exposed to toxicants with 1,25(OH)2D attenuated reactive oxygen species and nitric oxide, and enhanced intracellular gamma-glytamyl transpeptidase (an enzyme involved in glutathione synthesis) and glutathione in the intracellular pool [158,159].
Furthermore, evidence suggests that children with ASD have elevated levels of pro-inflammatory cytokines including, interleukin 6 (IL–6), tumour necrosis factor alpha (TNF–α), and interferon gamma (IFN-γ) in different tissues [3,4,5,6,7]. The activation of microglia, the unique resident cells of the central nervous system, is the prominent feature of autism [160] and is associated with increased several pro-inflammatory cytokines [161]. It is well known that vitamin D has an effect on immune system and can directly affect immune cells. For example, vitamin D metabolites have been shown to decrease the secretion of IL-6 and TNF-α [162], enhance the expression of anti-inflammatory cytokines such as interleukin 10 (IL–10) from activated B cells [163], and to direct dendritic cells (DCs) toward a more tolerogenic state [164].
Autism is also considered as an autoimmune disease in a subset of individuals. Elevated autoantibodies in the brain, specifically anti-myelin basic protein antibodies have been reported in autism [9]. Mostafa and Al-Ayadi reported significantly higher proportion of children with ASD having anti-MAG antiautoantibodies, anti-ganglioside M1, anti-neuronal and anti-nuclear antibodies than typically developing controls, that positively correlated with the severity of autism symptoms (CARS scores) [109,165,166,167]. Family history of autoimmune diseases was reported to be higher in children with ASD than controls [166,168]. Using a genome-wide data set of ASD and autoimmune disorders, Multiple Sclerosis (MS) was identified to be significantly associated with ASD [169]. Both conditions have been characterised by immunological abnormalities and dysfunctional myelination, at least in a subset of individuals [170,171]. Furthermore, anti-MAG antiautoantibodies correlated negatively with serum 25(OH)D concentrations (r = −0.86, p < 0.001) [109], and anti-nuclear antibody positive healthy controls were significantly more likely to be deficient in vitamin D than antibody negative healthy controls (71% vs. 22%, respectively, p = 0.003) [172]. Vitamin D has also been shown to modify the expression of several genes involved in axogenesis and myelination [173]. These findings suggest an important role of vitamin D in autoantibody production and ASD pathogenesis, perhaps similar to other autoimmune diseases, such as MS and systemic lupus erythematosus [172,174].
Evidence suggests that vitamin D has a neuroprotectory effect through its neurotrophic and immunomodulatory properties. Neurotrophins are involved in the development, maintenance, survival and synapsis of neurons [105]. Abnormal levels of several neurotrophic factors such as nerve growth factor (NGF) and brain-derived neurotrophic factors (BDNF) have been reported in subset of individuals with ASD [175,176]. Vitamin D analogues have been shown to upregulate NGF in cultured glial cell and embryonic hippocampal cells [177,178], to decrease the percentage of cultured hippocampal cells undergoing mitosis [178], and to decrease BDNF and concommitantly to improve memory in postmenopausal women [179].
Multiple lines of evidence suggest an involvement of dysregulated neurotransmitter systems (serotonergic, oxytocinergic and dopaminergic systems) in autism. These systems play key roles in neurotransmission, brain maturation, cortical organisation and behaviours (including social behaviour and repetitive behaviour) [180,181,182,183,184]. Lower levels of plasma oxytocin [185] and abnormal serotonin concentrations in the brain and tissues outside the blood-brain barrier have been reported in populations with ASD [186,187]. While the binding of brain serotonin transporter was significantly lower in adults with high functioning autism than healthy controls, the binding of brain dopamine transporter was significantly higher in autistic patients [184]. Recently, two ASD associated independent gene variants, including a gene variants regulating STX1 phosphorylation (regulates neurotransmitter transporter) and dopamine transporter, that alter dopamine transporter function have been identified [188]. Vitamin D receptors have been identified in dopamine neurones in humans substantia nigra [145], and vitamin D response elements on genes involved in serotonin and oxytocin synthesis [189]. Pre-treatment with calcitriol has been shown to protect rats’ striatum and accumbens from methamphetamine-induced reduction in dopamine and serotonin [143].
Finally, the above mentioned disorders may contribute to the aetiology of epilepsy, which is highly prevalent in populations with ASD [190]. According to retrospective follow up studies, 37% and 25% of the individuals with autism exhibited epilepsy and epileptic seizures, respectively [191,192]. Infants of mothers taking antiepileptic drugs during pregnancy have been shown to be at increased risk of being diagnosed with ASD and having low 25(OH)D concentrations [193]. Vitamin D and its analogues have been shown to increase seizure threshold, potentiate anticonvulsant activity of antiepileptic drugs, and to decrease the severity of seizures in animal studies [194,195,196]. Correction of vitamin D deficiency in patients with seizures, decreased the frequency of seizures by 30%–40% [197,198]. Vitamin D exerts its roles, in part, through its antioxidant and anti-inflammatory effects and also through its inhibitory effect on Ca2+ influx in the brain. Calcitriol upregulates the expression of calcium binding proteins (calbindin and parvalbumin) and inhibits the expression of L-type Ca2+ channels [199]. It should be noted that at the same time, antiepileptic drugs impair vitamin D and calcium metabolism, leading to reduced levels of vitamin D and calcium [125], and these deficiencies can, on the other hand, exacerbate the condition.

5. Summary

Low vitamin D status in utero, postnatal and in early childhood has been hypothesised as a risk factor for neurodevelopmental disorders, specifically ASD. Animal and human cellular, biological, and physiologic studies have provided compelling evidence for numerous roles of vitamin D in various body processes, some of which are involved in the pathobiology of ASD. Our literature review identified a large number of observational studies but very few intervention trials investigating the relationship between vitamin D and ASD. Conclusions are not yet possible due to the inconsistent results, different methodological approaches employed, and very few trials in the current literature. However, there are some indications that early exposure to inadequate vitamin D may interact with other factors and contribute to the aetiology of autism, low vitamin D status might be highly prevalent in populations with ASD, and intervention with vitamin D might be beneficial in reducing autism symptoms among those who have ASD. Therefore, there is an urgent need for randomised controlled trials of vitamin D in populations genetically predisposed to ASD and in populations with ASD to confirm these findings and to generate evidence-based clinical recommendations for the prevention of ASD and management of ASD symptoms. Until better data are available, health care providers and researchers are advised to consider vitamin D-related factors as potential preventive and disease-modifying measures for ASD.

Acknowledgments

No grants have been received in support of writing this review. The costs to publish this review in open access have been covered by Massey University.

Author Contributions

H.M. conceived the manuscript idea, H.M. researched and drafted the manuscript, all other authors critically reviewed the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. New Zealand Guidelines Group. What does ASD look like? In A resource to Help Identify Autism Spectrum Disorder; New Zealand Guidelines Group: Wellington, New Zealand, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5; American Psychiatric Publishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  3. Rose, S.; Melnyk, S.; Pavliv, O.; Bai, S.; Nick, T.G.; Frye, R.E.; James, S.J. Evidence of oxidative damage and inflammation associated with low glutathione redox status in the autism brain. Transl. Psychiatry 2012, 2, e134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Ashwood, P.; Krakowiak, P.; Hertz-Picciotto, I.; Hansen, R.; Pessah, I.; Van de Water, J. Elevated plasma cytokines in Autism Spectrum Disorders provide evidence of immune dysfunction and are associated with impaired behavioral outcome. Brain Behav. Immun. 2011, 25, 40–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Chez, M.G.; Dowling, T.; Patel, P.B.; Khanna, P.; Kominsky, M. Elevation of tumor necrosis factor-alpha in cerebrospinal fluid of autistic children. Pediatr. Neurol. 2007, 36, 361–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Napolioni, V.; Ober-Reynolds, B.; Szelinger, S.; Corneveaux, J.J.; Pawlowski, T.; Ober-Reynolds, S.; Kirwan, J.; Persico, A.M.; Melmed, R.D.; Craig, D.W.; et al. Plasma cytokine profiling in sibling pairs discordant for Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Neuroinflamm. 2013, 10, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Molloy, C.A.; Morrow, A.L.; Meinzen-Derr, J.; Schleifer, K.; Dienger, K.; Manning-Courtney, P.; Altaye, M.; Wills-Karp, M. Elevated cytokine levels in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Neuroimmunol. 2006, 172, 198–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Rossignol, D.A.; Frye, R.E. Evidence linking oxidative stress, mitochondrial dysfunction, and inflammation in the brain of individuals with autism. Front. Physiol. 2014, 5, 150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Singh, V.K. Phenotypic expression of autoimmune autistic disorder (AAD): A major subset of autism. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry 2009, 21, 148–161. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  10. Ming, X.; Stein, T.P.; Brimacombe, M.; Johnson, W.G.; Lambert, G.H.; Wagner, G.C. Increased excretion of a lipid peroxidation biomarker in autism. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 2005, 73, 379–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Giulivi, C.; Zhang, Y.; Omanska-Klusek, A.; Ross-Inta, C.; Wong, S.; Hertz-Picciotto, I.; Tassone, F.; Pessah, I.N. Mitochondrial dysfunction in autism. JAMA 2010, 304, 2389–2396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Napoli, E.; Wong, S.; Hertz-Picciotto, I.; Giulivi, C. Deficits in bioenergetics and impaired immune response in granulocytes from children with autism. Pediatrics 2014, 133, e1405–e1410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Rose, S.; Frye, R.E.; Slattery, J.; Wynne, R.; Tippett, M.; Pavliv, O.; Melnyk, S.; James, S.J. Oxidative stress induces mitochondrial dysfunction in a subset of autism lymphoblastoid cell lines in a well-matched case control cohort. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e85436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Melnyk, S.; Fuchs, G.J.; Schulz, E.; Lopez, M.; Kahler, S.G.; Fussell, J.J.; Bellando, J.; Pavliv, O.; Rose, S.; Seidel, L.; et al. Metabolic imbalance associated with methylation dysregulation and oxidative damage in children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012, 42, 367–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Ghezzo, A.; Visconti, P.; Abruzzo, P.M.; Bolotta, A.; Ferreri, C.; Gobbi, G.; Malisardi, G.; Manfredini, S.; Marini, M.; Nanetti, L.; et al. Oxidative stress and erythrocyte membrane alterations in children with autism: Correlation with clinical features. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e66418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Mostafa, G.A.; El-Hadidi, E.S.; Hewedi, D.H.; Abdou, M.M. Oxidative stress in egyptian children with autism: Relation to autoimmunity. J. Neuroimmunol. 2010, 219, 114–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Chakrabarti, S.; Fombonne, E. Pervasive Developmental Disorders in preschool children. JAMA 2001, 285, 3093–3099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Baio, J. Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders—Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 Sites, United States, 2008; Morbidity and mortality weekly report; Surveillance summaries; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention: Atlanta, GA, USA, 2012; Volume 61, pp. 1–19. [Google Scholar]
  19. Ghanizadeh, A. A preliminary study on screening prevalence of pervasive developmental disorder in schoolchildren in iran. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2008, 38, 759–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Kogan, M.D.; Blumberg, S.J.; Schieve, L.A.; Boyle, C.A.; Perrin, J.M.; Ghandour, R.M.; Singh, G.K.; Strickland, B.B.; Trevathan, E.; van Dyck, P.C. Prevalence of parent-reported diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder among children in the us, 2007. Pediatrics 2009, 124, 1395–1403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Hansen, S.N.; Schendel, D.E.; Parner, E.T. Explaining the increase in the prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders: The proportion attributable to changes in reporting practices. JAMA Pediatr. 2015, 169, 56–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Schaaf, C.P.; Zoghbi, H.Y. Solving the autism puzzle a few pieces at a time. Neuron 2011, 70, 806–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Cannell, J.J. Autism and vitamin D. Med. Hypotheses 2008, 70, 750–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. DeLuca, H.F. Overview of general physiologic features and functions of vitamin D. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2004, 80, 1689S–1696S. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  25. Holick, M.F.; MacLaughlin, J.A.; Clark, M.B.; Holick, S.A.; Potts, J.T.; Anderson, R.R.; Blank, I.H.; Parrish, J.A.; Elias, P. Photosynthesis of previtamin D3 in human skin and the physiologic consequences. Science 1980, 210, 203–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Holick, M.F. Vitamin D deficiency. N. Engl. J. Med. 2007, 357, 266–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Clements, M.R.; Davies, M.; Hayes, M.E.; Hlckey, C.D.; Lumb, G.A.; Mawer, E.B.; Adams, P.H. The role of 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D in the mechanism of acquired vitamin d deficiency. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf.) 1992, 37, 17–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Zerwekh, J.E. Blood biomarkers of vitamin D status. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 87, 1087S–1091S. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  29. Ministry of Health and Cancer Society of New Zealand. Consensus Statement on Vitamin D and Sun Exposure in New Zealand; Health, M.O., Ed.; Ministry of Health: Wellington, New Zealand, 2012.
  30. Holick, M.F. Vitamin D: A D-lightful health perspective. Nutr. Rev. 2008, 66, S182–S194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Kočovská, E.; Fernell, E.; Billstedt, E.; Minnis, H.; Gillberg, C. Vitamin D and autism: Clinical review. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 33, 1541–1550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Gentile, I.; Zappulo, E.; Militerni, R.; Pascotto, A.; Borgia, G.; Bravaccio, C. Etiopathogenesis of Autism Spectrum Disorders: Fitting the pieces of the puzzle together. Med. Hypotheses 2013, 81, 26–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Elsabbagh, M.; Divan, G.; Koh, Y.J.; Kim, Y.S.; Kauchali, S.; Marcin, C.; Montiel-Nava, C.; Patel, V.; Paula, C.S.; Wang, C.; et al. Global prevalence of autism and other Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Autism Res. 2012, 5, 160–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Wan, Y.; Hu, Q.; Li, T.; Jiang, L.; Du, Y.; Feng, L.; Wong, J.C.-M.; Li, C. Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders among children in China: A systematic review. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 2013, 25, 70–80. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  35. Wilson, C.E.; Gillan, N.; Spain, D.; Robertson, D.; Roberts, G.; Murphy, C.M.; Maltezos, S.; Zinkstok, J.; Johnston, K.; Dardani, C.; et al. Comparison of ICD-10R, DSM-IV-TR and DSM-5 in an adult Autism Spectrum Disorder diagnostic clinic. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2013, 43, 2515–2525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Zhang, X.; Ji, C.Y. Autism and mental retardation of young children in china. Biomed. Environ. Sci. 2005, 18, 334–340. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  37. Baird, G.; Charman, T.; Baron-Cohen, S.; Cox, A.; Swettenham, J.; Wheelwright, S.; Drew, A. A screening instrument for autism at 18 months of age: A 6-year follow- up study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2000, 39, 694–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Chakrabarti, S.; Fombonne, E. Pervasive Developmental Disorders in preschool children: Confirmation of high prevalence. Am. J. Psychiatry 2005, 162, 1133–1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Arvidsson, T.; Danielsson, B.; Forsberg, P.; Gillberg, C.; Johansson, M.; Kjellgren, G. Autism in 3–6-year-old children in a suburb of goteborg, sweden. Autism 1997, 1, 163–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kadesjö, B.; Gillberg, C.; Hagberg, B. Brief report: Autism and asperger syndrome in seven-year-old children: A total population study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1999, 29, 327–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Mattila, M.-L.; Kielinen, M.; Linna, S.-L.; Jussila, K.; Ebeling, H.; Bloigu, R.; Joseph, R.M.; Moilanen, I. Autism Spectrum Disorders according to DSM-IV-TR and comparison with DSM-5 draft criteria: An epidemiological study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2011, 50, 583–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Eapen, V.; Mabrouk, A.A.; Zoubeidi, T.; Yunis, F. Prevalence of Pervasive Developmental Disorders in preschool children in the uae. J. Trop. Pediatr. 2007, 53, 202–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Samadi, S.A.; Mahmoodizadeh, A.; McConkey, R. A national study of the prevalence of autism among five-year-old children in Iran. Autism 2012, 16, 5–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Lejarraga, H.; Menendez, A.M.; Menzano, E.; Guerra, L.; Biancato, S.; Pianelli, P.; Del Pino, M.; Fattore, M.J.; Contreras, M.M. Screening for developmental problems at primary care level: A field programme in San Isidro, Argentina. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2008, 22, 180–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Kawamura, Y.; Takahashi, O.; Ishii, T. Reevaluating the incidence of Pervasive Developmental Disorders: Impact of elevated rates of detection through implementation of an integrated system of screening in Toyota, Japan. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2008, 62, 152–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Baxter, A.J.; Brugha, T.S.; Erskine, H.E.; Scheurer, R.W.; Vos, T.; Scott, J.G. The epidemiology and global burden of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Psychol. Med. 2015, 45, 601–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Grant, W.B.; Soles, C.M. Epidemiologic evidence supporting the role of maternal vitamin d deficiency as a risk factor for the development of infantile autism. Dermatoendocrinology 2009, 1, 223–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Piirainen, T.; Laitinen, K.; Isolauri, E. Impact of national fortification of fluid milks and margarines with vitamin D on dietary intake and serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D concentration in 4-year-old children. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2006, 61, 123–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Tylavsky, F.A.; Cheng, S.; Lyytikäinen, A.; Viljakainen, H.; Lamberg-Allardt, C. Strategies to improve vitamin D status in northern european children: Exploring the merits of vitamin D fortification and supplementation. J. Nutr. 2006, 136, 1130–1134. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  50. Laaksi, I.T.; Ruohola, J.P.S.; Ylikomi, T.J.; Auvinen, A.; Haataja, R.I.; Pihlajamaki, H.K.; Tuohimaa, P.J. Vitamin D fortification as public health policy: Significant improvement in vitamin D status in young finnish men. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2006, 60, 1035–1038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. McAree, T.; Jacobs, B.; Manickavasagar, T.; Sivalokanathan, S.; Brennan, L.; Bassett, P.; Rainbow, S.; Blair, M. Vitamin D deficiency in pregnancy—Still a public health issue. Mater. Child. Nutr. 2013, 9, 23–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Pena, H.R.; de Lima, M.C.; Brandt, K.G.; de Antunes, M.M.; da Silva, G.A. Influence of preeclampsia and gestational obesity in maternal and newborn levels of vitamin D. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 2015, 15, 112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Saraf, R.; Morton, S.M.; Camargo, C.A., Jr.; Grant, C.C. Global summary of maternal and newborn vitamin D status—A systematic review. Mater. Child. Nutr. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Ng, W.; Ikeda, S. Use of sun-protective items by Japanese pedestrians: A cross-sectional observational study. Arch. Dermatol. 2011, 147, 1167–1170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Brito, A.; Cori, H.; Olivares, M.; Fernanda Mujica, M.; Cediel, G.; Lopez de Romana, D. Less than adequate vitamin D status and intake in latin America and the Caribbean:A problem of unknown magnitude. Food Nutr. Bull. 2013, 34, 52–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Xiao, J.P.; Zang, J.; Pei, J.J.; Xu, F.; Zhu, Y.; Liao, X.P. Low maternal vitamin D status during the second trimester of pregnancy: A cross-sectional study in Wuxi, China. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0117748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Kelishadi, R.; Sharifi-Ghazvini, F.; Poursafa, P.; Mehrabian, F.; Farajian, S.; Yousefy, H.; Movahedian, M.; Sharifi-Ghazvini, S. Determinants of hypovitaminosis D in pregnant women and their newborns in a sunny region. Int. J. Endocrinol. 2013, 2013, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Al Anouti, F.; Thomas, J.; Abdel-Wareth, L.; Rajah, J.; Grant, W.B.; Haq, A. Vitamin D deficiency and sun avoidance among university students at Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates. Dermatoendocrinology 2011, 3, 235–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Samadi, S.A.; McKonkey, R. Autism in developing countries: Lessons from Iran. Autism Res. Treat. 2011, 2011, 145359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Grant, W.B.; Cannell, J.J. Autism prevalence in the united states with respect to solar UV-B doses: An ecological study. Dermatoendocrinology 2013, 5, 159–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Magnusson, C.; Rai, D.; Goodman, A.; Lundberg, M.; Idring, S.; Svensson, A.; Koupil, I.; Serlachius, E.; Dalman, C. Migration and Autism Spectrum Disorder: Population-based study. Br. J. Psychiatry 2012, 201, 109–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Maimburg, R.D.; Vaeth, M. Perinatal risk factors and infantile autism. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2006, 114, 257–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Keen, D.V.; Reid, F.D.; Arnone, D. Autism, ethnicity and maternal immigration. Br. J. Psychiatry 2010, 196, 274–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Hultman, C.M.; Sparen, P.; Cnattingius, S. Perinatal risk factors for infantile autism. Epidemiology (Camb. Mass.) 2002, 13, 417–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Lauritsen, M.B.; Pedersen, C.B.; Mortensen, P.B. Effects of familial risk factors and place of birth on the risk of autism: A nationwide register-based study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2005, 46, 963–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Williams, K.; Helmer, M.; Duncan, G.W.; Peat, J.K.; Mellis, C.M. Perinatal and maternal risk factors for Autism Spectrum Disorders in new South Wales, Australia. Child. Care. Health Dev. 2008, 34, 249–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Haglund, N.G.S.; Källén, K.B.M. Risk factors for autism and asperger syndrome: Perinatal factors and migration. Autism 2010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Croen, L.A.; Grether, J.K.; Selvin, S. Descriptive epidemiology of autism in a California population: Who is at risk? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2002, 32, 217–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Becerra, T.A.; von Ehrenstein, O.S.; Heck, J.E.; Olsen, J.; Arah, O.A.; Jeste, S.S.; Rodriguez, M.; Ritz, B. Autism Spectrum Dsiorders and race, ethnicity, and nativity: A population-based study. Pediatrics 2014, 134, e63–e71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. van der Ven, E.; Termorshuizen, F.; Laan, W.; Breetvelt, E.J.; van Os, J.; Selten, J.P. An incidence study of diagnosed autism-spectrum disorders among immigrants to The Netherlands. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2013, 128, 54–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorder among children aged 8 years—Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. MMWR Surveill. Summ. 2014, 63, 1–21.
  72. Croen, L.A.; Najjar, D.V.; Fireman, B.; Grether, J.K. Maternal and paternal age and risk of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2007, 161, 334–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Tek, S.; Landa, R. Differences in autism symptoms between minority and non-minority toddlers. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012, 42, 1967–1973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Mazumdar, S.; Liu, K.Y.; Susser, E.; Bearman, P. The disappearing seasonality of autism conceptions in California. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e41265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Zerbo, O.; Iosif, A.-M.; Delwiche, L.; Walker, C.; Hertz-Picciotto, I. Month of conception and risk of autism. Epidemiology (Camb. Mass.) 2011, 22, 469–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Hebert, K.J.; Miller, L.L.; Joinson, C.J. Association of autistic spectrum disorder with season of birth and conception in a UK cohort. Autism Res. 2010, 3, 185–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Atladottir, H.O.; Parner, E.T.; Schendel, D.; Dalsgaard, S.; Thomsen, P.H.; Thorsen, P. Variation in incidence of neurodevelopmental disorders with season of birth. Epidemiology (Camb. Mass.) 2007, 18, 240–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Fernell, E.; Bejerot, S.; Westerlund, J.; Miniscalco, C.; Simila, H.; Eyles, D.; Gillberg, C.; Humble, M.B. Autism Spectrum Disorder and low vitamin D at birth: A sibling control study. Mol. Autism 2015, 6, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Uğur, Ç.; Gürkan, C.K. Serum vitamin D and folate levels in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2014, 8, 1641–1647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Maimburg, R.D.; Bech, B.H.; Væth, M.; Møller-Madsen, B.; Olsen, J. Neonatal jaundice, autism, and other disorders of psychological development. Pediatrics 2010, 126, 872–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Meguid, N.A.; Hashish, A.F.; Anwar, M.; Sidhom, G. Reduced serum levels of 25-hydroxy and 1,25-dihydroxy vitamin D in egyptian children with autism. J. Altern. Complement. Med. (New York NY) 2010, 16, 641–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Lee, L.C.; Newschaffer, C.J.; Lessler, J.T.; Lee, B.K.; Shah, R.; Zimmerman, A.W. Variation in season of birth in singleton and multiple births concordant for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2008, 22, 172–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Kolevzon, A.; Weiser, M.; Gross, R.; Lubin, G.; Knobler, H.Y.; Schmeidler, J.; Silverman, J.M.; Reichenberg, A. Effects of season of birth on Autism Spectrum Disorders: Fact or fiction? Am. J. Psychiatry 2006, 163, 1288–1290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Yeates-Frederikx, M.H.M.; Nijman, H.; Logher, E.; Merckelbach, H.L.G.J. Birth patterns in mentally retarded autistic patients. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2000, 30, 257–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Stevens, M.C.; Fein, D.; Waterhouse, L.H. Season of birth effects in autism. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2000, 22, 399–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Landau, E.C.; Cicchetti, D.V.; Klin, A.; Volkmar, F.R. Season of birth in autism: A fiction revisited. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1999, 29, 385–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Barak, Y.; Ring, A.; Sulkes, J.; Gabbay, U.; Elizur, A. Season of birth and autistic disorder in israel. Am. J. Psychiatry 1995, 152, 798–800. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  88. Mouridsen, S.E.; Nielsen, S.; Rich, B.; Isager, T. Season of birth in infantile autism and other types of childhood psychoses. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 1994, 25, 31–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Bolton, P.; Pickles, A.; Harrington, R.; Macdonald, H.; Rutter, M. Season of birth: Issues, approaches and findings for autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1992, 33, 509–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Gillberg, C. Do children with autism have march birthdays? Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1990, 82, 152–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Tanoue, Y.; Oda, S.; Asano, F.; Kawashima, K. Epidemiology of infantile autism in southern Ibaraki, Japan: Differences in prevalence in birth cohorts. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1988, 18, 155–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Konstantareas, M.M.; Hauser, P.; Lennox, C.; Homatidis, S. Season of birth in infantile autism. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 1986, 17, 53–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  93. Kanan, R.M.; Al Saleh, Y.M.; Fakhoury, H.M.; Adham, M.; Aljaser, S.; Tamimi, W. Year-round vitamin D deficiency among saudi female out-patients. Public Health Nutr. 2013, 16, 544–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Whitehouse, A.J.; Holt, B.J.; Serralha, M.; Holt, P.G.; Kusel, M.M.; Hart, P.H. Maternal serum vitamin D levels during pregnancy and offspring neurocognitive development. Pediatrics 2012, 129, 485–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Shelton, J.F.; Geraghty, E.M.; Tancredi, D.J.; Delwiche, L.D.; Schmidt, R.J.; Ritz, B.; Hansen, R.L.; Hertz-Picciotto, I. Neurodevelopmental disorders and prenatal residential proximity to agricultural pesticides: The charge study. Environ. Health Perspect. 2014, 122, 1103–1109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Lee, B.K.; Magnusson, C.; Gardner, R.M.; Blomstrom, A.; Newschaffer, C.J.; Burstyn, I.; Karlsson, H.; Dalman, C. Maternal hospitalization with infection during pregnancy and risk of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Brain Behav. Immun. 2015, 44, 100–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Lyall, K.; Schmidt, R.J.; Hertz-Picciotto, I. Maternal lifestyle and environmental risk factors for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2014, 43, 443–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Xu, X.-J.; Shou, X.-J.; Li, J.; Jia, M.-X.; Zhang, J.-S.; Guo, Y.; Wei, Q.-Y.; Zhang, X.-T.; Han, S.-P.; Zhang, R.; et al. Mothers of autistic children: Lower plasma levels of oxytocin and arg-vasopressin and a higher level of testosterone. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e74849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Fernell, E.; Barnevik-Olsson, M.; Bågenholm, G.; Gillberg, C.; Gustafsson, S.; Sääf, M. Serum levels of 25-hydroxyvitamin D in mothers of swedish and of somali origin who have children with and without autism. Acta Paediatr. 2010, 99, 743–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Whitehouse, A.O.; Holt, B.; Serralha, M.; Holt, P.; Hart, P.; Kusel, M.H. Maternal vitamin D levels and the autism phenotype among offspring. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2013, 43, 1495–1504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Wakayo, T.; Belachew, T.; Vatanparast, H.; Whiting, S.J. Vitamin D deficiency and its predictors in a country with thirteen months of sunshine: The case of school children in central Ethiopia. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0120963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Rabenberg, M.; Scheidt-Nave, C.; Busch, M.A.; Rieckmann, N.; Hintzpeter, B.; Mensink, G.B.M. Vitamin D status among adults in germany—Results from the German health interview and examination survey for adults (DEGS1). BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Morales, E.; Guxens, M.; Llop, S.; Rodríguez-Bernal, C.L.; Tardón, A.; Riaño, I.; Ibarluzea, J.; Lertxundi, N.; Espada, M.; Rodriguez, A.; et al. Circulating 25-hydroxyvitamin D3 in pregnancy and infant neuropsychological development. Pediatrics 2012, 130, e913–e920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Gale, C.R.; Robinson, S.M.; Harvey, N.C.; Javaid, M.K.; Jiang, B.; Martyn, C.N.; Godfrey, K.M.; Cooper, C.; The Princess Anne Hospital Study Group. Maternal vitamin D status during pregnancy and child outcomes. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 62, 68–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Groves, N.J.; McGrath, J.J.; Burne, T.H.J. Vitamin D as a neurosteroid affecting the developing and adult brain. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 2014, 34, 117–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Stubbs, G.; Henley, K.; Green, J. Autism: Will vitamin D supplementation during pregnancy and early childhood reduce the recurrence rate of autism in newborn siblings? Med. Hypotheses 2016, 88, 74–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Du, L.; Shan, L.; Wang, B.; Feng, J.Y.; Xu, Z.D.; Jia, F.Y. Serum levels of 25-hydroxyvitamin D in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Zhongguo Dang Dai Er Ke Za Zhi 2015, 17, 68–71. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  108. Kocovska, E.; Andorsdottir, G.; Weihe, P.; Halling, J.; Fernell, E.; Stora, T.; Biskupsto, R.; Gillberg, I.C.; Shea, R.; Billstedt, E.; et al. Vitamin D in the general population of young adults with autism in the faroe islands. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014, 44, 2996–3005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Mostafa, G.A.; Al-Ayadhi, L.Y. Reduced serum concentrations of 25-hydroxy vitamin D in children with autism: Relation to autoimmunity. J. Neuroinflamm. 2012, 9, 201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Tostes, M.H.F.D.S.; Polonini, H.C.; Gattaz, W.F.; Raposo, N.R.B.; Baptista, E.B. Low serum levels of 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25-OHD) in children with autism. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2012, 34, 161–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  111. Saad, K.; Abdel-Rahman, A.A.; Elserogy, Y.M.; Al-Atram, A.A.; Cannell, J.J.; Bjorklund, G.; Abdel-Reheim, M.K.; Othman, H.A.; El-Houfey, A.A.; Abd El-Aziz, N.H.; et al. Vitamin D status in Autism Spectrum Disorders and the efficacy of vitamin D supplementation in autistic children. Nutr. Neurosci. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Gong, Z.L.; Luo, C.M.; Wang, L.; Shen, L.; Wei, F.; Tong, R.J.; Liu, Y. Serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D levels in Chinese children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neuroreport 2014, 25, 23–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Molloy, C.A.; Kalkwarf, H.J.; Manning-Courtney, P.; Mills, J.L.; Hediger, M.L. Plasma 25(OH)D concentration in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2010, 52, 969–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Neumeyer, A.M.; Gates, A.; Ferrone, C.; Lee, H.; Misra, M. Bone density in peripubertal boys with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2013, 43, 1623–1629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Hashemzadeh, M.; Moharreri, F.; Soltanifar, A. Comparative study of vitamin D levels in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and normal children: A case-control study. J. Fundam. Ment. Health 2015, 17, 197–201. [Google Scholar]
  116. Adams, J.B.; Audhya, T.; McDonough-Means, S.; Rubin, R.A.; Quig, D.; Geis, E.; Gehn, E.; Loresto, M.; Mitchell, J.; Atwood, S.; et al. Nutritional and metabolic status of children with autism vs. neurotypical children, and the association with autism severity. Nutr. Metab. (Lond.) 2011, 8, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Feng, J.; Shan, L.; Du, L.; Wang, B.; Li, H.; Wang, W.; Wang, T.; Dong, H.; Yue, X.; Xu, Z.; et al. Clinical improvement following vitamin D3 supplementation in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Nutr. Neurosci. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Bener, A.; Khattab, A.O.; Al-Dabbagh, M.M. Is high prevalence of vitamin D deficiency evidence for autism disorder? In a highly endogamous population. J. Pediatr. Neurosci. 2014, 9, 227–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  119. Humble, M.B.; Gustafsson, S.; Bejerot, S. Low serum levels of 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25-OHD) among psychiatric out-patients in Sweden: Relations with season, age, ethnic origin and psychiatric diagnosis. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 2010, 121, 467–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Waldron, J.L.; Ashby, H.L.; Cornes, M.P.; Bechervaise, J.; Razavi, C.; Thomas, O.L.; Chugh, S.; Deshpande, S.; Ford, C.; Gama, R. Vitamin D: A negative acute phase reactant. J. Clin. Pathol. 2013, 66, 620–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Hummel, D.M.; Fetahu, I.S.; Gröschel, C.; Manhardt, T.; Kállay, E. Role of proinflammatory cytokines on expression of vitamin D metabolism and target genes in colon cancer cells. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 2014, 144, 91–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Ozkan, B.; Doneray, H.; Karacan, M.; Vançelik, S.; Yildirim, Z.K.; Ozkan, A.; Kosan, C.; Aydin, K. Prevalence of vitamin D deficiency rickets in the eastern part of Turkey. Eur. J. Pediatr. 2009, 168, 95–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Khayyat, Y.; Attar, S. Vitamin D deficiency in patients with irritable bowel syndrome: Does it exist? Oman Med. J. 2015, 30, 115–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Joseph, A.J.; George, B.; Pulimood, A.B.; Seshadri, M.S.; Chacko, A. 25 (OH) vitamin D level in crohn’s disease: Association with sun exposure & disease activity. Indian J. Med. Res. 2009, 130, 133–137. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  125. Menon, B.; Harinarayan, C.V. The effect of anti epileptic drug therapy on serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D and parameters of calcium and bone metabolism—A longitudinal study. Seizure 2010, 19, 153–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Stewart, P.A.; Hyman, S.L.; Schmidt, B.L.; Macklin, E.A.; Reynolds, A.; Johnson, C.R.; James, S.J.; Manning-Courtney, P. Dietary supplementation in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Common, insufficient, and excessive. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2015, 115, 1237–1248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Must, A.; Phillips, S.M.; Curtin, C.; Anderson, S.E.; Maslin, M.; Lividini, K.; Bandini, L.G. Comparison of sedentary behaviors between children with Autism Spectrum Disorders and typically developing children. Autism 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Emond, A.; Emmett, P.; Steer, C.; Golding, J. Feeding symptoms, dietary patterns, and growth in young children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Pediatrics 2010, 126, e337–e342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  129. Zimmer, M.; Hart, L.; Manning-Courtney, P.; Murray, D.; Bing, N.; Summer, S. Food variety as a predictor of nutritional status among children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012, 42, 549–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Bandini, L.G.; Anderson, S.E.; Curtin, C.; Cermak, S.; Evans, E.W.; Scampini, R.; Maslin, M.; Must, A. Food selectivity in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders and typically developing children. J. Pediatr. 2010, 157, 259–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Marí-Bauset, S.; Llopis-González, A.; Zazpe-García, I.; Marí-Sanchis, A.; Morales-Suárez-Varela, M. Nutritional status of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASDs): A case—Control study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015, 45, 203–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  132. Hyman, S.L.; Stewart, P.A.; Schmidt, B.; Cain, U.; Lemcke, N.; Foley, J.T.; Peck, R.; Clemons, T.; Reynolds, A.; Johnson, C.; et al. Nutrient intake from food in children with autism. Pediatrics 2012, 130 (Suppl. S2), S145–S153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  133. Moore, E.; Crook, T.; James, J.; Gonzales, D.; Hakkak, R. Nutrient intake among children with autism. J. Nutr. Disord. Ther. 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Cornish, E. A balanced approach towards healthy eating in autism. J. Hum. Nutr. Diet. 1998, 11, 501–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Lindsay, R.L.; Eugene Arnold, L.; Aman, M.G.; Vitiello, B.; Posey, D.J.; McDougle, C.J.; Scahill, L.; Pachler, M.; McCracken, J.T.; Tierney, E.; et al. Dietary status and impact of risperidone on nutritional balance in children with autism: A pilot study. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2006, 31, 204–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Williams-Hooker, R.; George, E.O.; Levy, M.; Morgan, C.; Smith, T.L.; Bittle, J.B. Calcium and vitamin D intake of boys who have autism. Infant Child Adolesc. Nutr. 2013, 5, 113–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Graf-Myles, J.; Farmer, C.; Thurm, A.; Royster, C.; Kahn, P.; Soskey, L.; Rothschild, L.; Swedo, S. Dietary adequacy of children with autism compared with controls and the impact of restricted diet. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2013, 34, 449–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  138. Herndon, A.; DiGuiseppi, C.; Johnson, S.; Leiferman, J.; Reynolds, A. Does nutritional intake differ between children with Autism Spectrum Disorders and children with typical development? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2009, 39, 212–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  139. Jia, F.; Wang, B.; Shan, L.; Xu, Z.; Staal, W.G.; Du, L. Core symptoms of autism improved after vitamin D supplementation. Pediatrics 2015, 135, e196–e198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  140. Azzam, H.M.E.; Sayyah, H.; Youssef, S.; Lotfy, H.; Abdelhamid, I.A.; Abd Elhamed, H.A.; Maher, S. Autism and vitamin D: An intervention study. Middle East Curr. Psychiatry 2015, 22, 9–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Ucuz, İ.İ.; Dursun, O.B.; Esin, İ.S.; Özgeriş, F.B.; Kurt, N.; Kiziltunç, A.; Orbak, Z. The relationship between vitamin D, autistic spectrum disorders, and cognitive development: Do glial cell line-derived neurotrophic factor and nerve growth factor play a role in this relationship? Int. J. Dev. Disabil. 2015, 61, 222–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Corrigan, N.M.; Shaw, D.W.; Estes, A.M.; Richards, T.L.; Munson, J.; Friedman, S.D.; Dawson, G.; Artru, A.A.; Dager, S.R. Atypical developmental patterns of brain chemistry in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 2013, 70, 964–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  143. Cass, W.A.; Smith, M.P.; Peters, L.E. Calcitriol protects against the dopamine- and serotonin-depleting effects of neurotoxic doses of methamphetamine. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2006, 1074, 261–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  144. Eyles, D.W.; Smith, S.; Kinobe, R.; Hewison, M.; McGrath, J.J. Distribution of the vitamin D receptor and 1 alpha-hydroxylase in human brain. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 2005, 29, 21–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  145. Cui, X.; Pelekanos, M.; Liu, P.Y.; Burne, T.H.J.; McGrath, J.J.; Eyles, D.W. The vitamin D receptor in dopamine neurons; its presence in human substantia nigra and its ontogenesis in rat midbrain. Neuroscience 2013, 236, 77–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  146. Frazier, T.W.; Thompson, L.; Youngstrom, E.A.; Law, P.; Hardan, A.Y.; Eng, C.; Morris, N. A twin study of heritable and shared environmental contributions to autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014, 44, 2013–2025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  147. Sandin, S.; Lichtenstein, P.; Kuja-Halkola, R.; Larsson, H.; Hultman, C.M.; Reichenberg, A. The familial risk of autism. JAMA 2014, 311, 1770–1777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  148. Schmidt, R.J.; Hansen, R.L.; Hartiala, J.; Allayee, H.; Sconberg, J.L.; Schmidt, L.C.; Volk, H.E.; Tassone, F. Selected vitamin D metabolic gene variants and risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder in the charge study. Early Hum. Dev. 2015, 91, 483–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  149. Nair-Shalliker, V.; Armstrong, B.K.; Fenech, M. Does vitamin D protect against DNA damage? Mutat. Res-Fund. Mol. Mech. Mut. 2012, 733, 50–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  150. Fedirko, V.; Bostick, R.M.; Long, Q.; Flanders, W.D.; McCullough, M.L.; Sidelnikov, E.; Daniel, C.R.; Rutherford, R.E.; Shaukat, A. Effects of supplemental vitamin D and calcium on oxidative DNA damage marker in normal colorectal mucosa: A randomized clinical trial. Cancer Epidemiol. Biomark. Prev. 2010, 19, 280–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  151. Mabley, J.G.; Wallace, R.; Pacher, P.; Murphy, K.; SzabÓ, C. Inhibition of poly(adenosine diphosphate-ribose) polymerase by the active form of vitamin D. Int. J. Mol. Med. 2007, 19, 947–952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  152. Chatterjee, M. Vitamin D and genomic stability. Mutat. Res-Fund. Mol. Mech. Mut. 2001, 475, 69–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. James, S.J.; Cutler, P.; Melnyk, S.; Jernigan, S.; Janak, L.; Gaylor, D.W.; Neubrander, J.A. Metabolic biomarkers of increased oxidative stress and impaired methylation capacity in children with autism. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2004, 80, 1611–1617. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  154. Alvarez, J.A.; Chowdhury, R.; Jones, D.P.; Martin, G.S.; Brigham, K.L.; Binongo, J.N.; Ziegler, T.R.; Tangpricha, V. Vitamin D status is independently associated with plasma glutathione and cysteine thiol/disulphide redox status in adults. Clin. Endocrinol. 2014, 81, 458–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  155. Codoner-Franch, P.; Tavarez-Alonso, S.; Simo-Jorda, R.; Laporta-Martin, P.; Carratala-Calvo, A.; Alonso-Iglesias, E. Vitamin D status is linked to biomarkers of oxidative stress, inflammation, and endothelial activation in obese children. J. Pediatr. 2012, 161, 848–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  156. Alfawaz, H.; Bhat, R.; Al-Ayadhi, L.; El-Ansary, A. Protective and restorative potency of vitamin D on persistent biochemical autistic features induced in propionic acid-intoxicated rat pups. BMC Complement. Altern. Med. 2014, 14, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  157. Alabdali, A.; Al-Ayadhi, L.; El-Ansary, A. A key role for an impaired detoxification mechanism in the etiology and severity of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Behav. Brain Func. 2014, 10, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  158. Shinpo, K.; Kikuchi, S.; Sasaki, H.; Moriwaka, F.; Tashiro, K. Effect of 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 on cultured mesencephalic dopaminergic neurons to the combined toxicity caused by l-buthionine sulfoximine and 1-methyl-4-phenylpyridine. J. Neurosci. Res. 2000, 62, 374–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  159. Garcion, E.; Sindji, L.; Leblondel, G.; Brachet, P.; Darcy, F. 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 regulates the synthesis of γ-glutamyl transpeptidase and glutathione levels in rat primary astrocytes. J. Neurochem. 1999, 73, 859–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  160. Morgan, J.T.; Chana, G.; Pardo, C.A.; Achim, C.; Semendeferi, K.; Buckwalter, J.; Courchesne, E.; Everall, I.P. Microglial activation and increased microglial density observed in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in autism. Biol. Psychiatry 2010, 68, 368–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  161. Vargas, D.L.; Nascimbene, C.; Krishnan, C.; Zimmerman, A.W.; Pardo, C.A. Neuroglial activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann. Neurol. 2005, 57, 67–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  162. Almerighi, C.; Sinistro, A.; Cavazza, A.; Ciaprini, C.; Rocchi, G.; Bergamini, A. 1α,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 inhibits CD40l-induced pro-inflammatory and immunomodulatory activity in human monocytes. Cytokine 2009, 45, 190–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  163. Heine, G.; Niesner, U.; Chang, H.-D.; Steinmeyer, A.; Zügel, U.; Zuberbier, T.; Radbruch, A.; Worm, M. 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 promotes IL-10 production in human b cells. Eur. J. Immunol. 2008, 38, 2210–2218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  164. Széles, L.; Keresztes, G.; Töröcsik, D.; Balajthy, Z.; Krenács, L.; Póliska, S.; Steinmeyer, A.; Zuegel, U.; Pruenster, M.; Rot, A.; et al. 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 is an autonomous regulator of the transcriptional changes leading to a tolerogenic dendritic cell phenotype. J. Immunol. 2009, 182, 2074–2083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  165. Mostafa, G.A.; Al-ayadhi, L.Y. Increased serum levels of anti-ganglioside m1 auto-antibodies in autistic children: Relation to the disease severity. J. Neuroinflamm. 2011, 8, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  166. Mostafa, G.A.; Kitchener, N. Serum anti-nuclear antibodies as a marker of autoimmunity in egyptian autistic children. Pediatr. Neurol. 2009, 40, 107–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  167. Mostafa, G.A.; Al-Ayadhi, L.Y. The relationship between the increased frequency of serum antineuronal antibodies and the severity of autism in children. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 2012, 16, 464–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  168. Mostafa, G.A.; El-Sayed, Z.A.; El-Aziz, M.M.; El-Sayed, M.F. Serum anti-myelin-associated glycoprotein antibodies in egyptian autistic children. J. Child Neurol. 2008, 23, 1413–1418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  169. Jung, J.Y.; Kohane, I.S.; Wall, D.P. Identification of autoimmune gene signatures in autism. Transl. Psychiatry 2011, 1, e63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  170. Zikopoulos, B.; Barbas, H. Changes in prefrontal axons may disrupt the network in autism. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 14595–14609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  171. Dendrou, C.A.; Fugger, L.; Friese, M.A. Immunopathology of multiple sclerosis. Nat. Rev. Immunol. 2015, 15, 545–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  172. Ritterhouse, L.L.; Crowe, S.R.; Niewold, T.B.; Kamen, D.L.; Macwana, S.R.; Roberts, V.C.; Dedeke, A.B.; Harley, J.B.; Scofield, R.H.; Guthridge, J.M.; et al. Vitamin D deficiency is associated with an increased autoimmune response in healthy individuals and in patients with systemic lupus erythematosus. Ann. Rheum. Dis. 2011, 70, 1569–1574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  173. Chabas, J.-F.; Stephan, D.; Marqueste, T.; Garcia, S.; Lavaut, M.-N.; Nguyen, C.; Legre, R.; Khrestchatisky, M.; Decherchi, P.; Feron, F. Cholecalciferol (vitamin D3) improves myelination and recovery after nerve injury. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e65034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  174. Mokry, L.E.; Ross, S.; Ahmad, O.S.; Forgetta, V.; Smith, G.D.; Leong, A.; Greenwood, C.M.; Thanassoulis, G.; Richards, J.B. Vitamin D and risk of multiple sclerosis: A mendelian randomization study. PLoS Med. 2015, 12, e1001866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  175. Wang, M.; Chen, H.; Yu, T.; Cui, G.; Jiao, A.; Liang, H. Increased serum levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Neuroreport 2015, 26, 638–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  176. Dincel, N.; Unalp, A.; Kutlu, A.; Ozturk, A.; Uran, N.; Ulusoy, S. Serum nerve growth factor levels in autistic children in turkish population: A preliminary study. Indian J. Med. Res. 2013, 138, 900–903. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  177. Neveu, I.; Naveilhan, P.; Jehan, F.D.R.; Baudet, C.; Wion, D.; De Luca, H.F.; Brachet, P. 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 regulates the synthesis of nerve growth factor in primary cultures of glial cells. Mol. Brain Res. 1994, 24, 70–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  178. Brown, J.; Bianco, J.I.; McGrath, J.J.; Eyles, D.W. 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 induces nerve growth factor, promotes neurite outgrowth and inhibits mitosis in embryonic rat hippocampal neurons. Neurosci. Lett. 2003, 343, 139–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  179. Pozzi, F.; Aloe, L.; Frajese, G.V.; Frajese, G. Vitamin D (Calcifediol) supplementation modulates NGF and BDNF and improves memory function in postmenopausal women: A pilot study. Res. Endocrinol. 2013, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  180. Crockett, M.J.; Clark, L.; Tabibnia, G.; Lieberman, M.D.; Robbins, T.W. Serotonin modulates behavioral reactions to unfairness. Science 2008, 320, 1739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  181. Anagnostou, E.; Soorya, L.; Chaplin, W.; Bartz, J.; Halpern, D.; Wasserman, S.; Wang, A.T.; Pepa, L.; Tanel, N.; Kushki, A.; et al. Intranasal oxytocin versus placebo in the treatment of adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders: A randomized controlled trial. Mol. Autism 2012, 3, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  182. Naranjo, C.A.; Tremblay, L.K.; Busto, U.E. The role of the brain reward system in depression. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2001, 25, 781–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  183. Staal, W.; de Krom, M.; de Jonge, M. Brief report: The dopamine-3-receptor gene (DRD3) is associated with specific repetitive behavior in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012, 42, 885–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  184. Nakamura, K.; Sekine, Y.; Ouchi, Y.; Tsujii, M.; Yoshikawa, E.; Futatsubashi, M.; Tsuchiya, K.J.; Sugihara, G.; Iwata, Y.; Suzuki, K.; et al. Brain serotonin and dopamine transporter bindings in adults with high-functioning autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  185. Modahl, C.; Green, L.; Fein, D.; Morris, M.; Waterhouse, L.; Feinstein, C.; Levin, H. Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children. Biol. Psychiatry 1998, 43, 270–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  186. Chugani, D.C.; Muzik, O.; Behen, M.; Rothermel, R.; Janisse, J.J.; Lee, J.; Chugani, H.T. Developmental changes in brain serotonin synthesis capacity in autistic and nonautistic children. Ann. Neurol. 1999, 45, 287–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  187. Mulder, E.J.; Anderson, G.M.; Kema, I.P.; de Bildt, A.; van Lang, N.D.; den Boer, J.A.; Minderaa, R.B. Platelet serotonin levels in Pervasive Developmental Disorders and mental retardation: Diagnostic group differences, within-group distribution, and behavioral correlates. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2004, 43, 491–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  188. Cartier, E.; Hamilton, P.J.; Belovich, A.N.; Shekar, A.; Campbell, N.G.; Saunders, C.; Andreassen, T.F.; Gether, U.; Veenstra-Vanderweele, J.; Sutcliffe, J.S.; et al. Rare autism-associated variants implicate syntaxin 1 (STX1 R26Q) phosphorylation and the dopamine transporter (HDAT R51W) in dopamine neurotransmission and behaviors. EBioMedicine 2015, 2, 135–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  189. Patrick, R.P.; Ames, B.N. Vitamin D hormone regulates serotonin synthesis. Part 1: Relevance for autism. FASEB J. 2014, 28, 2398–2413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  190. Frye, R.E. Metabolic and mitochondrial disorders associated with epilepsy in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Epilepsy Behav. 2015, 47, 147–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  191. Yasuhara, A. Correlation between eeg abnormalities and symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Brain Dev. 2010, 32, 791–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  192. Hara, H. Autism and epilepsy: A retrospective follow-up study. Brain Dev. 2007, 29, 486–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  193. Bromley, R.L.; Mawer, G.; Clayton-Smith, J.; Baker, G.A. Autism Spectrum Disorders following in utero exposure to antiepileptic drugs. Neurology 2008, 71, 1923–1924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  194. Kalueff, A.V.; Minasyan, A.; Tuohimaa, P. Anticonvulsant effects of 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D in chemically induced seizures in mice. Brain Res. Bull. 2005, 67, 156–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  195. Borowicz, K.K.; Morawska, M.; Furmanek-Karwowska, K.; Luszczki, J.J.; Czuczwar, S.J. Cholecalciferol enhances the anticonvulsant effect of conventional antiepileptic drugs in the mouse model of maximal electroshock. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2007, 573, 111–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  196. Siegel, A.; Malkowitz, L.; Moskovits, M.J.; Christakos, S. Administration of 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 results in the elevation of hippocampal seizure threshold levels in rats. Brain Res. 1984, 298, 125–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  197. Holló, A.; Clemens, Z.; Kamondi, A.; Lakatos, P.; Szűcs, A. Correction of vitamin D deficiency improves seizure control in epilepsy: A pilot study. Epilepsy Behav. 2012, 24, 131–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  198. Christiansen, C.; Rødbro, P.; Sjö, O. “Anticonvulsant action” of vitamin D in epileptic patients? A controlled pilot study. Br. Med. J. 1974, 2, 258–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  199. Kalueff, A.V.; Eremin, K.O.; Tuohimaa, P. Mechanisms of neuroprotective action of vitamin D3. Biochemistry 2004, 69, 738–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Risk of Autistic Disorder (AD) in relation to latitude & Risk of Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD) in relation to latitude.
Table 1. Risk of Autistic Disorder (AD) in relation to latitude & Risk of Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD) in relation to latitude.
ReferenceCountry, AreaLatitudeAgeDiagnosisPrevalence/10,000CI
Risk of Autistic Disorder (AD) in relation to latitude
[36]China, Tianjin39.13°N8DSM-IV/CABS-CV, CARS-CV10.93.4–25.4
[37]UK, South East Thames51.24°N7ICD-10/CHAT, CR, PDD-Q30.822.9–40.6
[38]UK, Staffordshire and Cannock52.80°N4–6ICD-10, DSM-IV/multidisciplinary screening ADI-R18.914.1–25.0
[17]UK, Staffordshire52.80°N2.5–6.5DSM-IV/Clinical evaluation, ADI-R16.811.0–24.6
[39]Sweden, Goteborg57.70°N3–6ICD-10/Clinical evaluation, ADI-R46.416.1–76.6
[40]Sweden, Karlstad59.37°N7ICD-10/ASSQ (cut off teachers only: 17), Clinical evaluation, ADI-R60.019.0–141.0
[41]Finland, Northern Ostrobothnia63.91°N8DSM-IV-TR, DSM-5/ASSQ (cut off: 30 parents and teacher combined), ADI-R, ADOS-341.026.0–64.0
Risk of Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD) in relation to latitude
[42]UAE, 3 regions24.46°N3DSM-IV/ASC (cut off: 15), Clinical evaluation29.00–79.0
[43]Iran, Country wide32.00°N5DSM-IV/National screening, SCQ, ADI-R6.35.8–6.7
[44]Argentina, San Isidro34.28°S0–5DSM-IV/PRUNAPE, BDI, VABS, multi-disciplinary evaluation13.1-
[45]Japan, Toyota35.08°N5–8DSM-IV/Integral screening system, Direct clinical evaluation181.1-
[37]UK, South East Thames51.24°N7ICD-10/CHAT, CR, PDD-Q57.946.8–70.9
[38]UK, Staffordshire and Cannock52.80°N4–6ICD-10, DSM-IV/multidisciplinary screening, ADI-R58.745.2–74.9
[17]UK, Staffordshire52.80°N2.5–6.5DSM-IV/Clinical evaluation, ADI-R61.950.2–75.6
DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Fourth Edition; CABS-CV, Clancy Autism Behaviour Checklist-Chinese Version; CARS, Childhood Autism Rating Scale; ICD-10, International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (10th edition); CHAT, Checklist for Autism in Toddlers; CR, Checklist for referral; PDD-Q, Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Questionnaire; ADI-R, Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised; ASSQ, Autism Spectrum Screening Questionnaire; ADOS-3, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Module 3; ASC, Autism Screening Questionnaire; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire; PRUNAPE, Prueba Nacional de Pesquisa; BDI, Battelle Developmental Inventory; VABS, Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales.
Table 2. Risk of autism in mothers born outside the reference country and according to mothers’ ethnicity.
Table 2. Risk of autism in mothers born outside the reference country and according to mothers’ ethnicity.
ReferenceReference Country/YearCohortAge (Year)Cases NumberCases AscertainmentMother’s Country of Birth/EthnicityOdds Ratio (95% CI)Covariates
[64]Sweden 20021987–1994<10408AD/ICD 9Europe and North America1.1 (0.5–2.5)Maternal age, parity, smoking during pregnancy, hypertensive disease, diabetes, pregnancy bleeding, mode of delivery, season of birth, gestational age, birth weight, Apgar score, congenital malformation
Outside Europe and North America3.0 (1.7–5.2)
[68]US/California 20021989–1994-4381AD/DSM-III-R or DSM-IVOther US states0.9 (0.8–1.0)Gender, birth weight, plurality, birth order, maternal age, maternal race, maternal education RR
Mexico0.6 (0.5–0.7)
Other1.1 (1.0–1.2)
[65]Denmark 20051984–1998<10818AD, atypical autism/medical record registry/ICD-10Scandinavia and Europe1.0 (0.8–1.4)Age, sex, interaction between age and sex, calendar year, history of autism or broader autism in siblings psychiatric disorders RR
Outside Europe1.4 (1.1–1.8)
[62]Denmark 20061990–1999<10473AD/ICD-8 and 10Foreign Citizenship (not mentioned)1.7 (1.3–2.4)Maternal and parental age, maternal citizenship, birth weight, gestational age, Apgar score, irregular foetal position, congenital malformation, psychoactive medicine use in pregnancy
[66]Australia 20081990–1996<5368ASD/Surveillance/DSM-IVOutside Australia1.4 (1.0–1.9)Gender, maternal age ≥35, gestational age <37
[67]Sweden 20101980–2005<25250AD and AS/DSM-IV or DSM-III or ICD-10/ADOS-G and ADI-ROutside Nordic countries:Year of birth, maternal age ≥40, gestational age <37, gestational age-adjusted birth weight
AD2.2 (1.6–3.1)
AS0.6 (0.3–1.0)
Sub-Saharan Africa5.6 (2.9–10.6)
South or Central America3.1 (1.3–7.1)
East Asia2.9 (1.4- 6.1)
Western Europe/USA2.8 (1.2–6.5)
Previous Eastern Europe2.1 (1.3–3.3)
Middle East/North Africa2.0 (1.2–3.2)
[63]UK 20101999–2005<18428 (267, Wandsworth and 161 in Lambeth)ASD/2 boroughs/Multidisciplinary team assessment/ICD-10 using ADI-R, DISCO, ADOSOther EuropeanFamily size RR
L1.3 (0.6–2.8)
W1.2 (0.8–1.9)
African
L7.9 (5.4–11.6)
W3.3 (2.4–4.5)
Caribbean
L10.0 (5.5–18.1)
W8.9 (5.9–15.5)
Asian
L4.0 (2.0–7.8)
W2.1 (1.3–3.3)
[61]Sweden 20122001–20070–173918ASD/Medical registry/Multidisciplinary teams/DSM-IVAfrican, American, Asian and EuropeanMaternal and parental age, family disposable income, for subsample, birth weight, gestational age, Apgar score at 5 min after birth
Low functioning1.2 (1.0–1.4)
High functioning0.5 (0.4–0.6)
[70]Netherlands 20131998–2007-518ASD/Psychiatric case registry/DSM-IVDeveloping countriesGender and paternal age RR
ASD0.6 (0.5–0.9)
AD1.4 (0.9–2.4)
AS and PDD-NOS0.4 (0.3–0.6)
Developed countries
ASD0.9 (0.6–1.3)
AD1.6 (0.8–3.5)
AS and PDD-NOS0.6 (0.4–1.1)
[69]US/Los Angeles 20141995–2006-7540AD/DSM-III-R/ICD-9-CM/ADOSWhite foreign1.0 (0.9–1.2)Maternal age, gender, birth year, birth type, parity, gestational age, birth weight, pregnancy complications, trimester pregnancy care began, maternal education, insurance and diagnostic variability (regional centres) RR
Black foreign1.8 (1.4–2.2)
Mexico1.1 (1.0–1.2)
Central/South America1.3 (0.9–1.1)
China0.7 (0.6–0.8)
Japan0.7 (0.5–1.0)
Korea1.0 (0.8–1.2)
Philippines1.3 (1.1–1.4)
Vietnam1.4 (1.2–1.7)
AD, Autistic Disorder; ICD, International Classification of Diseases; US, The United States; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorders; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; ADI-R, Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised; AS, Asperger; UK, The United Kingdom; DISCO, Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders.
Table 3. Risk of autism according to seasonality of conception and birth.
Table 3. Risk of autism according to seasonality of conception and birth.
ReferenceCountry/YearDiagnosisCase-Control NumbersCase—Control CharacteristicsConfounders/CovariatesExcess ConceptionExcess Birth
[92]Canada 1986DSM-III179-NRLow functioning (IQ < 50) and high functioning (IQ > 50) autism, non-verbal and verbal autism from medical records from two different centres-live birthsMarch vs. Sep-Feb
Spring-early summer vs. winter and autumn (Aggregated sample)
Spring vs. winter and autumn (Low functioning, nonverbal, male)
[91]Japan 1988DSM-III80–71,013Native infantile autism <8 years from clinic outpatients-children <8 years from annual reportsSecond quarter of the year (corresponding to spring) vs. first and third quarter*
[90]Sweden 1990DSM-III-R100-NRCryptogenic autism-populations born in Sweden (Central Bureau of statistics)Cases with medical conditions and of mothers immigrated to Sweden from non-European countries were excludedMarch
[89]UK 1992ICD-9, DSM-III1435–196–121–24,957,169National autistic sample-clinic sample-sibling controls-live birthsSignificantly deviated from the general populations’ expected moth of birth (national sample)
December, January, June, July and October
[88]Denmark 1994ICD-9328-NRInfantile autism-autism like disorder-borderline psychosis from clinic outpatients-live birthsMarch and April vs. November December
[87]Israel 1995DSM-III-R188–1,992,410Infantile autism-live birthsMarch and August
[86]International 1999DSM-IV, ICD-10620–284Cases with autism from international multisite field trial for DSM-IV-Individuals with mental retardation from patients of a clinicNo association
[84]Netherlands 2000ICD-91031-NRNational registry of mentally retarded patients with AD and PDD-NOS (IQ < 35)-general population birth dataNo association (month and season) (Aggregated sample)
Second quarter of the year (Low functioning)
[85]US 2000DSM-III-R175–123High and low functioning autism (verbal IQ cut off of 65) recruited for a research project-full siblings and half siblingsArbitrary assignations of month to seasonNo association (aggregated sample)
March (more low functioning and socially passive autism) (Boston subset)
[64]Sweden 2002ICD-9408–2040Infantile autism <10 years from medical birth register-birth registerMaternal age, parity, smoking, mother’s country of birth, hypertensive disease, diabetes, pregnancy bleeding, mode of delivery, gestational age, birth weight, Apgar score, congenital malformationNo association
[83]Israel 2006ICD-10211–311,169ASD adolescents (age of 17) from military medical registry-live birthsYear of birth, socioeconomic statusNo association
[77]Denmark 2007ICD-101860–407,117ASD and ASD subcategories from psychiatric registry-live birthsGeneral trend for increase in incidence over time, follow up time, length of gestationNo associationNo association
[82]US 2008DSM-IV1051–1,458,011ASD singletons and multiple births from medical records-statistics data for singleton and multiple live birthsNumber of births and genderSpring (April), summer (late July) and autumn (October) vs. winter (December and January) (Singletons and multiple births)
[80]Denmark 2010ICD-10317–733,826Infantile autism from medical birth register-live birthsGender, maternal smoking status, irregular fetal presentation, birth weight, gestational age, Apgar score, parental age, maternal citizenship, congenital malformationWinter (October to March) vs. Summer (April to September), 2.21 (1.24–3.94) vs. 1.02 (0.41–2.50)
[81]Egypt2010DSM-IV70–40ASD (recruited for the purpose of the study)-non ASD healthy controlsNo significant difference
June (26.7%) followed by March and April (11.4%,)
[76]UK 2010ICD-1086–13,892ASD from medical and educational records-live birthsSummer vs. winter 2.08 (1.18-3.70)Spring vs. autumn (reference), 1.86 (1.01-3.37)
[75]US 2011ICD-919,328–6,585,737Full syndrome autism <6 years and live births <6 years from datasetGender, race/ethnicity, Preterm birth, maternal age, maternal education, maternal place of residence at childbirth and maternal year of conceptionWinter (January, February and March) vs. Summer, 1.06 (1.02–1.10)November vs. April (reference), 1.12 (1.05–1.20)
[74]US 2012DSM-IV8,074–3,888,495AD not comorbid with mental retardation-live birthsGender, parental age and education, race and ethnicity, insurance status, preterm birth and low birth weightWinter (the last 3 weeks of November and first week of December), 2.11, 1.72 and 1.53 in 1994, 1995 and 1996, respectively
[79]Turkey 2014DSM-IV54–54ASD (recruited for the purpose of the study)-non ASD healthy controlsNo association
[78]Sweden 2015DSM-IV-TR58–58ASD (recruited for the purpose of the study)-non ASD siblingsNo association in children of Middle Eastern/African ethnicity
Spring vs. Summer, 38% vs. 10% in ASD and 18% vs. 35% in non ASD in children of Sweden and European ethnicity
Table 4. Vitamin D interventions to prevent or treat Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD).
Table 4. Vitamin D interventions to prevent or treat Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD).
ReferenceCountry/YearStudy Design/CountryPopulation CharacteristicsInterventionBaseline 25(OH)D (nmol/L)Follow up 25(OH)D (nmol/L)Outcome Measure
Vitamin D intervention to prevent ASD
[106]US 2016Prospective open label intervention trialPregnant mothers of autistic childrenDaily 5000 IU during pregnancy and 7000 IU during breastfeedingAll but two >5070–198Recurrence rate of autism in new born siblings was 5% which was lower than that reported in the literature (20%)
Daily 1000 IU during the first three years of life if child was not breastfed
Vitamin D intervention to treat ASD
[119]Sweden 2010Clinical quality assurance projectAutism and other psychiatric disorders (mean age of 43.7 years)Daily 1600–4000 IU vitamin D3 or 35,000–70,000 IU vitamin D2 once weekly31.5 (23–39) in autistic patients and 45 (31–60) in all patients *-Improvement in psychosis or depression
[139]China 2015Case studyA 32-month old male toddlerMonthly 150 00031.3 **203ABC 1 (from 80 to 39)
IU IM and CARS (from 35 to 28)
daily 400 IU orallySeverity of Illness of Clinical Global Impression (from 6 to 4).
for two months
[111]Egypt 2015Open label intervention trial106 autistic children with 25(OH)D <75 nmol/LDaily 300 IU vitamin D3/Kg not exceeding 5000 IU/day for three months<75-ABC2: Improvements in irritability (0.02), hyperactivity (0.03), social withdrawal (0.01) and stereotypic behaviour (0.04)
No improvements in inappropriate speech
CARS: Improvements in total (p < 0.001), relating to people (p < 0.001), imitation (p < 0.001), body use (p = 0.01), object use (p = 0.01), adaptation to change (p = 0.004), listening response (p = 0.01), visual response (p = 0.003) and general impression (p < 0.001)
No improvements in fear, verbal communication, activity level, nonverbal communication and intellectual response
The improvement was more pronounced in those with final 25(OH)D >100 nmol/L
[140] 1Egypt 2015Randomised controlled trial21 autistic children assigned to vitamin D or no treatment groups---CARS, social IQ and ATEC: Improved in both groups
Improvement was not significantly different across groups
[141] 2Turkey 2015Open label intervention trialToddlers with developmental delay without and with ASD 2 (2–5 years old), and 25(OH)D <50 nmol/L (n = 11, cases) and ≥50 nmol/L (n = 10, controls) Baseline 25(OH)D <37.5 nmol/L: daily 5000IU for one month and then daily 400IU for two months if 25(OH)D is between 37.5 and 50 nmol/L after one month--ABC 1 and Denver II: Significant improvement in both groups (ABC, from 90 ± 19 to 59 ± 15 in cases and from 77 ± 22 to 64 ± 29 in controls; Denver II, from 64 ± 13 to 72 ± 17 in cases and from 73 ± 11 to 80 ± 12 in controls).
Baseline 25(OH)D between 37.5 and 50 nmol/L: daily 400 IU for one to three months depending on the level at one month
Neither baseline nor endpoint scores were significantly different across groups, but improvement was more pronounced in cases
[117]China 2016Open label intervention trial37 autistic children with 25(OH)D <75 nmol/LMonthly 150,000 IU IM and daily 400 IU orally for three months--CARS: Improvement in total scores
ABC 1: Improvement in total and social skills, body and object use, language, and social and self-help
Improvement was more pronounced in younger children (≤3 vs. >3 years old).
* Median (25th–75th percentile); ** The values were ng/mL, they have been converted to nmol/L for easy comparison (1 ng/mL = 2.5 nmol/L); 1 The full text was not available at the time of writing this manuscript and are information provided here are based on the abstract; 2 The data for children with developmental delay and ASD are reported here; ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; 25(OH)D, 25 hydroxyvitamin D; IU, international unit; D3, cholecalciferol; D2, ergocalciferol; ABC1, Autism Behaviour Checklist; CARS, Childhood Autism Rating Scale; ABC2, Aberrant Behaviour Checklist; ATEC, Autism Treatment Evaluation Checklist; Denver II, Denver Developmental Screening Test II.
Table 5. Vitamin D status in ASD patients-Case-control studies.
Table 5. Vitamin D status in ASD patients-Case-control studies.
ReferenceCountry/YearCase-Control CharacteristicsAssessment ToolsCovariates/ConfoundersBoys: Girlsp-Value25(OH)D Concentration (nmol/L )*
CasesControls
[81]Egypt 201070 children with autism (mean age of 5.3 ± 2.8)DSM-IVSeason of birth-<0.00171.3 ± 41.0 **100.3 ± 29.5
40 age matched healthy controls of the same socioeconomic status controls (thoroughly examined)
[113]US 201171 Caucasian males with ASD (4–8 years old)DSM-IV/ADOSCovariates: age, BMI, use of supplement, antiepileptic medication, season of enrolmentOnly malen.s.49.8 (range, 27.0–77.5) **42.5 (range, 19.5–70.8)
(CFD)
n.s49.0 (range, 22.3–76.8)
69 age matched typically developing controls(NCFD)
[116]US 201155 children with ASD (aged 5–16 years old)Diagnostic confirmation from pediatrician or other professionals/PDD-BI (autism composite), ATEC, SAS49: 6n.s.74.6 ± 21.0 **71.5 ± 21.0
44 age, sex and geographically similar distribution
[110]Brazil 201224 children with ASD (mean age of 7.4 ± 2.7 years)DSM-IV18:6<0.00166.2 ± 8.7 **101.3 ± 7.8
24 age and sex matched healthy controls
[109]Saudi Arabia 201250 children with ASD (5–12 years)DSM-IV/CARS (severity)All blood samples drawn in summer39:11<0.00146.3 (IQR, 35.0) **82.5 (IQR, 27.5)
30 age and sex matched healthy controls
[114]US 201218 boys with ASD (mean age of 10.6 ± 0.4 years)DSM-IV/ADOSSeason of blood collection, bone ageNA0.0666.8 ± 4.8 **79.3 ± 4.0
19 boys without ASD
[112]China 201348 children with ASD (mean age of 3.7 ± 1.2 years)DSM-IV, CARS (all cases)Season of blood sampling, all study population belonged to Chinese Han population40:80.00249.8 ± 9.5 **56.5 ± 11.3
48 age and sex matched healthy controls (all examined thoroughly by paediatircian for any possible autistic features)
[118]Qatar 2014254 children with ASD (mean age of 5.5± 1.6 years)ADOS165:890.00446.0 ± 20.5 **54.0 ± 21.0
254 age, sex and ethnicity matched controls
[79]Turkey 201454 children with ASD (mean age of 59.6 ± 15.0 months)DSM-IV/ABC-T, ABC21 CARS, ADSI, Stanford-Binet or WISC-RSeason of enrolment47:70.06962.8 ± 28.3 **52.8 ± 24.3
54 age, sex, season of enrolment and societal status matched healthy controls
[108]Faroe Island 201440 individuals with ASD (white European origin of different age) 62 typically developing sibling 77 parents 40 healthy age, sex and season of birth matched controlsICD-10, DSM-IV/ADOS, DISCO, WISC-III, WAIS-RAdjustment for month of blood collection31:90.00224.8 (IQR, 27.5)37.6 (IQR, 32.3) (controls)
<0.00146.1 (IQR, 28.3) (siblings)
<0.00146.7 (IQR, 36.2) (parents)
[111]Egypt 2015122 children with ASD (mean age of 5.1 ± 1.4 years)DSM-IV/CARS, ABC 2Blood samples collected during two months75% male in control group<0.000145.1 ± 21.9 **106.3 ± 23.7
100 age, sex and societal status matched healthy controls (all screened for any mental and autistic manifestations)
[115]Iran 201513 children with ASD (3–12 years)DSM-IV/CARS11:20.3513.0 (IQR, 9.6–19.5)12.0 (IQR, 4.9–13.2)
14 age and sex matched controls
[78]Sweden 201558 multi-ethnic children with ASD diagnosed at the age of 4 or olderMulti-professional expert team51:60.0124.0 ± 19.631.9 ± 27.7
59 healthy siblings
[117]China 2016215 children with ASD (mean age of 4.8 ± 1.0 years)DSM-IV/ADOSBlood samples collected during six months173:420.02--
285 age and sex matched healthy controls (mean age of 5.1 ± 1.1 years)
* All values reported as means ± standard deviation, otherwise stated; ** The values were ng/mL, they have been converted to nmol/L for easy comparison (1 ng/mL = 2.5 nmol/L); ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; 25(OH)D, 25-hydroxyvitamin D; DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Fourth Edition; US, The United States; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; BMI, body mass index; CFD, casein free diet; NCFD, non casein free diet; PDD-BI, Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Behaviour Inventory (autism composite); ATEC, Autism Evaluation Treatment Checklist;; SAS, Severity of Autism Scale; IQR, interquartile range; CARS, Childhood Autism Rating Scale; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; ABC-T, Aberrant behaviour checklist-Turkish version; ABC1, Autism Behaviour Checklist; ADSI, Ankara Developmental Screening Inventory; WISC-R, Wechsler Intelligence for Children-Revised;.ICD-10, International Classification of Disease-Tenth Edition; DISCO, Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders; WISC-III, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition; WAIS-R, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised; ABC2, Aberrant Behaviour Checklist.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mazahery, H.; Camargo, C.A.; Conlon, C.; Beck, K.L.; Kruger, M.C.; Von Hurst, P.R. Vitamin D and Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Literature Review. Nutrients 2016, 8, 236. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu8040236

AMA Style

Mazahery H, Camargo CA, Conlon C, Beck KL, Kruger MC, Von Hurst PR. Vitamin D and Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Literature Review. Nutrients. 2016; 8(4):236. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu8040236

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mazahery, Hajar, Carlos A. Camargo, Cathryn Conlon, Kathryn L. Beck, Marlena C. Kruger, and Pamela R. Von Hurst. 2016. "Vitamin D and Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Literature Review" Nutrients 8, no. 4: 236. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu8040236

APA Style

Mazahery, H., Camargo, C. A., Conlon, C., Beck, K. L., Kruger, M. C., & Von Hurst, P. R. (2016). Vitamin D and Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Literature Review. Nutrients, 8(4), 236. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu8040236

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop