1. Introduction
Darwinian evolution is based on three observations that are widely accepted: (1) organisms vary, (2) organisms tend to increase geometrically, and (3) not all organisms survive. Darwin’s insight was that during the struggle for existence, some organisms perish, while others survive and reproduce, and in so doing, pass on to their offspring the basis for their success [
1] (pp. 31–33). In essence, evolution is any change in gene frequency in a population of organisms from one generation to the next, regardless of whether that change is the result of random sampling of the genome (genetic drift) or the result of selection. “Artificial” selection occurs when the experimenter determines which individuals in a population reproduce based on a specific trait or combination of traits; “natural” selection occurs when the environment determines which individuals in a population reproduce, regardless of the traits involved. Both forms of selection can take place in either the laboratory or in nature.
Dobzhansky and Spassky [
2] imposed directional selection for positive or negative geotaxis (artificial selection) in laboratory populations of
Drosophila. Following 20 generations of response to this artificial selection, selection was terminated and flies were maintained in the laboratory without selection (relaxed selection) for 20 additional generations, during which time natural selection resulted in flies reverting towards, but not converging with the original geotactic “score” 40 generations previously. They did not explore response to renewed directional selection after relaxed selection.
Application of an organophosphate to control
Culex pipiens imposes artificial selection on resistance alleles at an
esterase locus. Mosquitoes with the resistant alleles realize lower fitness in the absence of the organophosphate. A geographic gradient of decreasing pesticide application results in a corresponding cline of decreasing resistance alleles at the
esterase locus [
3,
4]. In this case, pesticide imposes tradeoffs between artificial selection and ongoing natural selection and, as artificial selection is relaxed, natural selection rises in importance, analogously to Dobzhansky and Spassky’s
Drosophila [
2].
Regardless of the agent of selection, response to that selection depends upon genetic variation underlying the trait or traits being selected [
5] (Equation (11.2)): R = h
2S, where response to selection (R) is the product of the heritability (h
2), a measure of the “extent to which phenotypes are determined by genes transmitted from parents” [
5] (p. 113), times the strength of selection (S) applied to that trait.
In accord with John Donne, (Donne J (1623) Meditation XVII. “No man is an island, entire of itself; every man is a piece of the continent, a part of the main.): No
gene is an island, entire of itself; every
gene is a piece of the
genome, a part of the
whole organism. After selection on geotaxis, Dobzhansky & Spassky [
2] (p. 78) also cited “slight but significant” correlated responses in body size, eye size, testis color, and wing venation, which also “showed some indications of instability [reversion] after relaxation of selection.” Such correlated responses to selection result from an underlying genetic correlation between the two traits. Hence, if directional selection is applied to a trait, whether in the laboratory or in nature, and there is a correlated response to that selection, then there must be (1) heritable variation in the first trait, (2) heritable variation in the correlated trait, and (3) a genetic correlation between the two traits (
Box 1).
Box 1. Correlated response to selection [
5] (Equation (19.6)).
CRY = correlated response in trait Y to direct selection on trait X
SX = selection differential imposed on trait X
δPX = phenotypic standard deviation in trait X
δYX = phenotypic standard deviation in trait Y
hX = (heritability of trait X)1/2
hY = (heritability of trait Y)1/2
rA = additive genetic correlation between trait X and trait Y
For there to be a correlated response, each of the elements in the parentheses must be non-zero.
In effect, response to strong directional selection on a trait becomes the major determinant of fitness, overriding other, but relatively minor correlated determinants of fitness, imposing a “cost of correlation” with respect to fitness [
6,
7]. When selection is relaxed, then natural selection again comes into play, overriding previous artificial selection, and the selected trait reverts towards pre-selection values.
Herein, we confirm and elaborate on this general pattern in the pitcher-plant mosquito,
Wyeomyia smithii (Coq.) (Diptera: Culicidae). We show that positive selection for blood feeding in a polymorphic population, results in a four-fold increase in propensity to take a blood meal (bite); but, after relaxation of selection, biting rapidly reverts to the unselected (control) phenotype. We interpret this reversion as a cost of blood feeding, as previously shown by an anticipatory commitment to up-regulation of multiple metabolic pathways in biters compared to more opportunistic non-biters [
8]. Importantly, in
Wy. smithii, despite convergence of phenotypes, the formerly selected line harbors greater genetic variation for biting than the unselected control and responds rapidly to renewed selection. History matters.
The pitcher-plant mosquito,
Wy. smithii deposits its eggs and completes its entire pre-adult development only within the water-filled leaves of the carnivorous purple pitcher plant,
Sarracenia purpurea L [
9,
10]. The range of the mosquito follows that of its host plant from the Gulf of Mexico to northern Canada [
11,
12]. Northern and mountain populations are obligate non-biting and mature multiple egg batches without a blood meal; southern populations produce the first batch of eggs without a blood meal (autogenous) but require a blood meal (bite) for the second and subsequent ovarian cycles (anautogenous) [
13,
14]. However, not all southern females bite, with propensity to bite being moderate along the Gulf Coast and low in the Carolina Coastal Plain (
Figure 1). The current study is based on over 14,000 larvae collected near Wilma, FL (30° N, 85° W, 10 m elevation).
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Standard Rearing Protocol
Selected and control populations were maintained as diapausing larvae on an 8:16 light:dark cycle at 21 °C. To continue generations, populations were transferred to and reared on programmed conditions of a typical summer day at 40° N: an 18:6 light:dark cycle using a sinewave thermoperiod with a maximum temperature of 32 °C and minimum temperature of 15 °C that lagged the light cycle by 3 h. Relative humidity was programmed for a constant 80%. Details and sources in
Text S1.
2.2. Histogram Method
When hatch of a given generation in the selected or control line exceeded a desired population size, the line was thinned or experimental animals removed using a “histogram” method to minimize unintentional selection on development time in any line. Neither any larvae removed for thinning or for experiments or their progeny were ever returned to the selected or to the control line. Details in
Text S1.
2.3. Blood Feeding
To determine incidence of biting, ≥390 individuals were reared to adults as above. The number of eclosing females was scored by sexing pupal exuviae (pupal cuticle). Starting at first female eclosion, adults were offered a rat anesthetized with a ketamine/xylazine cocktail for 15 min three times per week between 1200–1400 subjective time (25–30 °C). Any female that engorged blood was scored and recorded as a biter, removed from the cage, and discarded. When females aggregated on the host, any female with a bent labium was scored and recorded as a biting individual and was removed from the cage. The incidence of biting was then calculated by dividing the number of biting females by the total number of females having emerged as adults.
2.4. Selection Generations 1–19, Rat Host
Selection for biting began using ~14,000 individuals collected from a low-biting population in northern Florida (population WI in other papers from this lab: 31° N, 85° W, 10 m elevation). The environment and protocols used for selection were as above except biters were allowed to feed to completion, removed from their cage, and placed into a separate “biting” cage with supplemental males from the same generation of the selected line. All hatch from the biting cage were used to generate the subsequent generations (
Figure 2). Initially, hatch from biting females were not sufficient to maintain a line able to replace itself exclusively from biting individuals. In this situation, we used the abundant hatch generated by females from the same generation, but before they bit (pre-biters), in order to augment the selected line. The selected line was maintained at 10,000 individuals. Note that for each generation of selection after the first, both the pre-biters and the biters were offspring of biters from the previous generation. This protocol was followed until the selected line could sustain itself (R
0 > 1.0) exclusively from biting females in the 7th generation of selection. Thereafter, the selected line was maintained at 5000 individuals; hatch in excess of 5000 were used in experiments. Through all generations of selection, including those that were not offered a host, hatch were placed on short days (L:D = 10:14) at 21 °C to synchronize each generation and to mitigate inadvertent direct selection on development time, generation time, or the timing of reproductive allocation. After adults of a given generation had died, their offspring were transferred to long days and reared to adulthood, as above.
2.5. Response to Selection
Response to selection was determined in the initial P1 = generation 0, and generations 7, 9, 11, and 14, using a rat for a host as described above. Selection was maintained through the 19th generation. After generation 19, both the selected and control lines were maintained separately at 5000 hatch per generation from pre-biters without access to a host. Each generation continued to be synchronized in diapause and individuals in excess of 5000 thinned by the histogram method.
2.6. Artificial Host
To meet the university requirement to obviate the need for live animals for invasive experimental research, we developed an artificial “host” comprised of a heated CO
2-infused dental pledget soaked in defibrinated sheep blood (
Figure 3). To test the efficacy of the artificial host, we compared biting propensity in the biting and the control lines of
Wy. smithii in the 27th generation with biting by the local tree-hole mosquito,
Aedes sierrensis, known to be an avid biter in the field and in the lab on a vertebrate host.
Preliminary observations indicated that the blood-soaked pledgets were as successful as a live rat at stimulating blood feeding when cup water temperature was set at 51 °C for Ae. sierrensis and 45 °C for Wy. smithii. Consequently, cup temperatures of 45 °C were used for all subsequent experiments with Wy. smithii.
2.7. Secondary Selection
Starting in the 30th generation, the previously selected line and the un-selected control line were exposed to the artificial host as in selection from generations 0–19. Blood-fed females were removed to a separate cage and continued to be offered a host until all biting females had died. Incidence of biting was determined each generation for four generations (30–33). In each generation, the hatch from all blood-fed females were synchronized in diapause and used to found subsequent generations of selection without the addition of any pre-biters.
2.8. Statistical Methods
Regression, correlation, and ANOVA used the Excel Analysis Toolpak in Microsoft Office 10. ANCOVA used
JMP Start Statistics [
15]. Standard errors of percentages were estimated as: SE = sqrt(pq/n), where
n = number of eclosing females,
p = % biting, q = % not biting, subject to
xn > 6.0, where
x is the lesser
frequency [
16] (pp. 134–135).
3. Results
Before selection, 18% of emerging females bit (
Figure 4). With increasing selection, biting increased to 84% in generation 14 of the selected line. Hatch per biting female on the rat host (
Figure 2) averaged 9.6 ± 0.71SE and was not significantly correlated with generation of selection (r
2 = 0.11,
n = 9,
p = 0.80). Hence, any increase in productivity in the selected line was due more to an increasing incidence of biting than to the resulting fecundity of biters.
Selection continued through the 19th generation. Selection was relaxed after the 19th generation and both the selected and control lines maintained without access to a host until the 27th generation. After relaxed selection, biting in the selected line had converged with the unselected control line in the 27th generation; this result was confirmed in the 30th generation. By comparison with ~20% biting by
Wy. smithii in the 27th generation, biting by
Ae. sierrensis exceeded 80% (
Figure 4), demonstrating the efficacy of the blood-soaked pledgets in stimulating biting in an avid biting mosquito.
When selection for biting was imposed both on the previously selected line and on the unselected control line, only the previously selected line responded to that selection (
Figure 5).